back to indexJournal Club with Dr. Peter Attia | Metformin for Longevity & The Power of Belief Effects
Chapters
0:0 Dr. Peter Attia, Journal Club
3:27 Sponsors: Helix Sleep & Levels
6:11 Dreams
12:36 Article #1, Metformin, Mitochondria, Blood Glucose
19:47 Type 2 Diabetes & Causes, Insulin Resistance
25:30 Type 2 Diabetes Medications, Metformin, Geroprotection, Bannister Study
36:19 Sponsor: AG1
37:15 TAME Trial; Demographics, Twin Cohort
44:27 Metformin & Mortality Rate
51:28 Kaplan-Meier Mortality Curve, Error Bars & Significance, Statistical Power
61:17 Sponsor: InsideTracker
62:23 Hazard Ratios, Censoring
69:0 Metformin Advantage?, Variables, Interventions Testing Program
76:2 Berberine, Acarbose, SGLT2 Inhibitors
83:48 Blood Glucose & Energy Balance; Caloric Restriction, Aging Biomarkers
92:22 Tool: Reading Journal Articles, 4 Questions, Supplemental Information
98:10 Article #2, Belief Effects vs. Placebo Effect
105:22 Nicotine Effects
111:7 Nicotine Doses & Belief Effects, fMRI Scan
120:7 Biological Effects, Dose-Dependent Response & Belief Effects
125:14 Biology & Beliefs, Significance, Dopamine Response, Non-Smokers
130:57 Dose-Dependence & Beliefs, Side Effects, Nocebo Effect
139:6 Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Momentous, Neural Network Newsletter, Social Media
00:00:02.280 |
where we discuss science and science-based tools 00:00:10.040 |
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology 00:00:20.440 |
For any of you that are not familiar with Dr. Peter Attia, 00:00:24.840 |
who is an expert in all aspects of health and lifespan. 00:00:28.760 |
He is the author of a bestselling book entitled "Outlive," 00:00:35.440 |
And he is the host of the very popular podcast, "The Drive," 00:00:41.160 |
in all domains of medicine and scientists as well. 00:00:51.600 |
a Journal Club is a common practice in graduate school 00:00:55.520 |
whereby students get together to discuss one or two papers 00:00:59.960 |
and to really compare their own conclusions of those papers 00:01:07.880 |
to do a Journal Club together for a very long time, 00:01:19.280 |
By the way, it could just have easily been called 00:01:25.120 |
First, Peter is going to discuss a paper on metformin, 00:01:29.400 |
which is a drug that many people are interested in 00:01:45.180 |
and whether or not other people might be well-advised 00:01:50.080 |
based on the data in that paper and at this time. 00:01:53.600 |
Then I present a paper which is about the placebo effect. 00:02:03.320 |
that the placebo effect can actually follow a dose response. 00:02:25.320 |
in cognitive enhancement by way of pharmacology, 00:02:28.320 |
or frankly, for people who are simply interested 00:02:33.080 |
I think you'll find that discussion to be very interesting. 00:02:41.000 |
one in the realm of longevity as it relates to metformin 00:02:49.000 |
but you will also learn how a journal club is conducted. 00:02:52.160 |
I think you'll see in observing how we parse these papers 00:02:55.040 |
and discuss them, even arguing in them at times, 00:02:59.940 |
is they take a look at the existing peer-reviewed research 00:03:07.520 |
does this paper really show what it claims to show or not? 00:03:21.440 |
some of which you may want to apply or avoid, 00:03:24.880 |
about how science and medicine is carried out. 00:03:27.580 |
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast 00:03:30.360 |
is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. 00:03:34.840 |
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science 00:03:37.400 |
and science-related tools to the general public. 00:03:41.000 |
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. 00:03:50.880 |
Now, sleep is the foundation of mental health, 00:03:56.600 |
mental health, physical health, and performance 00:04:00.160 |
One of the key things to getting a great night's sleep 00:04:13.800 |
Do you tend to run hot or cold in the middle of the night? 00:04:16.280 |
Maybe you don't know the answers to those questions 00:04:26.800 |
And when I started sleeping on a Dusk mattress 00:04:28.780 |
about two years ago, my sleep immediately improved. 00:04:31.480 |
So if you're interested in upgrading your mattress, 00:04:37.000 |
and they'll match you to a customized mattress for you. 00:04:39.280 |
And you'll get up to $350 off any mattress order 00:04:43.880 |
Again, if interested, go to helixsleep.com/huberman 00:04:50.260 |
Today's episode is also brought to us by Levels. 00:04:54.800 |
how different foods and behaviors affect your health 00:05:02.400 |
impacting your immediate and long-term health 00:05:04.860 |
is the way that your body manages its blood glucose, 00:05:07.540 |
or sometimes referred to as blood sugar levels. 00:05:10.200 |
To maintain energy and focus throughout the day, 00:05:16.540 |
Using Levels, you can monitor how different types of foods 00:05:21.160 |
as well as food timing and things like exercise, 00:05:26.360 |
I started using Levels a little over a year ago, 00:05:30.160 |
into how specific foods were spiking my blood sugar 00:05:35.960 |
as well as how the spacing of exercise and my meals 00:05:41.200 |
And in doing so, it really allowed me to optimize 00:05:43.500 |
how I eat, what I eat, when I exercise, and so on, 00:05:47.480 |
such that my blood glucose levels and energy levels 00:05:51.720 |
If you're interested in learning more about Levels 00:05:53.560 |
and trying a continuous glucose monitor yourself, 00:06:20.760 |
And it's basically something that we do all the time, 00:06:29.540 |
And oftentimes that will lead to a text dialogue 00:06:34.620 |
But this time we've opted to try talking about these papers 00:06:38.940 |
that we find particularly exciting in real time 00:06:46.220 |
First of all, so that people can get some sense 00:06:49.820 |
We do feel that people should know about these findings. 00:06:53.700 |
And second of all, that it's an opportunity for people 00:06:58.980 |
and think about the papers they hear about in the news, 00:07:04.940 |
Also just to start thinking like scientists and clinicians 00:07:10.440 |
to pick through a paper, the good, the bad, and the ugly. 00:07:14.620 |
So we're flying a little blind here, which is fun. 00:07:17.880 |
I'm definitely excited for all the above reasons. 00:07:25.060 |
You and I have been talking about this for some time. 00:07:40.260 |
just because we've been a little stretched then. 00:08:03.700 |
And I think the two papers we've chosen today 00:08:06.660 |
illustrate two opposite ends of the spectrum. 00:08:14.620 |
And the paper ultimately I've chosen to present, 00:08:17.240 |
although I apologize, I'm surprising you with this, 00:08:28.400 |
on a much more nuanced paper about ATP binding cassettes 00:08:35.680 |
might be more interesting to a broader audience. 00:08:45.520 |
with making this certain drink that was like your elixir. 00:08:49.440 |
And it had all of these crazy ingredients in it. 00:08:55.440 |
But the one thing I remembered when I woke up, 00:08:58.400 |
I was really trying so hard to remember them. 00:09:03.760 |
Like you had to collect a certain amount of dew 00:09:06.160 |
off the leaves every morning to put into this drink. 00:09:23.080 |
with a special pocket that you could put the thermos into 00:09:26.440 |
so that you were never without the special Andrew drink. 00:09:30.440 |
And again, you know how dreams, when you're having them, 00:09:38.760 |
Like, why would he want the thermos in his shirt? 00:09:45.720 |
And there were lots of things in it, including dew. 00:10:05.360 |
My dad's Argentine, so that's where I picked it up. 00:10:07.800 |
I started drinking it when I was like five years old 00:10:09.600 |
or younger, which I don't recommend people do. 00:10:12.140 |
Don't drink the smoked versions either, folks. 00:10:17.960 |
of carrying around the thermos close to the body, 00:10:23.800 |
or if you ever spot grown men in a restaurant 00:10:26.720 |
anywhere in the world, carrying a thermos with them 00:10:42.360 |
I don't carry the thermos, but I do drink mate every day. 00:10:45.040 |
And I'm going to start collecting dew off the leaves. 00:10:55.720 |
Recently, I've been doing some dream exploration. 00:10:58.920 |
I've had some absolutely transformative dreams 00:11:02.760 |
One dream in particular that allowed me to feel something 00:11:06.400 |
I've never felt before and has catalyzed a large number 00:11:10.780 |
of important decisions in a way that no other experience, 00:11:19.680 |
- And do you think you could have had that dream, 00:11:21.720 |
we don't have to get into it if you don't want 00:11:22.900 |
to talk about it now, but was there a lot of work 00:11:25.620 |
you had to do to prepare for that dream to have taken place? 00:11:31.000 |
At least 18 months of intensive analysis type work 00:11:56.680 |
I just decided it's going to keep working on it. 00:11:58.480 |
And then two nights later, I traveled to a meeting 00:12:00.860 |
in Aspen and I had the most profound dream ever 00:12:04.240 |
where I was able to sense something and feel something 00:12:06.500 |
I've always wanted to feel as so real within the dream. 00:12:14.000 |
this is what people close to me that I respect 00:12:16.680 |
have been talking about, but I was able to feel it 00:12:19.200 |
and therefore I can actually access this in my waking life. 00:12:44.600 |
Reassessing the Evidence of a Survival Advantage 00:12:55.260 |
This is by Matthew Thomas Keyes and colleagues. 00:13:08.720 |
Bannister published a paper that I think in many ways 00:13:13.940 |
kind of got the world very excited about metformin. 00:13:18.880 |
And I'm sure many people have heard about this paper, 00:13:24.620 |
And in many ways, it's the paper that has led 00:13:32.240 |
And I should probably just define for the audience 00:13:46.220 |
for many years, depends where it was first approved, 00:13:52.440 |
But call it directionally, 50 plus years of use 00:13:56.200 |
as a first line agent for patients with type 2 diabetes. 00:14:03.560 |
So this is a drug that's been around forever, 00:14:13.000 |
The mechanism by which metformin works is debated hotly. 00:14:23.540 |
which is it inhibits complex one of the mitochondria. 00:14:29.320 |
So the mitochondria, as everybody thinks of those 00:14:47.800 |
We put that into an electron transport chain. 00:14:51.140 |
And we basically trade chemical energy for electrons 00:14:56.140 |
that can then be used to make phosphates onto ADP. 00:15:00.580 |
So it's, you know, you think of everything you do. 00:15:02.580 |
Eating is taking the chemical energy in food, 00:15:07.000 |
making electrical energy in the mitochondria. 00:15:09.580 |
Those electrons pump a gradient that allow you to make ATP. 00:15:13.420 |
To give a sense of how primal and important this is, 00:15:16.700 |
if you block that process completely, you die. 00:15:20.120 |
So everybody's probably heard of cyanide, right? 00:15:21.880 |
Cyanide is something that is incredibly toxic, 00:15:26.020 |
Cyanide is a complete blocker of this process. 00:15:30.880 |
I think it blocks complex four of the mitochondria. 00:15:33.320 |
I don't know if you recall if it complex three 00:15:36.240 |
I know a lot about toxins that impact the nervous system, 00:15:44.480 |
that animals make and insects make and how they kill you. 00:15:47.600 |
- Yeah, like detrototoxin and all these things 00:15:49.960 |
that block sodium channels, yeah, yeah, yeah. 00:15:53.680 |
'cause it allows me to talk about neuroscience, 00:16:06.800 |
electron transport chain will kill you instantly. 00:16:08.720 |
People understand that, of course, a drop of cyanide 00:16:10.760 |
and you would be dead literally instantaneously. 00:16:14.000 |
So Metformin works at the first of those complexes. 00:16:17.180 |
I believe there are four, if my memory serves correctly, 00:16:22.400 |
But of course it's not a complete inhibition of it. 00:16:30.500 |
So the net effect of that is that it changes the ratio 00:16:32.960 |
of adenosine monophosphate to adenosine diphosphate. 00:16:42.720 |
Because what it unambiguously does is reduces 00:16:45.640 |
the amount of glucose that the liver puts out. 00:16:48.060 |
So hepatic glucose output is one of the fundamental problems 00:16:58.000 |
you and I sitting here with normal blood sugar 00:17:00.400 |
have about five grams of glucose in our total circulation. 00:17:05.080 |
Think about how quickly the brain will go through that 00:17:12.240 |
is our liver's ability to titrate out glucose. 00:17:18.120 |
if the glucose level was consistently two teaspoons, 00:17:24.040 |
So the difference between being metabolically healthy 00:17:26.880 |
and having profound type two diabetes is one teaspoon 00:17:40.880 |
Is it fair to provide this overly simplified summary 00:17:45.840 |
of the biochemistry, which is that when we eat, 00:17:49.140 |
the food is broken down, but the breaking of bonds 00:17:56.200 |
And the mitochondria are central to that process. 00:17:58.440 |
And that metformin is partially short-circuiting 00:18:06.780 |
when we have metformin in our system, presumably, 00:18:15.920 |
but less of that is turned into blood sugar, glucose. 00:18:32.480 |
That's probably a better way to think about it 00:18:47.560 |
Elastro, folks, for those of you who don't remember, 00:18:50.560 |
by the way, if you ever ate this stuff, you'd remember, 00:18:53.320 |
because it was a fat that was not easily digested. 00:19:01.460 |
So it was being put into potato chips and whatnot. 00:19:03.520 |
And the idea was that people would simply excrete it. 00:19:12.120 |
except that people got a lot of stomach aches. 00:19:14.400 |
- Well, the anal seepage. - Everyone got fatter 00:19:17.660 |
- The anal seepage is what really did that product in. 00:19:23.640 |
Peter's a clinician, a physician, an MD, and I'm not, 00:19:27.200 |
could find it an appropriate term to describe 00:19:53.780 |
So again, what's happening when you have type two diabetes, 00:19:56.600 |
the primary insult probably occurs in the muscles 00:20:08.840 |
So let's just talk about it through the lens of the muscle 00:20:10.680 |
'cause the muscle is responsible for most glucose disposal. 00:20:18.320 |
And we want to put most of it into our muscles. 00:20:31.020 |
But a chemical reaction takes place inside the cell 00:20:38.520 |
And a transporter, just think of like a little tunnel, 00:20:42.360 |
like a little straw goes up through the level of the cell. 00:20:51.840 |
Things that move against gradients need pumps to move them. 00:20:57.220 |
Glucose is moving with its gradient into the cell. 00:21:09.160 |
I mean, I certainly know active and passive transport 00:21:12.020 |
as it relates to like neurotransmitter and ion flow. 00:21:15.320 |
But I'd never heard that when insulin binds to a cell 00:21:21.140 |
- Yeah, the glucose doesn't need a pump to move it in. 00:21:33.780 |
So what happens is as, and Gerald Shulman at Yale 00:21:45.340 |
and by intramuscular, I mean intracellular fat, 00:21:49.040 |
triacyl and diacylglycerides accumulate in a muscle cell, 00:21:55.360 |
And all of a sudden, I'm making these numbers up, 00:22:01.160 |
to trigger the little transporter now, you need three, 00:22:04.480 |
and then you need four, and then you need five. 00:22:07.020 |
You need more and more insulin to get the thing up. 00:22:10.300 |
That is the definition of insulin resistance. 00:22:13.840 |
The cell is becoming resistant to the effect of insulin. 00:22:17.000 |
And therefore, the early mark of insulin resistance, 00:22:20.560 |
the canary in the coal mine is not an increase in glucose. 00:22:41.300 |
someone with type 2 diabetes has long passed that system. 00:22:54.920 |
And now they're not getting glucose into the cell. 00:22:59.920 |
And even though it's continually being chewed up 00:23:05.180 |
would account for most of that glucose disposal, 00:23:09.160 |
the liver is now becoming insulin resistant as well. 00:23:19.300 |
being pumped into the circulation by the liver, 00:23:21.300 |
and you have the muscles that can't dispose of it. 00:23:25.980 |
because the same problem of fat accumulating in the muscle 00:23:31.260 |
And now the relatively few cells in the pancreas 00:23:36.700 |
are undergoing inflammation due to the fat accumulation 00:23:41.620 |
And so now the thing that you need to make more insulin 00:23:50.900 |
might actually even require insulin exogenously. 00:23:53.740 |
- Could you share with us a few of the causes 00:23:58.980 |
I mean, one it sounds like is accumulating too much fat. 00:24:01.820 |
- Yeah, so energy imbalance would be an enormous one. 00:24:09.500 |
So when Gerald Schulman was running clinical trials at Yale, 00:24:14.500 |
they would be recruiting undergrads to study, obviously, 00:24:17.500 |
'cause you're typically recruiting young people. 00:24:26.580 |
and actually look at what's happening in the muscle. 00:24:39.460 |
You couldn't have active people in these studies. 00:24:42.180 |
So exercising is one of the most important things 00:24:45.100 |
you're going to do to ward off insulin resistance. 00:24:53.820 |
I think we probably talked about this previously, 00:24:55.440 |
but if you, you know, some very elegant mechanistic studies 00:24:59.860 |
you let them only sleep for four hours for a week, 00:25:02.500 |
you'll reduce their glucose disposal by about half. 00:25:06.500 |
- Which is, I mean, that's a staggering amount of, 00:25:08.720 |
you're basically inducing profound insulin resistance 00:25:16.680 |
So where, when you're accumulating excess energy, 00:25:25.040 |
into the muscle, into the liver, into the pancreas, 00:25:35.380 |
so every drug you give a person with type 2 diabetes 00:25:40.200 |
So some of the drugs tell you to make more insulin. 00:25:57.420 |
It tamps down glucose by addressing the glucose, 00:26:07.260 |
initially causing you to also make more insulin. 00:26:13.540 |
that berberine is more or less the poor man's metformin? 00:26:19.500 |
It just happens to have the same properties of- 00:26:37.780 |
we can talk about berberine a little bit later. 00:26:39.180 |
I had a couple great experiences with berberine 00:26:48.340 |
In 2011, I became very interested in metformin personally, 00:26:56.460 |
and just somehow decided like, I should be taking this. 00:27:02.620 |
I started it in May of 2011, and I realized that 00:27:05.600 |
because I was on a trip with a bunch of buddies. 00:27:08.260 |
We went to the Berkshire Hathaway shareholder meeting, 00:27:11.860 |
which is, you know, the Buffett shareholder meeting. 00:27:15.340 |
And, you know, it was kind of like a fun thing to do. 00:27:20.480 |
because I didn't titrate up the dose of metformin. 00:27:28.740 |
- Is that characteristic of your approach to things? 00:27:33.520 |
- Next time, I'll give you a thermos of this dew 00:27:39.740 |
So I remember being so sick that the whole time 00:27:42.340 |
we were in Nebraska or Omaha, I guess, I couldn't, 00:27:45.940 |
we went to Dairy Queen 'cause you do all the Buffett things 00:27:49.220 |
Like I couldn't have an ice cream at Dairy Queen. 00:27:53.020 |
- Oh, 'cause I would say if you've got metformin 00:27:56.020 |
You could have four ice cream cones and probably- 00:28:00.860 |
So clearly metformin has this side effect initially, 00:28:03.700 |
which is a little bit of appetite suppression. 00:28:05.700 |
But regardless, that's the story on metformin. 00:28:08.100 |
There were a lot of reasons I was interested in it. 00:28:13.280 |
That term wasn't in my vernacular at the time. 00:28:17.380 |
this is gonna help you buffer glucose better. 00:28:19.700 |
And this was sort of my first foray into, you know, 00:28:28.460 |
- Yeah, so yeah, zero from geriatric old protection. 00:28:34.460 |
And when we talk about a drug like metformin or rapamycin 00:28:42.700 |
the idea is we're talking about them as zero protective 00:28:47.340 |
that are not targeting a specific disease of aging. 00:28:50.540 |
For example, a PCSK9 inhibitor is sort of zero protective, 00:28:57.740 |
which is cardiovascular disease and dyslipidemia. 00:29:08.760 |
I've never been able to get all nine straight, 00:29:11.300 |
but people know what we're talking about, right? 00:29:12.920 |
So decreased autophagy, increased senescence, 00:29:15.900 |
decreased nutrient sensing or defective nutrient sensing, 00:29:19.400 |
proteomic instability, genomic instability, methylation, 00:29:29.780 |
those deep down biological hallmarks of aging. 00:29:38.560 |
that basically got the world focused on this problem. 00:29:41.240 |
By the world, I mean the world of anti-aging. 00:29:50.660 |
and they got a set of patients who were on metformin 00:29:59.100 |
So these were people who had just progressed to diabetes. 00:30:02.440 |
They were not put on any other drug, just metformin. 00:30:18.920 |
So you're trying to account for all the biases 00:30:21.740 |
that could exist by saying, we're gonna take people 00:30:24.720 |
who look just like that person with diabetes. 00:30:28.080 |
So can we match them for age, sex, socioeconomic status, 00:30:37.420 |
And then let's look at what happened to them over time. 00:30:40.900 |
Now again, this is all happening in the future. 00:30:46.380 |
And so let me just kind of pull up the sort of table here 00:30:51.540 |
And this is not in the paper we talked about, 00:31:05.340 |
But I didn't notice this until about five years ago 00:31:10.020 |
And they did something called informative censoring. 00:31:14.980 |
So the way the study worked is if you were put on metformin, 00:31:19.440 |
If you're not on metformin, we're gonna follow you. 00:31:27.260 |
And it's really the gold standard in a trial of this nature 00:31:30.940 |
or a study of this nature or even a clinical trial. 00:31:32.840 |
You wanna know how much are people dying from anything 00:31:35.700 |
'cause we're trying to prevent or delay death of all causes. 00:31:44.500 |
who's on metformin deviates from that inclusion criteria, 00:31:49.140 |
we will not count them in the final assessment. 00:31:54.280 |
Well, one, the person can be lost to follow up. 00:31:57.720 |
Two, they can just stop taking their metformin. 00:32:08.480 |
So all of those patients were excluded from the study. 00:32:18.240 |
Because what you're basically doing is saying 00:32:31.740 |
So an analogy here would be imagine we're gonna do a study 00:32:35.300 |
of two groups that we think are almost identical. 00:32:44.740 |
But every time somebody dies in the smoking group, 00:32:59.060 |
the Bannister study found a very interesting result, 00:33:10.740 |
this is also one of the challenges of epidemiology 00:33:19.920 |
So everything is normalized to 1,000 person years. 00:33:24.000 |
So the crude death rate in the group of people 00:33:33.760 |
So 14.4 deaths occurred per 1,000 patient years. 00:33:38.760 |
If you look at the control group, it was 15.2. 00:33:48.800 |
and being like, holy crap, this is really amazing. 00:33:58.200 |
It's a difference of about a year and a half. 00:33:59.620 |
Now, of course, a difference of about a year and a half 00:34:07.180 |
So taking a step back, type 2 diabetes on average 00:34:14.240 |
between having type 2 diabetes and not all comers. 00:34:17.260 |
But you're right, this is not a huge difference. 00:34:21.000 |
than one year of life per 1,000 patient years studied. 00:34:24.540 |
- Okay, and by the way, up here, just point out, 00:34:26.620 |
my math was wrong when I said about a year and a half. 00:34:39.720 |
And the fact that it was statistically significant 00:34:51.320 |
The people who took metformin and had diabetes 00:35:05.600 |
compared to the non-group. - Well, that seems to be 00:35:09.360 |
- Because, could you repeat those numbers again? 00:35:13.640 |
- Yeah, so 15% reduction in all-cause mortality 00:35:21.000 |
- It is, and again, there's no clear explanation for it 00:35:27.720 |
unless you believe that metformin is doing something 00:35:47.200 |
and I put myself in this category at one point, 00:35:49.560 |
I would put myself today in the category of undecided, 00:36:06.080 |
Metformin potentially tamps down on senescent cells 00:36:11.320 |
There are lots of things metformin could be doing 00:36:19.440 |
- As many of you know, I've been taking AG1 daily since 2012, 00:36:23.040 |
so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. 00:36:32.480 |
of vitamins and minerals through whole food sources 00:36:34.780 |
that include vegetables and fruits every day, 00:36:37.040 |
but oftentimes I simply can't get enough servings. 00:36:39.720 |
But with AG1, I'm sure to get enough vitamins and minerals 00:36:44.440 |
and it also contains adaptogens to help buffer stress. 00:36:47.600 |
Simply put, I always feel better when I take AG1. 00:36:50.400 |
I have more focus and energy and I sleep better, 00:36:56.960 |
"If you could take just one supplement, what would it be?" 00:37:03.120 |
go to drinkag1.com/huberman to claim a special offer. 00:37:19.260 |
and I think most people took the Bannister study 00:37:21.640 |
as kind of the best evidence we have for the benefits 00:37:25.640 |
of metformin, and I'm sure you've had lots of people 00:37:28.160 |
come up to you and ask you, "Should I be on metformin? 00:37:33.520 |
almost as much as I'm asked any question, outside of dew. 00:37:43.540 |
- So okay, so let's kind of fast forward to now the paper 00:37:47.200 |
that I wanted to spend a few more minutes on. 00:37:49.880 |
I'm still dazzled by the insertion of the straw 00:37:57.560 |
I don't think I've ever heard that described. 00:38:06.760 |
- Just to give people a sense of why I'm so dazzled by it, 00:38:09.960 |
I am always fascinated by how quickly, how efficiently, 00:38:24.840 |
I mean, you're talking about an on-demand creation 00:38:28.480 |
I mean, these are cells engineering their own machinery 00:38:30.980 |
in real time in response to chemical signals. 00:38:34.960 |
- Yeah, but I'm sort of rusty on my neuroscience, 00:38:37.280 |
but an action potential works in reverse the same way. 00:38:40.240 |
Like you need the ATP gradient to restore the gradient. 00:38:51.740 |
the way that neurons become electrically active 00:38:53.940 |
is by the flow of ions from the outside of the cell 00:39:00.580 |
meaning they're triggered by electrical changes 00:39:02.260 |
in the gradients, by changes in electrical potential. 00:39:09.220 |
until there's a balance equal inside and outside the cell. 00:39:12.280 |
I think what's different is that there's some movement 00:39:18.520 |
when neurotransmitters like dopamine binds to its receptor 00:39:20.800 |
and then a bunch of, it's like a bucket brigade 00:39:24.740 |
But it's not often that you hear about receptors 00:39:34.980 |
And then those are long, slow things that take place 00:39:38.620 |
What you're talking about is a real on-demand insertion 00:39:43.180 |
- And it makes sense as to why that would be required, 00:39:50.500 |
"We would like to redo the entire banister analysis." 00:39:58.660 |
the interest in this topic is through the roof. 00:40:02.140 |
There is a clinical trial called the TAME trial 00:40:12.620 |
is going to try to ask this question prospectively 00:40:17.060 |
- So this is the Targeting Aging with Metformin trial? 00:40:20.820 |
Near Barzilai is probably the senior PI on that. 00:40:25.820 |
And I think in many ways, the banister study, 00:40:34.060 |
probably provided some of the motivation for the TAME trial. 00:40:37.040 |
So they said, "Okay, look, we're gonna do this. 00:40:38.820 |
We're gonna use a different cohort of people." 00:40:41.580 |
So the first study that we just talked about, 00:40:47.780 |
roughly they sampled 95,000 subjects from a UK biobank. 00:41:06.100 |
of what banister did, which was just a group of people 00:41:09.880 |
with and without diabetic that they tried to match 00:41:13.280 |
But then they did a second analysis in parallel 00:41:25.740 |
because here you have a degree of genetic similarity 00:41:29.460 |
and you have similar environmental factors during childhood 00:41:40.180 |
of a journal club, virtually any clinical paper 00:41:43.420 |
you're gonna read, table one is the characteristics 00:41:51.160 |
So when I look at table one here, you can see it's, 00:41:54.480 |
and by the way, just for people watching this, 00:41:56.400 |
we're gonna make all these papers and figures available. 00:41:58.440 |
So if you're, don't, we'll have nice show notes 00:42:08.220 |
And again, it's almost always gonna be the first table 00:42:11.800 |
Usually the first figure in the paper is a study design. 00:42:21.900 |
And you can see here that there are four columns. 00:42:28.520 |
And then the second two are the twins who are matched. 00:42:31.380 |
And you can see, remember how I said they sampled 00:42:36.500 |
So they got, you know, 7,842 singletons on metformin, 00:42:44.220 |
On the twins, they got 976 on metformin with diabetes. 00:42:48.860 |
And then by definition, 976 co-twins without them. 00:42:58.860 |
What was the year of indexing when we got them? 00:43:08.460 |
that really cannot be matched in a study like this, 00:43:11.540 |
so this is a very important limitation, is the medication. 00:43:16.780 |
Notice how pretty much everything else is perfectly matched 00:43:27.700 |
In other words, just to give you a couple of examples, 00:43:29.420 |
right, on the, and let's just talk about the singletons, 00:43:32.160 |
'cause it's basically the same story on the twins. 00:43:36.460 |
with type 2 diabetes are on lipid-lowering medication, 00:43:39.860 |
it's 45.6% versus 15.4% in the matched without diabetes. 00:44:00.620 |
So this is, again, a fundamental flaw of epidemiology. 00:44:08.820 |
- This is why I became an experimental scientist, 00:44:13.060 |
- That's right, because without random assignment, 00:44:17.100 |
Now you'll see in a moment when we get into the analysis, 00:44:23.200 |
but they can never correct this medication one. 00:44:30.740 |
in this experiment, or in this survey, or study, 00:44:36.500 |
the twin trick, which I think is pretty cool. 00:44:47.340 |
So one of the other things they wanted to know was, 00:45:10.160 |
So the most important row, I think, in this table 00:45:21.780 |
in the Bannister study, those were on the ballpark 00:45:45.300 |
- Are you talking about pooling the lifespans 00:45:51.540 |
- Yeah, because this-- - Because you're normalizing, 00:45:53.540 |
so it's not who's gonna live a thousand years, 00:45:56.940 |
You're essentially taking, so you've got some people 00:45:59.720 |
that are gonna live 76 years, 52 years, 91 years, 00:46:04.660 |
and you're pooling all of those until you hit a thousand. 00:46:12.060 |
You're basically like, so let's say the control group, 00:46:16.220 |
you're asking if there were a thousand person years 00:46:19.700 |
available to live, how likely is it that this person 00:46:24.700 |
- Yeah, so a couple of ways to think about it. 00:46:26.220 |
So taking a step back, we always have to have 00:46:29.420 |
So when we talk about the mortality from a disease 00:46:32.060 |
like cancer in the population, we report it as 00:46:41.860 |
- Okay, that's a much more intuitive way to express it. 00:46:44.500 |
- It is, but the reason we can do it that way 00:46:47.460 |
is because we're literally looking at how many people died 00:46:51.420 |
this calendar year, and we divide it by the number 00:46:55.820 |
So it's typically what you're doing when you look 00:47:03.160 |
So that's why we can say the highest mortality 00:47:09.580 |
Even though the absolute number of deaths is small, 00:47:12.100 |
it's because there's not that many people there, right? 00:47:14.560 |
The majority of deaths in absolute terms probably occur 00:47:25.100 |
So we just normalize to the number of people. 00:47:26.960 |
Here are all the people that started the year. 00:47:31.140 |
Why are these done in a slightly more complicated way? 00:47:34.200 |
Because we don't follow these people for their whole lives. 00:47:37.620 |
We're only following them for a period of observation, 00:47:42.060 |
So to say something like we have a crude death rate 00:47:49.980 |
one way to think about that is if you had a thousand people 00:47:58.560 |
If you had 500 people and you followed them for two years, 00:48:04.420 |
If you have a thousand people and you follow them 00:48:08.740 |
Those would all be considered equivalent mortalities. 00:48:16.220 |
like I find epidemiology, when you get in the weeds, 00:48:19.180 |
is way more complicated than following the basics 00:48:25.460 |
you get to push all this stuff into the garbage bin 00:48:28.660 |
and just say, hey, we're gonna take this number of people, 00:48:30.900 |
we're gonna exclude this group, we're gonna randomize, 00:48:33.980 |
- Yeah, that's what, like the paper we'll talk about next. 00:48:41.940 |
and they don't show it in this table, it's only in the text, 00:48:44.600 |
when you adjust for age, a very important check to do 00:48:48.800 |
is what is the crude death rate of the people on metformin 00:49:01.320 |
and 21.68 for the twin group that's on metformin. 00:49:05.900 |
When you adjust for age, they're almost identical. 00:49:23.460 |
Those parentheses are offering the 95% confidence interval. 00:49:32.180 |
is the crude death rate of how many people are dying 00:49:39.300 |
that the actual number is between 23.23 and 26.64. 00:49:43.700 |
If a 95% confidence interval does not cross the number zero, 00:49:52.920 |
Okay, so the first thing that just jumps out at you, 00:50:01.000 |
between the people who have diabetes and those who don't. 00:50:13.200 |
24.93 to 16.86, which by the way remains after age adjustment 00:50:18.200 |
when you go to the twin group, it's 24.73 to 12.94. 00:50:24.780 |
what you're describing, if I understand correctly, 00:50:31.760 |
let's just talk about the singletons, the non-twins, 00:50:43.100 |
but there's a bad joke to be made here, but-- 00:50:49.460 |
- Right, 17 deaths per 1000 versus 25 deaths. 00:50:54.460 |
And the 25 is in the folks that took metformin. 00:50:58.340 |
Now, that to the naive listener and to me means, 00:51:09.340 |
but we have to remember that these people have another, 00:51:16.480 |
- Because people weren't assigned drug or not assigned drug. 00:51:22.720 |
for a bad health issue and compare to everyone else. 00:51:40.020 |
So if you look at figure one, it's a Kaplan-Meier curve, 00:51:45.500 |
So you'll see these in any study that is looking at death. 00:51:59.220 |
So on the X-axis is always time and on the Y-axis 00:52:05.060 |
So it's a curve that always goes from zero to one, 00:52:08.980 |
one or 100%, and it's always decreasing monotonically, 00:52:18.660 |
So that's what a cumulative mortality curve looks like. 00:52:22.340 |
- Now we're looking at, you're starting it alive 00:52:39.360 |
You have those that were on metformin with type 2 diabetes 00:52:47.580 |
are the darker lines and the people with type 2 diabetes 00:52:54.060 |
You'll also notice, and I like the way they've done it here, 00:52:58.500 |
And we should mention for those that are just listening 00:53:02.980 |
the downward trending line from the controls, 00:53:07.460 |
so again, non-diabetic, not taking metformin, 00:53:10.220 |
is above the line corresponding to the diabetics 00:53:16.500 |
Put crudely, the people who are taking metformin 00:53:22.620 |
that have diabetes are dying at a faster rate 00:53:27.320 |
The two lines do not overlap except at the beginning 00:53:30.860 |
It's like a foot race where basically the people 00:53:32.420 |
with metformin and diabetes are falling behind 00:53:37.740 |
- That's right, and I'm glad you brought up a good point. 00:53:46.980 |
It's not a requirement that they never cross. 00:53:51.020 |
that they're monotonically decreasing or staying flat. 00:53:59.500 |
and one starts out looking really, really bad, 00:54:05.900 |
and then it actually crosses and goes underneath. 00:54:09.400 |
So to your point, the people with diabetes taking metformin 00:54:14.240 |
in both the matched singletons and the discordants 00:54:17.100 |
are dropping much faster, and they always stay below. 00:54:22.840 |
is just showing you a 95% confidence interval. 00:54:25.320 |
So you're just putting basically error bars along this. 00:54:30.420 |
if you were doing an experiment with a group of mice 00:54:40.060 |
So this is, because you have much more data here, 00:54:43.800 |
- For those that haven't been familiar as to statistics, 00:54:48.800 |
like if you were just gonna measure the heights 00:54:53.600 |
You have the very tall kid and the very shorter kid, 00:54:57.460 |
and you have the short kid and the medium kid. 00:55:01.460 |
and then there'll be standard deviations and standard errors. 00:55:14.580 |
Within a given year, they're gonna be different ages. 00:55:26.860 |
We're not tracking one diabetic taken metformin 00:55:38.220 |
'Cause notice it's running for the full eight years, 00:55:40.400 |
even though they're only following people for, 00:55:46.980 |
So they're using this quite complicated type of mathematics 00:55:54.420 |
And basically, any model has to have some error in it. 00:55:58.180 |
And so they're basically saying this is the error. 00:56:00.420 |
So you could argue when you look at that figure, 00:56:03.420 |
we don't know exactly where the line is in there, 00:56:25.340 |
which is a lot of people, when they look at a paper, 00:56:43.260 |
then that's a real and meaningful difference. 00:56:46.580 |
It depends a lot on the form of the experiment. 00:56:49.880 |
I often see some of the more robust Twitter battles 00:56:59.360 |
hopefully the correct statistics for the sample, 00:57:12.960 |
p less than 0.00001% chance that it's due to chance, right? 00:57:20.680 |
tends to be the kind of gold standard cutoff. 00:57:23.500 |
But when you're talking about data like these, 00:57:32.120 |
you're saying they've modeled it to make predictions 00:57:35.420 |
We're not necessarily looking at raw data points here. 00:57:38.200 |
- Yeah, the raw data was in the previous table. 00:57:40.520 |
That's now taken and run through this Cox model, 00:57:48.720 |
yeah, I think the other thing you always wanna understand 00:57:55.840 |
the only way you can say it's not significant 00:57:58.360 |
is you have to know what it was powered to detect. 00:58:01.060 |
And statistical power is a very important concept 00:58:13.300 |
of what you believe the difference is between the groups. 00:58:16.600 |
And you have to determine the number of samples 00:58:25.300 |
So you use something, it's called a power table, 00:58:30.840 |
So if you're doing treatment A versus treatment B, 00:58:37.880 |
and I think treatment B will have a 65% response. 00:58:41.620 |
You literally go to a power table that says 50% response, 00:58:46.160 |
15% difference, that gives you a place on the grid, 00:58:50.080 |
and I wanna be 90% sure that I'm right, so 90% power. 00:58:55.080 |
so there's gonna be a statistician listening to this 00:58:57.880 |
but this is directionally the way we would describe it. 00:59:00.440 |
And that tells you this is how many animals or people 00:59:08.640 |
And by the way, if you now do the experiment with 147 00:59:14.440 |
you can comfortably say there is no statistical difference 00:59:24.460 |
- Yeah, and very important point that you're making. 00:59:37.000 |
If I just walk into a 10th grade class and go, 00:59:40.480 |
and I look up by the first three kids that I see, 00:59:49.960 |
and I'm likely to get a skewed representation, 00:59:55.040 |
So increasing sample size tends to decrease variation. 01:00:01.000 |
from the UK Biobank or from half a million Danish citizens, 01:00:09.880 |
So even though this is not an experimental study, 01:00:15.460 |
there's tremendous power by way of the enormous number 01:00:24.760 |
So you could never do a randomized assignment study 01:00:30.540 |
So epidemiology makes up for its biggest limitation, 01:00:36.740 |
which is it can never compensate for inherent biases 01:00:41.460 |
by saying we can do infinite duration if we want, 01:00:44.300 |
like we could survey people over the course of their lives, 01:00:47.320 |
and we can have the biggest sample size possible, 01:00:54.420 |
where you have tens of thousands of people is prohibitive. 01:00:56.400 |
I mean, if you look at the Women's Health Initiative, 01:00:57.920 |
which was a five-year study on, I don't know, 01:01:04.360 |
So this is the balancing act between epidemiology 01:01:14.740 |
but you just have to know the blind spots of each one. 01:01:21.520 |
InsideTracker is a personalized nutrition platform 01:01:29.820 |
I'm a big believer in getting regular blood work done 01:01:32.140 |
for the simple reason that many of the factors 01:01:34.460 |
that impact your immediate and long-term health 01:01:36.680 |
can only be analyzed from a quality blood test. 01:01:39.260 |
However, with a lot of blood tests out there, 01:01:44.320 |
but you don't know what to do with that information. 01:01:46.340 |
With InsideTracker, they have a personalized platform 01:01:48.640 |
that makes it very easy to understand your data, 01:01:55.300 |
and behavioral, supplement, nutrition, and other protocols 01:01:58.480 |
to adjust those numbers to bring them into the ranges 01:02:01.280 |
that are ideal for your immediate and long-term health. 01:02:03.560 |
InsideTracker's ultimate plan now includes measures 01:02:05.840 |
of both ApoB and of insulin, which are key indicators 01:02:09.480 |
of cardiovascular health and energy regulation. 01:02:19.300 |
Again, that's insidetracker.com/huberman to get 20% off. 01:02:27.560 |
which I think is the most important table in here, 01:02:35.840 |
So this is the way we wanna really be thinking about this. 01:02:44.440 |
and you always subtract one from the hazard ratio, 01:02:48.500 |
and that tells you, if it's a positive number, 01:02:52.040 |
if it's a number greater than one, you subtract one, 01:03:08.080 |
So that means it's a 15% reduction in relative risk, 01:03:12.200 |
and here you can see all the hazard ratios are positive. 01:03:18.260 |
'cause there's a lot of information packed here. 01:03:22.880 |
They're showing you three different ways that they do it. 01:03:30.040 |
with and without metformin and you make no adjustments, 01:03:36.620 |
meaning the people on metformin had a 48% greater chance 01:03:46.280 |
it's not 'cause I enjoy people dying quite to the contrary, 01:03:52.640 |
and that I've read some epidemiological studies before, 01:03:54.720 |
but it's not normally where I spend the majority of my time. 01:04:05.120 |
that I wasn't looking at all this backwards, okay? 01:04:11.740 |
that's taking metformin who doesn't have diabetes, 01:04:24.240 |
- Yeah, now there's another arm to this study 01:04:26.260 |
that I'm not getting into 'cause it adds more complexity, 01:04:33.880 |
and takes sulfonylureas, which is a bigger drug, 01:04:45.920 |
they're never able to erase the effect of diabetes. 01:04:49.700 |
- But in this case, it seems that they might be accelerating, 01:04:53.120 |
possibly accelerating death due to diabetes, possibly. 01:05:00.100 |
we would need to see diabetics who don't take metformin, 01:05:03.840 |
and I would bet that they would do even worse. 01:05:06.200 |
So my intuition is that the metformin is helping, 01:05:08.960 |
but not helping nearly as much as we thought before. 01:05:13.780 |
So my point is they make another set of adjustments. 01:05:18.160 |
in the unadjusted model, we only matched for age and gender. 01:05:24.280 |
What if we adjust for the medications they're on, 01:05:27.200 |
cardiovascular, psychiatric, pulmonary, dementia, meds, 01:05:31.900 |
I don't know why they threw marital status in there, 01:05:34.240 |
- I don't know, maybe being married or unmarried can-- 01:05:36.200 |
- I'm sure it can, but it just seems like a random thing 01:05:39.220 |
I would have personally done that adjustment higher up, 01:05:43.580 |
all of a sudden the hazard ratio drops from 1.48 to 1.32, 01:05:48.580 |
which means, yep, you still have a 32% greater chance 01:05:54.920 |
All right, what if we also adjust for the highest level 01:05:58.880 |
of education along with any of the other covariates? 01:06:03.880 |
It ends up at 1.33, or a 33% chance increase in death. 01:06:08.160 |
- I always knew that more school wasn't gonna save me. 01:06:13.800 |
If you do the twin study, which you could argue 01:06:15.760 |
is a slightly purer study, because you at least have 01:06:18.920 |
one genetic and environmental thing that you've attached, 01:06:29.460 |
These are twins who, in theory, are the same in every way, 01:06:34.980 |
and the one with diabetes on metformin still has 115% 01:06:38.960 |
greater chance of dying than the non-diabetic co-twin. 01:06:42.780 |
When you make that first adjustment of all the meds 01:06:45.140 |
and marital status, you bring it down to a 70% increase 01:07:00.140 |
So everything I just said here was based on no censoring. 01:07:06.020 |
- Censoring is when you stop counting the metformin people 01:07:09.620 |
Okay, so in the singleton group, when you unadjust it, 01:07:15.580 |
and the reason I'm doing the unadjusted is that's where 01:07:21.260 |
You just have to draw a line in the sand somewhere. 01:07:29.700 |
if you stop counting, pardon me, if you don't censor, 01:07:36.300 |
including when people on metformin with diabetes die. 01:07:43.020 |
In other words, this is a very important finding, 01:07:56.140 |
When keys censored, it got better, but not that much better. 01:08:04.820 |
In the twins, it went from 115% down to only 97%. 01:08:09.820 |
So in some ways, this presents a little bit of an enigma, 01:08:27.020 |
There's another technical detail of this paper, 01:08:30.220 |
which is they, you can see on the right side of table four, 01:08:33.160 |
they did something called a nested case control. 01:08:36.540 |
But you'll see, and I was gonna go into a long explanation 01:08:41.340 |
It's another pretty elegant way to do case control studies, 01:08:45.500 |
where you sample by year and you sort of normalize, 01:08:53.980 |
I don't think it's worth getting into, Andrew, 01:09:00.540 |
The point here is, the keys paper makes it undeniably clear 01:09:05.380 |
that in that population, there was no advantage 01:09:08.980 |
offered by metformin that undid the disadvantage 01:09:14.380 |
This does not mean that metformin wasn't helping them, 01:09:16.940 |
because we don't know what these people would have been like 01:09:20.700 |
It could be that this bought them a 50% reduction 01:09:31.680 |
This is what you would have expected 10 years ago 01:09:47.760 |
- One of, you know, it's not quite that simple 01:10:00.380 |
And presumably that was done and analyzed in other figures 01:10:16.200 |
or not on psychiatric meds as some way to tease out 01:10:20.000 |
some contribution in metformin to this result. 01:10:22.840 |
- Well, that's what they're doing in the partial adjustment, 01:10:32.600 |
- They're going drug by drug all the way through, 01:10:34.640 |
high blood pressure, non-high blood pressure, 01:10:37.640 |
- Right, and the way they would do that, presumably, 01:10:55.120 |
that can be purely explained by these other variables. 01:10:58.680 |
- Yes, although, again, this is a great opportunity 01:11:01.020 |
to talk about why, no matter how slick you are, 01:11:08.000 |
virtually every study that compares meat eaters 01:11:18.120 |
- Yes, and we can, or disease incidence studies. 01:11:26.860 |
until you realize that it takes a lot of work 01:11:34.760 |
that's a very significant decision a person makes. 01:11:42.760 |
And it is probably the case that they're making 01:11:45.560 |
other changes with respect to their health as well 01:11:50.560 |
Now, there's a million other problems with that. 01:11:53.440 |
because the whole meat discussion then gets into, 01:11:56.160 |
well, when we say eating meat, what do we mean? 01:12:05.040 |
- That's right, so how do we get into all those things? 01:12:07.000 |
But my point is, it's very difficult to quantify 01:12:12.080 |
And I think that even a study that goes to great lengths, 01:12:16.600 |
to make these corrections can never make the corrections. 01:12:18.700 |
And so, for me, the big takeaway of this study is, 01:12:28.060 |
And again, I was first critical of the Bannister paper 01:12:32.320 |
That's about the time I stopped taking Metformin, by the way. 01:12:37.320 |
But that was the first time I went back and said, 01:12:45.520 |
And I think we weren't looking at a true group 01:12:52.240 |
In other words, maybe, and even the Keys paper 01:12:56.520 |
doesn't tell us that Metformin wouldn't be beneficial, 01:13:05.720 |
would have been dying at a hazard ratio of three, 01:13:17.120 |
All of this is to say, absent a randomized control trial, 01:13:22.080 |
- Has there been a randomized control trial in Metformin? 01:13:31.760 |
which is kind of the gold standard for animal studies, 01:13:37.960 |
So it's an NIH funded program that's run out of three labs. 01:13:41.300 |
They basically test molecules for zero protection. 01:13:48.040 |
that really put rapamycin on the map in 2009. 01:13:50.360 |
That was the study that's fortuitously demonstrated 01:13:53.840 |
that even when rapamycin was given very, very late in life, 01:13:59.280 |
It still afforded them a 15% lifespan extension. 01:14:29.360 |
And so when something works in the ITP, it's pretty exciting. 01:14:36.940 |
Another one we should talk about at a subsequent time 01:14:44.760 |
and it produces comparable effects to rapamycin. 01:14:51.840 |
This is 17 alpha estradiol, not beta estradiol, 01:15:10.400 |
should try and have their estrogen as high as possible 01:15:15.200 |
because of the importance of estrogen for libido, 01:15:17.340 |
for brain function, tissue health, bone health. 01:15:21.980 |
and raising testosterone is just silly, right? 01:15:25.080 |
Let's just leave raising testosterone out of it. 01:15:27.760 |
But many of the approaches to raising testosterone 01:15:30.880 |
that are pharmacologic in nature also raise estrogen. 01:15:33.400 |
A lot of people try and push down on estrogen. 01:15:38.120 |
unless people are getting hyperestrogenic effects, 01:15:46.920 |
- Estrogen is a very important hormone for men and women. 01:15:51.960 |
Canagaflozin, an SGLT2 inhibitor, also very successful 01:16:05.760 |
so presumably you were taking it to buffer blood glucose 01:16:14.080 |
and this will be the last thing before we move on. 01:16:15.720 |
- Well, 'cause you couldn't go to the Dairy Queen 01:16:18.400 |
- No, finally the nausea went away after a few weeks 01:16:39.600 |
And only when you start to exercise should lactate go up. 01:16:47.440 |
So previously, when I was doing zone two testing, 01:16:49.720 |
I was just going off my power meter and heart rate. 01:16:54.840 |
and I started wanting to use the lactate threshold 01:17:04.360 |
And I'm like doing finger pricks before I start 01:17:14.540 |
- Well, no, that would put me a lot higher, right? 01:17:17.200 |
And when I-- - I was being generous to your fitness. 01:17:19.980 |
- No, but that's when I started doing a little digging 01:17:30.960 |
You're gonna shunt more glucose into pyruvate 01:17:36.040 |
I'm anaerobic at a baseline. - Yeah, you need 01:17:40.240 |
And then my zone two numbers just seemed off. 01:18:04.940 |
because I like to eat meat and vegetables and starches. 01:18:13.640 |
I could exercise, I could think, I could sleep. 01:18:21.840 |
what I eat is to enjoy myself, but also have mental energy. 01:18:27.480 |
If I don't replenish, I'm going to feel like garbage. 01:18:29.000 |
I don't care how lean I am or what, you know. 01:18:41.440 |
except after a resistance training session, et cetera. 01:18:44.000 |
But one day a week, you have this so-called cheat day. 01:18:49.760 |
And so I would eat, you know, eight croissants, 01:18:55.140 |
you don't want to look at an item of food at all. 01:18:57.120 |
So the only modification I made to the slow carb diet 01:19:00.340 |
four hour body thing was the day after the cheat day, 01:19:13.840 |
But since you said that, I won't up the ante here, 01:19:16.940 |
but I'll at least match your anal seepage comment by saying, 01:19:19.620 |
I had, let's just call it profound gastric distress 01:19:24.120 |
So the last thing you want to do is eat any food, 01:19:32.920 |
poor man's metformin, could buffer blood glucose, 01:19:40.680 |
and in many cases, spiking my blood sugar and insulin 01:19:45.180 |
because you're having ice cream and et cetera. 01:20:05.360 |
I wasn't crashing in the afternoon nap and that whole thing. 01:20:08.640 |
- And do you remember how much you were taking? 01:20:10.080 |
- I think it was a couple hundred milligrams. 01:20:20.160 |
that if I took berberine and I did not ingest 01:20:23.240 |
a profound number of carbohydrates very soon afterwards, 01:20:29.840 |
I didn't measure it, but I just felt I had headaches. 01:20:33.200 |
And then I would eat a pizza or two and feel fine. 01:20:37.000 |
And so I realized that berberine was putting me 01:20:48.960 |
'cause I don't follow the slow carb diet anymore, 01:20:55.680 |
I didn't have any reason to take the berberine 01:20:57.920 |
and I feared that I wasn't ingesting enough carbohydrates 01:21:00.900 |
in order to really justify trying to buffer my blood glucose. 01:21:03.320 |
Also, my blood glucose tends to be fairly low. 01:21:18.600 |
that actually found a survival benefit in the ITP. 01:21:29.420 |
Anybody can nominate a candidate to be tested. 01:21:32.160 |
Then the panel over there reviews it and they decide, 01:21:36.040 |
When I think David Allison nominated Acarbos to be studied, 01:21:41.040 |
the rationale was it would be a caloric restriction mimetic 01:21:45.060 |
because you would literally just fail to absorb, 01:21:49.660 |
15 to 20% of your carbohydrates would not be absorbed 01:21:53.960 |
the mice would effectively be calorically restricted. 01:22:05.880 |
So they lived longer, but not through calorie restriction. 01:22:14.160 |
because they had lower glucose and lower insulin. 01:22:17.200 |
- And I don't want to send this down some rabbit holes here, 01:22:19.720 |
but there are all sorts of interesting ideas about, 01:22:31.200 |
lowering blood glucose, ketogenic diets, et cetera, 01:22:36.600 |
and I know you've explored a lot of that on your podcast. 01:22:43.480 |
One, you don't want insulin too high, nor too low. 01:22:47.520 |
You don't want blood glucose too high, nor too low. 01:22:50.480 |
If the buffering systems for that are disrupted, 01:22:56.400 |
is the best way to keep that system in check. 01:23:16.520 |
- The only one that I take is an SGLT2 inhibitor. 01:23:25.020 |
by people with type two diabetes, which I don't have, 01:23:27.800 |
but because of my faith in the mechanistic studies 01:23:31.720 |
of this drug, coupled with its results in the ITP, 01:23:41.720 |
I think there's something very special about that drug. 01:23:51.720 |
as a way to extend life, or are you more of the, 01:23:58.540 |
in your book, "Outlive" and elsewhere on your podcast. 01:24:06.660 |
obviously you believe in buffering blood glucose 01:24:09.000 |
in addition to just doing all the right behaviors. 01:24:10.880 |
- Yeah, I think you can uncouple a little bit 01:24:12.880 |
the buffering of blood glucose from the caloric deficit. 01:24:15.660 |
So I think you can be in a reasonable energy balance 01:24:28.160 |
So it's not entirely clear if profound caloric restriction 01:24:34.840 |
even if it were tolerable to most, which it's not. 01:24:37.560 |
So for most people, it's just kind of off the table. 01:24:39.980 |
Like if I said, Andrew, you need to eat 30% fewer calories 01:24:50.720 |
But the question is, do you need to be fasting all the time? 01:24:56.640 |
Do you need to be doing all of these other things? 01:24:58.800 |
And the answer appears to be outside of using them as tools 01:25:03.400 |
to manage energy balance, it's not clear, right? 01:25:06.060 |
And energy balance probably plays a greater role 01:25:10.600 |
in glucose homeostasis from a nutrition standpoint 01:25:15.600 |
than the individual constituents of the meal. 01:25:24.920 |
eating Twix bars all day and drinking big gulps. 01:25:29.080 |
But I also don't think that's a very sustainable thing to do 01:25:33.580 |
in negative energy balance consuming that much crap, 01:25:42.800 |
You're not gonna be satiated eating pure garbage 01:25:48.540 |
You're gonna end up having to go into caloric excess. 01:25:51.240 |
So that's why it's interesting thought experiment. 01:25:53.600 |
I don't think it's a very practical experiment. 01:25:56.960 |
and in energy balance, they're probably eating 01:26:00.760 |
But I don't think that the specific macros matter 01:26:05.560 |
- I'm a believer in getting most of my nutrients 01:26:08.640 |
from unprocessed or minimally processed sources 01:26:12.200 |
simply because it allows me to eat foods I like 01:26:20.200 |
I so physically enjoy the sensation of chewing 01:26:23.400 |
that I'll just eat cucumber slices for fun, right? 01:26:28.320 |
I mean, that's not my only form of fun fortunately. 01:26:30.680 |
This is an amazing paper for the simple reason 01:26:41.240 |
of the benefits and drawbacks of this type of work. 01:26:45.720 |
And I think it's also wonderful because we hear a lot 01:27:05.460 |
And I give them a much, much, much, much shorter version 01:27:12.340 |
which should answer this question more definitively, right? 01:27:17.600 |
and randomizing them to placebo versus metformin 01:27:26.860 |
that there is a zero protective benefit of metformin, 01:27:34.920 |
I think, all walk around with an appropriate degree 01:27:37.280 |
of humility around what we know and what we don't know. 01:27:51.400 |
which is the impact on hypertrophy and strength, 01:27:53.540 |
which appears to be attenuated as well by metformin. 01:27:56.140 |
I still prescribe it to patients all the time 01:28:05.860 |
for the person who's insulin sensitive and exercising a lot. 01:28:31.400 |
where I'm going to be in a slight caloric deficit, 01:28:37.000 |
and then go back to eating the way that I ate before, 01:28:44.000 |
Is there any benefit to it in terms of cellular health? 01:28:51.200 |
the clearing of senescent cells that we hear about, 01:28:57.640 |
and then go back to your regular pattern of eating 01:29:00.140 |
and then periodically, once every couple of weeks 01:29:11.440 |
Because there's all discipline function there, 01:29:16.120 |
there's probably an insulin sensitivity function, 01:29:17.880 |
but is there any evidence that it can help us live longer? 01:29:21.040 |
- I think the short answer is no, for two reasons. 01:29:25.040 |
One, I don't think that that duration would be sufficient 01:29:29.440 |
but two, even if you went with something longer, 01:29:33.860 |
I used to do seven days of water only per quarter, 01:29:38.800 |
So I was, basically always like it would be three day fast, 01:29:43.860 |
Just imagine doing that all year, rotating, rotating, 01:29:48.860 |
Now I certainly believed, and to this day I would say 01:30:05.920 |
which I obviously now look back at and realize 01:30:09.920 |
It's very difficult to gain back the muscle cumulatively, 01:30:17.200 |
like there's got to be a resetting of the system here. 01:30:24.120 |
but you're getting at a bigger problem with neuroscience, 01:30:44.120 |
So we're really good at using molecules or interventions 01:30:54.460 |
you can look at how much weight you're lifting, 01:30:58.460 |
and see how much muscle mass you're generating, 01:31:01.760 |
Those are giving you outputs that say my input is good 01:31:15.120 |
When you take metformin, when you take rapamycin, 01:31:23.220 |
that gives us any insight into whether or not 01:31:36.140 |
you would want to see more research dollars put to 01:31:41.060 |
And it's unquestionably this, as unsexy as it is, 01:31:48.340 |
I don't think we're going to get great answers 01:31:56.960 |
Well, I'm grateful that you're sitting across the table 01:31:59.900 |
for me telling me all this and that everyone can hear this. 01:32:03.820 |
But again, we will put a link to the papers, plural, 01:32:10.120 |
And for those of you that are listening and not watching, 01:32:21.740 |
But my additional suggestion on parsing papers 01:32:31.380 |
Unlike a newspaper article or a Instagram post, 01:32:35.620 |
with a paper you're not necessarily going to get it 01:32:40.820 |
So I think spending some time with papers for me 01:32:52.380 |
Like if you're encountering a paper for the first time, 01:32:55.240 |
do you have an order in which you like to go through? 01:33:02.180 |
- Yeah, unless it's an area that I know very, very well 01:33:12.060 |
And actually this is fun because I used to teach a class 01:33:17.620 |
called Neural Circuits in Health and Disease. 01:33:19.400 |
And it was an evening course that grew very quickly 01:33:27.500 |
And I had everyone ask what I called the four questions. 01:33:34.560 |
but I have a little three by five card next to me 01:33:37.740 |
or a piece of a main half by 11 paper typically. 01:33:42.240 |
I want to figure out what is the question they're asking? 01:34:02.960 |
but it is worthwhile that if you encounter a method 01:34:06.800 |
like PCR or, you know, chromatography or fMRI 01:34:24.940 |
What are, you know, figure one, here's the header. 01:34:29.460 |
it will tell you what they want you to think they found. 01:34:32.340 |
And then I tend to want to know the conclusion of the study. 01:34:37.500 |
And this is the one that would really distinguish 01:34:40.740 |
the high-performing students from the others. 01:34:54.500 |
spending some time thinking about what they identified. 01:35:00.820 |
and different papers require different formats. 01:35:03.040 |
But those four questions really form the cornerstone 01:35:10.200 |
And again, it's something that can be cultivated. 01:35:18.440 |
So what I do typically is I'll read title, abstract. 01:35:36.900 |
the cell press journals too, into each figure. 01:35:39.620 |
And it's coded with no definition of the acronyms 01:35:42.180 |
that almost always I'm into the introduction and results 01:35:56.900 |
or was it our friend Paul Conte when he was here 01:36:00.140 |
who said that, oh no, I'm sorry, it was neither. 01:36:05.220 |
Dr. Jeffrey Goldberg, who was a guest on a podcast recently, 01:36:09.980 |
told us that if you look at the total number of words 01:36:14.140 |
and terms that a physician leaving medical school 01:36:20.660 |
it's the equivalent of like two additional full languages 01:36:28.820 |
and I don't know, do you speak a language other than English? 01:36:35.700 |
So no one is expected to be able to parse these papers 01:36:39.480 |
the first time through without substantial training. 01:36:50.620 |
versus when I'm not. - Yeah, how do you do it? 01:37:00.120 |
And then sometimes I'll just do a quick skim on methods. 01:37:18.700 |
like what do you mean, what is this, how did they do that? 01:37:23.180 |
And then I gotta go back and read methods typically. 01:37:26.020 |
And one of the other things that's probably worth mentioning 01:37:28.220 |
is a lot of papers these days have supplemental information 01:37:43.520 |
So a lot of the times when you're submitting something, 01:37:46.780 |
like if you wanna put any additional information in there, 01:37:53.200 |
there were a couple of the numbers I spouted off 01:37:57.400 |
For example, when they did the sensitivity analysis 01:38:07.140 |
That was actually not even in the paper we presented. 01:38:15.420 |
It's an experimental paper where there's a manipulation. 01:38:24.200 |
I'm so excited about this paper for so many reasons, 01:38:28.040 |
but I wanna give a couple of caveats up front. 01:38:30.460 |
First of all, the paper is not published yet. 01:38:40.320 |
I would say five, six years of people posting the papers 01:38:42.940 |
that they've submitted to journals for peer review online 01:38:46.340 |
so that people can look at them prior to those papers 01:39:04.820 |
First of all, the group that published this paper 01:39:11.880 |
And they publish a lot of really nice papers in this area. 01:39:21.520 |
who's at the Econ School of Medicine in Mount Sinai, 01:39:24.500 |
runs a laboratory there studying addiction in humans. 01:39:28.300 |
And the first author of the paper is Ofer Pearl. 01:39:34.800 |
And I'll just give you a couple of the takeaways first 01:39:37.120 |
as a bit of a hook to hopefully entice people 01:39:43.500 |
This paper basically addresses how our beliefs 01:39:54.580 |
not just at a subjective level, but at a biological level. 01:39:59.900 |
a former guest on this podcast, Dr. Ali Crum, 01:40:05.500 |
but she goes by Ali Crum, talked about belief effects. 01:40:08.540 |
Belief effects are different than placebo effects. 01:40:11.920 |
Placebo effects are really just category effects. 01:40:14.940 |
It's, okay, I'm going to give you this pill, Peter, 01:40:24.320 |
and that it's going to make your memory better. 01:40:30.820 |
who I give a pill to and I say, this is just a placebo. 01:40:36.780 |
where people will get a drug and you tell them it's placebo. 01:40:39.980 |
They'll get a placebo, you tell them it's drug. 01:40:45.460 |
You're either in the drug group or the placebo group, 01:40:47.500 |
and you're either told that you're getting drug or placebo. 01:41:12.620 |
and just reminds me of some of the horrors of high school. 01:41:30.460 |
Belief effects are not A or B, placebo or non-placebo. 01:41:37.120 |
to enrich one's belief about a certain something 01:41:41.140 |
that can shift their psychology and physiology 01:41:45.360 |
And I think the best examples of these, really, 01:41:47.860 |
of these belief effects really do come from Allie Crump's lab 01:42:00.980 |
about how stress really limits our performance, 01:42:03.580 |
let's say at archery or at mathematics or at music 01:42:07.640 |
and then you test them in any of those domains 01:42:10.700 |
or other domains in a stressful circumstance, 01:42:18.460 |
because we're by virtue of a heightened stress response. 01:42:40.420 |
It narrows your vision such that you can perform tasks better 01:42:43.940 |
and their performance increases above a control group 01:42:48.620 |
or at least useless as it relates to the task. 01:42:54.560 |
Stress can be depleting or it can enhance performance, 01:42:59.920 |
because now it's scaling according to the amount 01:43:02.780 |
and the type of information that they're getting. 01:43:07.140 |
of benefit or detriment that one could experience 01:43:09.500 |
in a situation like the one you just described? 01:43:14.220 |
So the stress gets us worse, makes you, let's say, 01:43:18.020 |
I think that if we were to just put a rough percentage 01:43:20.540 |
on this, it would be somewhere between 10 and 30% worse 01:43:30.580 |
Even more striking is the studies that Ali's lab did 01:43:41.420 |
and then you measure ghrelin secretion in the blood. 01:43:44.620 |
And ghrelin is a marker of hunger that increases 01:43:49.060 |
And what you notice is that suppresses ghrelin 01:43:51.220 |
to a great degree and for a long period of time. 01:43:58.840 |
but that doesn't have much fat, not much sugar, et cetera. 01:44:01.220 |
They drink the shake, less ghrelin suppression. 01:44:07.380 |
And satiety lines up with that also in that study. 01:44:10.580 |
And then the third one, which is also pretty striking, 01:44:16.080 |
informing them that moving around during the day 01:44:17.920 |
and vacuuming and doing all that kind of thing is great. 01:44:30.980 |
leaning down, cleaning out trash cans, et cetera. 01:44:37.260 |
lose 12% more weight compared to the other group. 01:44:47.200 |
I mean, literally this was sparked by, in Allie's words, 01:44:50.980 |
this was sparked by her graduate advisor saying, 01:44:55.580 |
"What if all the effects of exercise are placebo?" 01:44:58.220 |
Right, which is not what anyone really believes, 01:45:05.100 |
because it just really speaks to how really smart 01:45:18.420 |
'Cause the nervous system extends through both." 01:45:25.880 |
which is really about belief effects, not placebo effects. 01:45:32.700 |
we know that nicotine, vaped, smoked, dipped, or snuffed, 01:45:37.420 |
or these little zen pouches, or taken in capsule form, 01:45:55.220 |
Well, you have a couple of sites in the brain, 01:45:57.380 |
namely in the basal forebrain, nucleus basalis, 01:46:01.140 |
in the back of the brain structures like locus coeruleus, 01:46:05.340 |
but also this, what's called, it's got a funny name, 01:46:13.880 |
that sends those little axon wires into the thalamus. 01:46:16.340 |
The thalamus is a gateway for sensory information. 01:46:21.260 |
the auditory information, it has nicotinic receptors. 01:46:25.180 |
And when the pedunculopontine nucleus releases nicotine, 01:46:29.580 |
what it does is it increases the signal to noise 01:46:32.600 |
of information coming in through your senses. 01:46:35.060 |
So the fidelity of the signal that gets up to your cortex, 01:46:38.060 |
which is your conscious perception of those senses, 01:46:41.260 |
- And how much endogenous nicotine do we produce? 01:46:49.460 |
- We're not making nicotine. - We're just binding. 01:46:50.420 |
So this is a nicotinic acetylcholine receptor. 01:46:57.620 |
But so, right, they're called nicotinic receptors 01:47:03.200 |
that cannabinoid receptors are called cannabinoid receptors, 01:47:14.200 |
- The drug is named after the receptor, yeah. 01:47:21.780 |
or pedunculopontine nucleus or locus coeruleus, 01:47:31.060 |
in general, it just tends to be a signal-to-noise enhancer. 01:47:34.120 |
And so for the non-engineering types out there, no problem. 01:47:39.660 |
and there's a lot of static in the background. 01:47:43.940 |
We can reduce the static or I can increase the fidelity, 01:47:46.980 |
the volume and the clarity of what I'm saying, okay? 01:47:51.260 |
For instance, and that's really what acetylcholine does. 01:47:56.200 |
they get that boost of nicotine and they just feel clear. 01:48:09.300 |
the mesolimbic reward pathway that releases dopamine. 01:48:11.740 |
And typically when nicotine is increased in our system, 01:48:16.060 |
That's one of the reasons why nicotine is reinforcing. 01:48:20.680 |
I've done beautiful experiments with honeybees even, 01:48:25.100 |
or it comes from certain plants and they'll forage there. 01:48:31.240 |
In any event, there's also an output from this thing, 01:48:34.300 |
the thalamus, to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, 01:48:37.700 |
which is an area of the forebrain that really allows us 01:48:40.240 |
to limit our focus and our attention for sake of learning. 01:48:45.760 |
- You talked about this in your fantastic podcast 01:48:59.760 |
- Yeah, why it's counterintuitive that a stimulant 01:49:02.600 |
would be a treatment for someone with difficulty focusing. 01:49:06.200 |
- Yeah, in young kids who have difficulty focusing, 01:49:08.920 |
if you give them something they love, they're like a laser. 01:49:12.580 |
And the reason is that ventromedial prefrontal cortex circuit 01:49:16.840 |
can engage is when the kid is interested and engaged, 01:49:20.400 |
but kids with ADD, ADHD tend to have a hard time 01:49:28.420 |
And it turns out that giving those stimulant drugs 01:49:30.580 |
in many cases can enhance the function of that circuit 01:49:34.420 |
and it can strengthen so that ideally the kids 01:49:38.820 |
although that's not often the way that it plays out. 01:49:53.960 |
that can help improve their symptoms without drugs? 01:49:57.000 |
Is the combination of all those things together 01:50:07.400 |
Some kids need a lot, some kids need a little. 01:50:09.160 |
I probably just gained and lost a few enemies there. 01:50:12.160 |
So the point is that these circuits are hardwired circuits. 01:50:23.020 |
doesn't nicotine potentially have a calming effect as well? 01:50:26.860 |
And that seems a bit counterintuitive to the focusing one. 01:50:42.340 |
So it's kind of the perfect drug if you think about it. 01:50:54.000 |
and now hardly anyone smokes for all the obvious reasons. 01:50:56.420 |
But yeah, it provides that really ideal balance 01:51:12.060 |
come into the laboratory, they gave them a vape pen. 01:51:21.620 |
Typically there's a washout before they come in 01:51:30.420 |
- Yeah, which must be miserable for those people. 01:51:33.100 |
'cause they can't have nicorette gum or anything. 01:51:36.840 |
- But they can measure carbon monoxide, right? 01:51:40.140 |
and they're measuring nicotine in the blood as well. 01:51:57.420 |
into a functional magnetic resonance imaging machine. 01:52:00.420 |
So where they can look at, it's really blood flow, 01:52:06.540 |
it's the ratio of the oxygenated to deoxygenated blood 01:52:09.380 |
because when blood will flow to neurons that are active 01:52:15.620 |
and then there's a change in what's called the bold signal. 01:52:18.100 |
So FMRI, when you see these like hotspots in the brain 01:52:24.220 |
And then there's some interesting physics around 01:52:34.900 |
The right hand rule, if you put your thumb out 01:52:39.660 |
I think that the thumb represents the charge, 01:52:42.860 |
And then isn't the electromagnetic field is the downward 01:52:45.660 |
facing figure and then it's, do I have that right? 01:52:52.580 |
But what you do is when you put a person's head 01:52:54.300 |
in this big magnet and then you pulse the magnet, 01:52:56.960 |
what happens is the oxygenated and deoxygenated blood, 01:53:00.980 |
it interacts with the magnetic field differently 01:53:03.500 |
and that difference in signal can be detected. 01:53:05.680 |
And you can see that in the form of activated brain areas. 01:53:08.900 |
- Yeah, I mean, MRI all works by proton detection. 01:53:11.980 |
So presumably there's a difference in the proton signal 01:53:15.260 |
when you have high oxygen versus low oxygen concentration. 01:53:20.180 |
And what they'll do is they'll pulse with the magnet 01:53:23.620 |
and this is definitely getting beyond my expertise, 01:53:26.800 |
but that the spin orientation of the protons, 01:53:29.460 |
then it's going to relax back at a different rate as well. 01:53:34.820 |
you can also get not just resting state activation, 01:53:41.120 |
But you can look at connectivity between areas 01:53:44.420 |
and how one area is driving the activity of another area. 01:53:48.840 |
So what they do is they put people in a scanner 01:53:53.220 |
- What are the limitations of FMRI in terms of, 01:53:59.240 |
I mean, where are the blind spots of the technique? 01:54:01.620 |
- So resolution, you can get down to sub centimeter. 01:54:05.020 |
They talk about it always in these paper as a voxels, 01:54:13.160 |
but you're not going to get down to millimeter. 01:54:20.100 |
that have been basically worked out over the last 10 years 01:54:24.200 |
You can't just give somebody a stimulus compared to nothing. 01:54:30.160 |
that when someone would move their right hand, 01:54:33.880 |
I just went for one of these recently for clinical, 01:54:36.140 |
not a problem, but just for a diagnostics hand, 01:54:38.080 |
you're leaning back and you can move your right hand a bit. 01:54:41.420 |
And they would see an area in motor cortex lighting up. 01:54:45.640 |
corresponding to the left hand was also lighting up. 01:55:08.680 |
and did an, like they did an FMRI of a dead salmon 01:55:12.140 |
that demonstrated like some interesting signal. 01:55:17.000 |
- We got to find this one for the show notes. 01:55:19.680 |
- We should do one of these wild papers ones. 01:55:22.120 |
There are papers of people putting, don't do this folks, 01:55:24.960 |
putting elephants on LSD that were published in science 01:55:27.660 |
and things like that, like crazy experiments. 01:55:29.320 |
We should definitely do a crazy experiments journal club. 01:55:36.980 |
with fairly high spatial resolution, fairly high, 01:55:47.500 |
because a lot of neural transmission is happening 01:55:51.500 |
especially when you're in talking about auditory processing. 01:56:09.620 |
You'll like this 'cause you have a background in finance. 01:56:20.480 |
and they're looking at a squiggle line, then it stops. 01:56:25.000 |
They're either going to invest a certain number 01:56:35.280 |
You could explain shorting better than I could, for sure. 01:56:43.360 |
and they're going to be rewarded in real money 01:56:46.920 |
So this is going to engage this type of circuitry. 01:56:48.820 |
Now, remember, these groups were given a vape pen 01:57:02.520 |
The goal is not to get them to perform better on the task. 01:57:05.400 |
The goal is to engage the specific brain areas 01:57:15.040 |
that includes the mesolimbic reward pathway and dopamine. 01:57:17.840 |
It includes the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. 01:57:20.920 |
First of all, they measure nicotine in the blood. 01:57:28.840 |
for the sake of experiment and not recommending people vape, 01:57:34.620 |
after they can measure how long they inhaled, 01:57:43.060 |
They measured people's belief as to whether or not 01:57:45.680 |
they got low, medium, or high amounts of nicotine. 01:57:54.040 |
And then of course they looked at brain area activation 01:57:58.680 |
And what they found was very straightforward. 01:58:00.560 |
- Sorry, they were all given the same amount. 01:58:03.840 |
I was going to offer it as a punchline, but that's okay. 01:58:05.680 |
No, I think that the cool thing about this experiment 01:58:12.840 |
of relatively low nicotine containing vape pen. 01:58:18.020 |
and they're measuring it from their bloodstream. 01:58:19.400 |
So they all have fairly low levels of nicotine, 01:58:30.600 |
but the most interesting things are the following. 01:58:36.280 |
of being on the drug matches what they were told. 01:58:42.880 |
Like, "Yeah, it feels like a high amount of nicotine." 01:58:47.760 |
they're like, "Yeah, that feels like a medium amount." 01:58:49.600 |
If it was a low amount, they think it was a low amount. 01:58:58.120 |
But if you look at the activation of the thalamus 01:59:04.680 |
where you would predict acetylcholine transmission 01:59:16.780 |
It scales with what they thought they got in the vape pen. 01:59:24.720 |
you got a little bit of activation in these areas. 01:59:26.840 |
If you were told that you got a medium amount of nicotine 01:59:35.840 |
And if you were told you got high amounts of nicotine, 01:59:47.120 |
everyone got the exact same amount of nicotine in reality. 01:59:52.520 |
isn't just changing what one subjectively experiences. 01:59:56.160 |
Oh, this is the effect of high nicotine or low nicotine. 02:00:12.200 |
that if you believe you got a lot of nicotine, 02:00:14.200 |
you're just faster or you're reading the lines better 02:00:17.360 |
or your response time to hit the button is quicker. 02:00:38.880 |
that would represent that kind of difference, 02:00:50.480 |
that pathway scales in the most beautiful way 02:00:54.160 |
such that people that were told they had smoked a low 02:01:02.240 |
People that were told that they got a moderate amount 02:01:04.200 |
of nicotine got a more robust activation of that pathway. 02:01:07.280 |
And the people that were told that they got a high amount 02:01:09.240 |
of nicotine in the vape pen saw a very robust activation 02:01:12.840 |
of the thalamus to this ventral prefrontal cortical pathway. 02:01:17.680 |
under the hood of the skull simply on the basis 02:01:20.680 |
of what they were told and what they believe. 02:01:23.000 |
- And technically, the fMRI is showing the activation 02:01:26.320 |
of those two areas, and that's how you can infer 02:01:32.180 |
There's a separate method called diffuser tensor imaging, 02:01:38.280 |
Minnesota has a very robust group in terms of neuroimaging 02:01:42.020 |
that can measure activation in fiber pathways. 02:01:44.720 |
This is not that, but you can look at the timing 02:01:49.760 |
So we haven't talked so much about figures here, 02:01:52.260 |
but I guess if we were gonna look at any one figure, 02:01:58.980 |
that doesn't have the figure in front of them. 02:02:01.440 |
Let's see, probably the most important figure is figure two. 02:02:08.320 |
Remember, I said I like to read the titles of figures, 02:02:12.180 |
which is that the belief about nicotine strength 02:02:14.400 |
induced a dose-dependent response in the thalamus. 02:02:23.300 |
that's essentially the response that they saw. 02:02:32.080 |
as a function of the estimate in the thalamus 02:02:39.660 |
it's a mess when you look at all the dots at once, 02:02:41.820 |
but if you just separate it out by high, medium, and low, 02:02:45.120 |
what you find is that there's a gradual increase, 02:02:48.340 |
but a legitimate one from low to medium to high. 02:02:55.500 |
your brain will react as if it's a high dose of nicotine. 02:02:58.320 |
Now, what they didn't do was give people zero nicotine. 02:03:02.200 |
there's a control that's missing here, right? 02:03:04.240 |
- Yeah, so what they didn't do is give people zero nicotine 02:03:07.040 |
and then tell them this is a high amount of nicotine, 02:03:10.360 |
sort of the equivalent of the cruel high school experiment. 02:03:22.740 |
It's unclear whether or not they had been drunk previously, 02:03:28.820 |
to feel drunk, et cetera, and there's the social context. 02:03:33.820 |
and outrageously interesting about this study 02:03:36.340 |
is simply that what we are told about the dose of a drug 02:03:44.480 |
And in my understanding, this is the first study 02:03:48.100 |
to ever look at dose dependence of belief effects, right? 02:03:58.180 |
You look at zero, low dose, medium dose, high dose, 02:04:01.400 |
and here they clearly are seeing a dose dependent response 02:04:14.340 |
In other words, you can bypass pharmacology somewhat, right? 02:04:25.040 |
You've got the group who were told they got a low dose, 02:04:28.100 |
the group who was told they got a medium dose, 02:04:30.480 |
the group that was told they had a high dose, 02:04:40.200 |
- Yeah, this is measuring parameter estimate. 02:04:57.480 |
So what they're doing is they're just saying, 02:05:10.900 |
- And nobody else was statistically different. 02:05:15.420 |
So when you look at the output from the thalamus 02:05:46.180 |
- So isn't it interesting that at the thalamus, 02:05:50.360 |
which is, and you'll immediately appreciate my stupidity 02:06:04.360 |
you have a lower difference of signal strength, 02:06:13.720 |
- It is surprising, and it surprised them as well, 02:06:26.820 |
is that it doesn't take much nicotinic receptor occupancy 02:06:37.820 |
but in terms of its output to the prefrontal cortex, 02:06:42.260 |
- Because that figure, 4B, is more convincing 02:06:52.580 |
the correlation coefficient is .27, it's not that strong. 02:07:00.540 |
by the way, this goes back to our earlier discussion, 02:07:03.040 |
there could be a huge signal here and we're underpowered, 02:07:06.500 |
So you wouldn't have a lot of subjects in this experiment. 02:07:08.540 |
- Yeah, no, and this just speaks to the general challenge 02:07:14.060 |
It's hard to get a lot of people in and through the scanner. 02:08:07.280 |
they'll miss out on some of the significance, 02:08:19.940 |
I was also surprised that they didn't see a difference, 02:08:22.420 |
this is kind of interesting in its own right, 02:08:34.060 |
- Yeah, exactly, so if you look at figure 3B, 02:08:39.320 |
what you'll see is that there's no difference 02:08:45.260 |
in these reward pathways if people got a low, medium, 02:08:48.740 |
Now, that actually could be leveraged, I believe, 02:08:53.160 |
if somebody were trying to quit nicotine, for instance, 02:08:57.360 |
by progressively reducing the amount of nicotine 02:09:01.400 |
but you told them that it was the same amount, 02:09:10.780 |
and if they believed it to be a greater amount, 02:09:13.060 |
then it might actually not disrupt their reward pathways, 02:09:25.740 |
but it was done exactly the same way with nonsmokers? 02:09:30.340 |
- Ooh, well, one thing that's sort of interesting, 02:09:34.060 |
you asked about potential sources of artifact, 02:09:38.980 |
One of the challenges that they know in this study 02:09:41.060 |
was you have to stay very still in the machine, 02:09:47.780 |
So, okay, so presumably the data would be higher fidelity. 02:09:52.420 |
but I was like, I had to read that one twice. 02:09:55.680 |
They're smokers, they're coughing, they can't stay still, 02:10:01.380 |
I think that for people that are naive to nicotine, 02:10:11.740 |
Sort of like the first time effect of pretty much any drug. 02:10:14.180 |
- But I wonder if they would be more or less susceptible 02:10:22.020 |
Right, because they have no prior to compare it to. 02:10:27.160 |
with respect to the obviously beneficial effects of nicotine 02:10:36.000 |
into thinking the non-alcoholic beer was at alcohol, 02:10:46.120 |
But that kid, having never been actually drunk before, 02:10:50.560 |
presumably would experience it more susceptible. 02:10:52.280 |
- I would feel like he'd be more susceptible potentially. 02:10:56.540 |
So, you know, my glee for this experiment is not, 02:11:02.480 |
is not because I think it's the be all end all, 02:11:08.040 |
that they're starting to explore dose dependence of belief, 02:11:21.120 |
we're talking about a behavioral intervention, 02:11:25.200 |
and I'm not referring to any one specific vaccine. 02:11:39.960 |
we were talking about before, metformin, et cetera. 02:11:44.520 |
What we believe about the effects of a drug, presumably, 02:11:50.360 |
in addition to what we believe about how much we're taking 02:12:04.960 |
that have peripheral effects or peripheral outputs 02:12:16.200 |
You know, I don't think anybody fully understands 02:12:22.560 |
but it's hard to argue that they're impacting, 02:12:25.440 |
that the GLP-1 analog is having a central impact. 02:12:35.200 |
- Yeah, and I think the mouse data point to different areas 02:12:38.000 |
of the hypothalamus that are related to satiety, 02:12:55.320 |
But again, it speaks to like, what do you need to believe 02:13:05.560 |
I mean, of course, those drugs have all been tested 02:13:07.200 |
via placebo and the placebo groups, you know, 02:13:11.120 |
That's how we know that there's activity of the drug. 02:13:15.720 |
that's a little bit different than being told 02:13:24.520 |
you might be getting it, you might not be getting it. 02:13:26.760 |
So it's not quite the same as this experiment. 02:13:28.720 |
This experiment is one level up where you're being told, 02:13:35.960 |
- Yeah, to take this to maybe the ADHD realm, 02:13:38.440 |
let's say a kid has been on ADHD meds for a while 02:13:42.400 |
the physician decided they want to cut back on the dosage. 02:13:46.040 |
But if they were to tell the kid it's the same dosage 02:13:49.520 |
and it's had a certain positive effect for them, 02:13:52.160 |
according to the results, at least in this paper, 02:13:55.640 |
which are not definitive, but are interesting, 02:14:02.360 |
And this is the part that makes it so cool to me is that, 02:14:15.900 |
So that's where this- - No, that's very cool. 02:14:17.400 |
- This is why, because it's done in the brain, 02:14:21.180 |
it gets to these kind of abstract, nearly mystical, 02:14:24.720 |
but not quite mystical aspects of belief effects, 02:14:32.740 |
but it's clear that one of the more important pieces of data 02:14:36.200 |
are your beliefs about how these things impact you. 02:14:44.640 |
The thalamus is behaving as if it's a high dose 02:14:47.000 |
when it's the same dose as the low dose group. 02:14:50.760 |
- Yeah, I mean, I think of the implications, for example, 02:14:54.200 |
Like we don't really understand essential hypertension, 02:14:56.840 |
which is the majority of people walking around 02:15:04.600 |
How do we know that the belief system about it 02:15:09.240 |
And yeah, this is, I don't know, this is eye-opening. 02:15:15.960 |
And allochrome is onto some other really cool stuff. 02:15:21.580 |
where these belief effects are starting to show up. 02:15:23.880 |
If you tell a group that the side effects of a drug 02:15:27.860 |
that they're taking are evidence that the drug really works 02:15:33.680 |
even though those side effects are kind of annoying, 02:15:38.120 |
and they report more relief from the primary symptoms 02:15:46.480 |
- can really impact how quickly and how compatible 02:15:51.260 |
we feel about, how quickly a drug works, excuse me, 02:16:02.620 |
And you don't want to lie to people obviously, 02:16:08.220 |
which is reading the list of side effects of a drug 02:16:11.760 |
and then developing all of those side effects 02:16:15.200 |
when, and then maybe later coming to the understanding 02:16:18.460 |
that some of those were raised through belief effects. 02:16:23.920 |
That's the one we see a lot with all sorts of drugs. 02:16:28.920 |
And it's tough because how do you know which is which? 02:16:51.120 |
of the different diseases that they're learning about? 02:16:56.400 |
you start to think of the zebras more than the horses 02:17:02.520 |
You know, like, you know what I'm referring to, right? 02:17:05.540 |
You know, you see footprints, you see hoof prints, 02:17:11.620 |
There are some really funny things in medical school. 02:17:16.840 |
that you have a very warped sense of their prevalence. 02:17:22.840 |
Like I feel like we never stop talking about sarcoidosis. 02:17:26.280 |
I've seen like three cases in my life, right? 02:17:31.960 |
- Does it provide a great teaching tool or something? 02:17:34.860 |
Like I just, some of these things I don't know. 02:17:38.280 |
How much time did we spend talking about situs inversus? 02:17:40.860 |
This is when people embryologically have a reversed rotation 02:18:00.540 |
- I was thinking about boxing and the liver shot. 02:18:04.620 |
- No, I swear to God, like as a medical student, 02:18:06.920 |
if you were told someone had left-sided lower quadrant pain 02:18:20.880 |
in the top 10 things that it could possibly be. 02:18:24.000 |
But yes, you just have a totally warped sense 02:18:34.180 |
but for me, this is among the things that I just delight in 02:18:38.680 |
and even more so because you're the one across the table 02:18:42.240 |
for me teaching me about these incredible findings 02:18:48.360 |
because they're equally important to know about. 02:18:54.840 |
- Very well, and bring a little bit of that dew 02:18:59.180 |
- Yeah, I'll bring a low, medium, and high amount. 02:19:00.260 |
- Low, medium, and high amount, I want to know. 02:19:07.180 |
for today's Journal Club discussion with Dr. Peter Attia. 02:19:10.220 |
If you're learning from and/or enjoying this podcast, 02:19:14.420 |
That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us. 02:19:24.520 |
If you have questions for me or comments about the podcast 02:19:26.840 |
or guests that you'd like me to consider hosting 02:19:30.480 |
please put those in the comment section on YouTube. 02:19:36.400 |
at the beginning and throughout today's episode. 02:19:42.900 |
but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast, 02:19:46.880 |
While supplements aren't necessary for everybody, 02:19:49.020 |
many people derive tremendous benefit from them 02:19:57.560 |
If you'd like to access the supplements discussed 02:20:08.560 |
Again, that's Live Momentous, spelled O-U-S.com/huberman. 02:20:25.320 |
describing, for instance, tools to improve sleep, 02:20:29.940 |
We talk about deliberate cold exposure, fitness, 02:20:35.720 |
And to sign up, you simply go to hubermanlab.com, 02:20:40.240 |
scroll down to newsletter, and provide your email. 02:20:44.800 |
If you're not already following me on social media, 02:20:49.120 |
So that's Instagram, Twitter, Threads, LinkedIn, 02:20:54.680 |
I talk about science and science-related tools, 02:20:57.720 |
with the content of the Huberman Lab Podcast, 02:21:00.680 |
from the content of the Huberman Lab Podcast. 02:21:02.760 |
Again, it's Huberman Lab on all social media platforms. 02:21:07.240 |
for today's Journal Club discussion with Dr. Peter Attia.