back to indexHow Meditation Works & Science-Based Effective Meditations | Huberman Lab Podcast #96
Chapters
0:0 Meditation
4:13 InsideTracker, Thesis, ROKA, Momentous Supplements
8:25 Brief History of Meditation: Consciousness, Psychedelics, fMRI
16:19 How the Brain Interprets the Body & Surrounding Environment; Mindfulness
26:7 Neuroscience of Meditation; Perceptual Spotlights
32:27 AG1 (Athletic Greens)
33:41 Interoception vs. Exteroception
42:20 Default Mode Network, Continuum of Interoception & Exteroception
53:30 Tools: Interoceptive or Exteroceptive Bias, Meditation Challenge
61:48 State & Trait Changes, Interoceptive & Exteroceptive Meditations, Refocusing
67:35 Tool: Brief Meditations, Waking Up App
70:30 “Third Eye Center” & Wandering Thoughts
80:46 Meditation: Practice Types, Focal Points & Consistency
84:10 Breathwork: Cyclic Hyperventilation, Box Breathing & Interoception
90:41 Tool: Meditation Breathwork, Cyclic vs. Complex Breathwork
99:22 Interoception vs. Dissociation, Trauma
107:43 Model of Interoception & Dissociation Continuum
113:39 Meditation & Dissociation: Mood, Bias & Corresponding Challenge
120:18 Meditation & Sleep: Yoga Nidra, Non-Sleep Deep Rest (NSDR)
131:33 Choosing a Meditative Practice; Hypnosis
134:53 Tool: Space-Time Bridging (STB)
145:0 Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Social Media
00:00:02.260 |
where we discuss science and science-based tools 00:00:10.060 |
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology 00:00:17.300 |
We are going to discuss the science of meditation, 00:00:23.460 |
and we will talk about the science of meditation 00:00:25.540 |
as it relates to how the brain and body change 00:00:29.800 |
That is, what you export or take from a meditation practice 00:00:33.420 |
that can impact everything from your sleep to your mood. 00:00:40.940 |
and we will also talk about how meditation can be used 00:00:45.980 |
that are useful for work and other aspects of life. 00:00:49.000 |
Now, of course, most of you have probably heard 00:00:50.420 |
of meditation, and when we think of meditation, 00:00:52.680 |
most often we think of somebody either sitting 00:01:02.580 |
crossed in our lap or something of that sort. 00:01:04.900 |
Typically, we think of somebody who's in a very calm state, 00:01:07.580 |
eyes closed, focused on their so-called third eye center. 00:01:11.080 |
The third eye center is the area just behind one's forehead. 00:01:14.180 |
There's no third eye there, at least there shouldn't be, 00:01:17.960 |
but I'll tell you why it's called the third eye center 00:01:22.100 |
and why it's relevant, actually, for a meditative practice. 00:01:25.180 |
With all that said, it turns out that meditation encompasses 00:01:30.360 |
Some of those practices indeed are done sitting 00:01:35.920 |
Other of those practices are focused on a body scan, 00:01:39.100 |
you know, really focusing on one area of the body 00:01:41.340 |
and its contact with whatever surface you happen 00:01:43.460 |
to be sitting or lying on, or can be done walking. 00:01:47.020 |
In fact, there are walking meditations done with eyes open. 00:01:50.200 |
So there are many different forms of meditation, 00:01:55.740 |
and specific areas of the brain that are activated 00:01:58.700 |
during those meditations change our way of being 00:02:01.660 |
in fundamental ways, not just during the meditation practice 00:02:08.460 |
in changing your default state of mood or of thinking 00:02:11.940 |
or enhancing your ability to focus or improving your sleep 00:02:17.400 |
or physical endeavor, meditation is powerful, 00:02:23.520 |
So we will talk about picking a meditation practice 00:02:26.420 |
that isn't just feasible because you'll do it, 00:02:28.620 |
but is actually directed at the goals specific to you 00:02:33.760 |
So to give you some sense of the contour of today's episode, 00:02:36.480 |
first I'm going to talk about some of the underlying biology, 00:02:41.600 |
and also the areas of the body that are activated 00:02:46.880 |
and equally important, which areas of the brain and body 00:02:57.100 |
how to get the most out of that meditation practice. 00:03:01.180 |
how to change or alter your meditation practices 00:03:07.480 |
And this can get a little bit counterintuitive, 00:03:17.580 |
Sort of like if you get better at running endurance races 00:03:20.020 |
that you need to keep running longer and longer, 00:03:21.940 |
first a 5K, then a 10K, then a marathon, then ultras. 00:03:25.500 |
With meditation is actually quite the opposite. 00:03:32.180 |
and the more your so-called traits of brain state shift, 00:03:35.660 |
not just states as they're sometimes referred to, 00:03:37.820 |
but traits, this is a theme that I've picked up 00:03:39.720 |
from a terrific book that I'll refer to later. 00:03:50.240 |
in order to derive the benefits of meditation. 00:03:53.060 |
So that's a wonderful aspect of meditative practices 00:03:55.460 |
that's unlike a lot of other forms of mental exercise 00:04:02.780 |
And I promise that by the end of today's episode, 00:04:05.140 |
you will have a rich array of meditative practices 00:04:09.880 |
and why they can be directed toward particular goals 00:04:18.800 |
or where you're suffering from lack of sleep. 00:04:20.900 |
I think a lot of people will be excited to know 00:04:28.240 |
and still allow you to enhance your cognitive 00:04:35.420 |
from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. 00:04:46.260 |
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. 00:04:51.220 |
InsideTracker is a personalized nutrition platform 00:05:02.160 |
for the simple reason that many of the factors 00:05:04.420 |
that impact your immediate and long-term health 00:05:06.540 |
can only be analyzed from a quality blood test. 00:05:11.900 |
is that you get information back about hormones, 00:05:15.500 |
but you don't know what to do with that information. 00:05:17.220 |
InsideTracker makes understanding all of that very easy 00:05:19.700 |
and even better points to specific directives, 00:05:22.640 |
that is, things you can do in terms of your lifestyle, 00:05:26.980 |
in order to bring those numbers related to metabolic factors, 00:05:46.340 |
Today's episode is also brought to us by Thesis. 00:05:50.840 |
And to be honest, I am not a fan of the word nootropics. 00:05:53.620 |
I've said this many times before on this podcast 00:05:56.540 |
And the reason I don't like the word nootropics 00:06:07.900 |
or our ability to switch tasks or creativity, et cetera. 00:06:14.580 |
And so Thesis understands this, and as a consequence, 00:06:18.980 |
that are designed to bring your brain and body 00:06:20.920 |
into specific states to become, for instance, more focused 00:06:26.180 |
or to have more energy for workouts and things of that sort. 00:06:29.500 |
At Thesis, you can go there, you take a brief quiz, 00:06:33.580 |
of four different custom nootropics that you can try 00:06:37.780 |
so that you can develop a kit of custom nootropics 00:06:42.180 |
perhaps one for creativity, another one for motivation, 00:06:44.620 |
another one for focus, and another one for energy. 00:06:48.400 |
To get your own personalized nootropic starter kit, 00:06:50.340 |
you can go online to takethesis.com/huberman. 00:06:59.400 |
Today's episode is also brought to us by Roca. 00:07:05.940 |
The company was founded by two All-American swimmers 00:07:07.900 |
from Stanford, and everything about Roca eyeglasses 00:07:10.420 |
and sunglasses were designed with performance in mind. 00:07:13.340 |
Now, I've spent a lifetime working on the visual system, 00:07:15.160 |
and I can tell you that your visual system has to contend 00:07:20.740 |
Roca understands this and has designed their sunglasses 00:07:26.080 |
and for you to still be able to see with crystal clarity. 00:07:28.940 |
They were designed for things like cycling and running, 00:07:31.260 |
so they won't slip off your face if they get sweaty. 00:07:34.180 |
In fact, most of the time, I can't even remember 00:07:41.860 |
Unlike a lot of other performance eyeglasses out there, 00:07:48.680 |
Some people like those, but they also make versions 00:07:51.180 |
of their eyeglasses and sunglasses with frames 00:07:53.340 |
that you can wear out to dinner, to work, et cetera. 00:07:55.980 |
If you'd like to try Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses, 00:08:20.620 |
of those supplements is constantly expanding. 00:08:27.280 |
As I mentioned earlier, we are going to talk about 00:08:39.460 |
This is a topic I've long been interested in. 00:08:47.120 |
it was a bit of a wild one early in my high school years. 00:08:50.080 |
And as a consequence of a program that I was in, 00:08:58.280 |
That book is called "Wherever You Go, There You Are" 00:09:04.280 |
but one of the first people to really start popularizing 00:09:06.980 |
meditation mindfulness practices in the United States. 00:09:15.980 |
that there were very few studies of meditation, 00:09:20.920 |
Now you can find many, many thousands of studies 00:09:26.460 |
of brain imaging studies, changes in hormones in the body. 00:09:29.380 |
But in the late 1980s and in the early 1990s, 00:09:34.680 |
so-called MRI or FMRI was really just starting to emerge 00:09:38.800 |
as a popular tool in laboratories and hospitals, 00:09:41.640 |
there really wasn't that much mechanistic understanding 00:09:45.220 |
But of course, there was a deep understanding 00:09:53.440 |
as long as we're talking about the history of meditation, 00:09:58.080 |
is going to be a discussion about states of mind. 00:10:04.220 |
a kind of a dangerous topic to get into in any format, 00:10:07.900 |
because a lot of people talk about consciousness, 00:10:15.140 |
It doesn't have one standard operational definition, 00:10:28.860 |
And so in the 1960s, and especially in the 1970s, 00:10:31.980 |
meditation and psychedelics were actually close cousins 00:10:35.880 |
in the conversation about consciousness and states of mind. 00:10:45.580 |
It gets to a little bit of interesting academic sociology, 00:10:48.540 |
but what happened was there were a couple of guys at Harvard 00:10:56.540 |
in particular, LSD, lysergic acid diethylamide. 00:11:05.380 |
and they were really encouraging students at Harvard 00:11:10.600 |
They were also very interested in meditation, 00:11:12.780 |
but what ended up happening is they essentially got kicked 00:11:19.660 |
if you're interested in learning more about all this, 00:11:24.740 |
Nowadays, there's a lot of interest in psychedelics. 00:11:31.300 |
who's running clinical trials on psychedelics 00:11:37.020 |
We've also had Dr. Nolan Williams on the podcast, 00:11:39.700 |
my colleague at Stanford, who's doing incredible studies 00:11:44.060 |
So nowadays, the conversation about psychedelics 00:11:53.460 |
the conversation about psychedelics and meditation 00:11:58.580 |
That changed in the late 1980s and early 1990s 00:12:02.740 |
when people like Jon Kabat-Zinn started writing books 00:12:07.220 |
and suggesting that people explore meditative practices 00:12:10.020 |
for the utility to bring calmness, adjust stress, 00:12:14.860 |
divorced from the conversation about psychedelics. 00:12:17.460 |
Now, that's not to say that the scientific community 00:12:19.620 |
immediately embraced the conversation about meditation. 00:12:28.780 |
to start embracing and funding studies of meditation, 00:12:33.020 |
asking what sorts of brain areas are involved, 00:12:37.900 |
how a meditation practice can shift the brain and body 00:12:42.560 |
and is off in their life doing their everyday things. 00:12:45.700 |
In the late 1980s and especially within the 1990s, 00:12:56.060 |
was a way to look at the brain while it was active, 00:13:04.500 |
When all of that technology became accessible and popular, 00:13:07.580 |
well, that allowed a large number of laboratories 00:13:25.980 |
have them meditate and then look at how the brain changed 00:13:32.080 |
what was discovered was really quite miraculous, really. 00:13:37.640 |
but what was discovered was a huge laundry list 00:13:41.820 |
And then when people were evaluated in their outside life, 00:13:52.380 |
like changes in hormones or markers of inflammation, et cetera, 00:13:56.260 |
a large list of information fell out of that, 00:14:07.600 |
And some of those meditation practices could be quite short. 00:14:14.180 |
And in fact, that has a lot to do with the fact 00:14:16.780 |
that many of the major tech companies in the Bay Area 00:14:23.200 |
and any number of different social media companies 00:14:25.640 |
and other companies and business ventures, et cetera, 00:14:35.560 |
So nowadays we think of meditation as this thing 00:14:37.820 |
that almost everybody understands can benefit us, 00:14:44.240 |
where most people think of meditation as one thing, 00:14:51.180 |
It could mean high intensity interval training, 00:14:53.380 |
all of which, as you know, will get you different results 00:14:56.900 |
depending on what you do, how often you do it, 00:15:01.740 |
So too, meditation can give you very specific results. 00:15:10.100 |
just like exercise can, depending on the exercise. 00:15:14.580 |
is the specific changes that happen in the brain 00:15:19.640 |
That is, what happens when you close your eyes? 00:15:22.180 |
What happens when you focus your attention inward 00:15:27.940 |
there's third eye meditation where you close your eyes 00:15:29.820 |
and focus on that spot just behind your forehead, 00:15:40.140 |
not letting your mind wander or think about yesterday 00:15:47.220 |
There are also meditation practices, of course, 00:15:49.420 |
where you are in a format of interpersonal communication, 00:15:52.720 |
where you're really listening very intensely. 00:15:56.620 |
So we're going to parse each of these things, 00:16:02.960 |
so that you can develop specific meditation practices 00:16:05.860 |
that you can invoke in your real life on a daily basis, 00:16:09.260 |
or thankfully, I would say for some who are pretty busy, 00:16:13.120 |
that you could even do once a week or even once a month 00:16:15.900 |
that will still clearly benefit you in specific ways. 00:16:21.500 |
talking about the neuroscience of meditation. 00:16:24.100 |
I promise you, I'm not going to just list off 00:16:32.860 |
without also giving some mechanistic explanation 00:16:39.660 |
knowing what different brain areas do in their names 00:16:41.760 |
if I can't actually manipulate those brain areas? 00:16:43.940 |
But the good news is you actually can manipulate 00:16:48.180 |
you can turn up the activity in certain brain areas 00:16:51.280 |
and turn down the activity in specific brain areas 00:16:54.020 |
with specific elements of a meditation practice. 00:16:56.840 |
So that's quite exciting and quite different really 00:17:07.220 |
And again, the names themselves aren't essential, 00:17:14.540 |
to parse and use the information that follows. 00:17:23.240 |
Basically, it's the front bumper of your head 00:17:30.680 |
actually encompasses a lot of different things. 00:17:36.440 |
and you have one on the left side of your brain 00:17:41.400 |
The area that I'd like to focus on today for a bit 00:17:48.780 |
we'd say the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. 00:17:54.420 |
So if you want to touch the left side of your head 00:17:58.820 |
toward the sort of top of your head a little bit, 00:18:03.020 |
As long as your hand is still on the side of your head, 00:18:04.580 |
you're in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, okay? 00:18:12.500 |
That area of the brain we know from lesion studies 00:18:15.240 |
where it's been damaged in animals or humans, 00:18:19.880 |
where it's been selectively stimulated in animals, 00:18:23.080 |
or yes, indeed, also it's been done in humans, 00:18:26.780 |
has an incredible ability to control your bodily senses 00:18:31.440 |
and to make sense, that is to interpret what's going on 00:18:35.300 |
in terms of your emotions and your bodily sensations. 00:18:47.060 |
just for sake of simplicity and ease of communication. 00:18:51.260 |
of prefrontal cortex, I'll talk about another area. 00:18:54.940 |
what I mean is left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. 00:18:57.920 |
Stimulation of left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, 00:19:02.480 |
when your left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is active, 00:19:09.940 |
to interpret what's going on with you emotionally, 00:19:13.220 |
to interpret your bodily signals of comfort or discomfort, 00:19:22.260 |
And that's because the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 00:19:25.980 |
is in direct communication with and is directly connected 00:19:31.620 |
called the anterior cingulate cortex, or ACC. 00:19:35.020 |
Now I'm just going to refer to it as the ACC, okay? 00:19:41.300 |
that is interpreting a lot of different things 00:19:47.280 |
whether or not your heart is beating quickly or slowly, 00:19:52.980 |
whether or not your heart is beating quickly or slowly 00:20:04.060 |
it's unlikely that you are going to be concerned 00:20:08.500 |
because that is appropriate for the circumstance. 00:20:12.660 |
and all of a sudden your heart starts beating very quickly 00:20:23.060 |
inappropriate for the context that you happen to be in. 00:20:30.040 |
is the area of the brain that actually has some control over 00:20:38.660 |
Now, most of you probably haven't heard of the ACC. 00:20:41.140 |
Most of you probably have heard of a brain area 00:20:46.760 |
People talk about it as the fear center, et cetera. 00:20:55.340 |
but it also gets input from an enormous number 00:21:03.900 |
So it gets information about how full that is distended 00:21:09.680 |
It gets information about how quickly you're breathing 00:21:11.980 |
from input from your lungs and related structures. 00:21:18.980 |
for making sense of what's going on in your body. 00:21:21.220 |
And it works very closely along with one other structure. 00:21:24.340 |
And I promise it's going to be the third structure 00:21:26.500 |
in this triad, and then I'll stop listing off names. 00:21:33.860 |
You have the ACC, or anterior cingulate cortex, 00:21:44.100 |
If you have any pain or an itch or a mosquito bite 00:21:52.400 |
which is called the insula, I-N-S-U-L-A, insula. 00:21:55.780 |
And the insula has a bunch of different parts to it, 00:21:58.100 |
but the insula is another area that is interpreting signals 00:22:05.180 |
So the ACC and the insula are working together 00:22:07.300 |
to try and figure out what's going on inside me. 00:22:10.260 |
And in addition to that, the insula is interpreting 00:22:13.240 |
information about what's going on outside of you. 00:22:18.820 |
hey, this is a steep hill that I'm running up. 00:22:20.900 |
And as a consequence, whatever heart rate increase 00:22:25.540 |
or burning in my lungs, this all makes sense. 00:22:27.860 |
I don't have to be worried, I don't have to be scared. 00:22:29.620 |
I might want to slow down, but this makes sense. 00:22:32.100 |
Whereas it, for instance, in the example I previously gave, 00:22:36.420 |
and everything is pretty calm, and all of a sudden, 00:22:43.220 |
or you start having a so-called anxiety or panic attack, 00:22:51.340 |
or doesn't correspond to something in the outside world. 00:22:55.900 |
which includes the left or lateral prefrontal cortex, 00:22:58.760 |
the cingulate or anterior cingulate cortex, and the insula. 00:23:05.180 |
It's a neural conversation, but a conversation nonetheless, 00:23:08.100 |
trying to figure out, okay, what's going on inside me? 00:23:21.140 |
that you're experiencing, meaning how quick your breathing is 00:23:29.020 |
any sensations of pain or pleasure for that matter, 00:23:32.600 |
whether or not that makes sense for the situation you're in 00:23:44.180 |
of these different neural structures in the brain, 00:23:50.600 |
that there's a conversation that's constantly occurring 00:23:55.720 |
trying to figure out what's going on inside of you, 00:24:00.340 |
relative to what's going on outside and around you. 00:24:04.900 |
That is, we are to some extent conscious of the fact 00:24:11.800 |
awareness of the present and anticipation of the future. 00:24:17.300 |
that we can be seated at the dinner table, excuse me, 00:24:37.320 |
or maybe we are very excited about the next day 00:24:43.820 |
because we do have access to this knowledge about self 00:24:55.840 |
because what it means is that we can be doing something, 00:25:04.420 |
and our bodily state may or may not match what we are doing 00:25:16.420 |
Now, a major emphasis of a meditation practice 00:25:25.660 |
universally accepted operational definition of mindfulness. 00:25:30.940 |
people can't agree exactly what mindfulness should be, 00:25:40.540 |
that mindfulness includes something about being present, 00:25:45.900 |
that doesn't necessarily mean present to one's surroundings 00:25:48.620 |
because, of course, a lot of meditation practices 00:25:50.760 |
that are designed to make us more mindful and present 00:25:53.160 |
are designed to make us more mindful and present 00:25:56.500 |
while ignoring everything that's happening externally, 00:25:59.940 |
but they are designed to make us more present 00:26:04.500 |
our breathing and our thoughts in the moment. 00:26:09.420 |
what a generic meditation practice looks like, 00:26:11.580 |
and let's evaluate how that tends to change the activity 00:26:15.060 |
of these neural circuits in the brain and body, 00:26:16.900 |
and then from there, we can split the conversation 00:26:25.660 |
meditation practices that are ideal for improving mood, 00:26:28.860 |
meditation practices that are ideal for improving sleep, 00:26:32.480 |
and meditation practices that, believe it or not, 00:26:36.020 |
benefit all of those things in one fell swoop. 00:26:39.060 |
Okay, so what happens during a meditation practice 00:26:45.900 |
That means you and I are going to be scientists now. 00:26:58.500 |
that occur with those different component parts. 00:27:01.520 |
let's use a somewhat generic form of meditation, 00:27:12.260 |
a meditation practice is going to involve stopping, 00:27:15.560 |
meaning getting out of motion, sitting or lying down, 00:27:28.640 |
There are many forms of meditation that are done eyes open, 00:27:35.660 |
that is not ambulating, not walking or running, 00:27:42.940 |
meaning when we sit or lie down and close our eyes, 00:27:51.260 |
in the way that your brain and other neural circuits 00:27:53.880 |
in your body function for the following reason. 00:28:00.060 |
we shut down a major avenue of what's called exteroception. 00:28:13.240 |
We are also sensing things from outside of us all the time, 00:28:15.900 |
so these could be sights or sounds, touch on our body, 00:28:21.420 |
Now, sensation is distinct from what we call perception. 00:28:28.640 |
the sensations that we happen to be paying attention to. 00:28:32.080 |
So at any given moment, you are sensing many, many things. 00:28:36.460 |
there are pressure receptors on the bottoms of your feet 00:28:38.860 |
sensing your shoes or your sandals or the floor, et cetera, 00:28:48.580 |
is that you have so-called spotlights of attention. 00:29:04.500 |
so for instance, you could focus all of your perception 00:29:09.740 |
and really pour all of your awareness, your attention, 00:29:15.260 |
what it feels like if there's tingling or pressure, 00:29:19.940 |
Or you can broaden that spotlight to include both feet 00:29:24.620 |
and then your legs and your whole body or the entire room. 00:29:27.860 |
Perception is like a spotlight, and I should mention, 00:29:30.760 |
there are very good data that we can split our attention 00:29:33.620 |
into two but probably not more than two spotlights, 00:29:37.520 |
and we can make those spotlights of perception 00:29:40.420 |
either very broad and diffuse or very narrow. 00:29:45.000 |
You can pick a spot on the wall away from you anywhere, 00:29:48.580 |
or if you're driving, you can look at some location, 00:29:50.480 |
and you can focus intensely on one small location, 00:29:57.480 |
or any number of different things outside of you, 00:30:04.300 |
You can also focus a spotlight of perception on your body, 00:30:24.620 |
but most people can split to two points of attention 00:30:30.220 |
The other thing that most people can do pretty easily 00:30:35.120 |
or rather to have just one spotlight of attention. 00:30:43.080 |
but you could, for instance, have two points of attention. 00:30:45.980 |
So you're talking to somebody and you're paying attention 00:30:47.600 |
to whether or not somebody's walking in the door or not, 00:30:49.960 |
so that's two, or you could be completely focused 00:30:53.420 |
or you could be completely focused on the stomachache 00:30:55.720 |
or the great sensation of hunger that you have in your belly 00:31:02.240 |
Okay, so you have two spotlights of perception. 00:31:09.480 |
Those spotlights of perception can intensify or dim, 00:31:14.720 |
What I mean by that is your perception of what's happening 00:31:17.660 |
within those spotlights can be very, very high acuity. 00:31:21.880 |
That is, you can register very fine changes in detail, 00:31:34.460 |
but the point is that you can consciously adjust the acuity 00:31:43.560 |
because of the incredible ability of a brain structure 00:31:48.940 |
which is the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, 00:31:56.920 |
to specific things in your environment or within your body, 00:32:00.480 |
or to split those points of attention or merge them, 00:32:18.160 |
All of that is under control because of your ability 00:32:20.880 |
to engage this area that we call the prefrontal cortex 00:32:28.960 |
and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens. 00:32:36.700 |
that covers all of your foundational nutritional needs. 00:32:42.280 |
so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. 00:32:57.440 |
that communicate with the brain, the immune system, 00:32:59.200 |
and basically all the biological systems of our body 00:33:01.600 |
to strongly impact our immediate and long-term health. 00:33:12.120 |
contains a number of adaptogens, vitamins, and minerals 00:33:16.280 |
nutritional needs are met and it tastes great. 00:33:26.560 |
that make it really easy to mix up Athletic Greens 00:33:31.460 |
And they'll give you a year's supply of vitamin D3K2. 00:33:41.960 |
Okay, so now if we look at the example of what happens 00:33:49.200 |
you should immediately realize that that's a tremendous shift 00:33:58.200 |
while it can be oriented toward, for instance, 00:34:09.560 |
we shut down one of the major avenues for sensory input, 00:34:16.680 |
there's a tendency for those perceptual spotlights 00:34:28.440 |
And that informs us about something very important, 00:34:36.840 |
Up until now, I've been talking about perception 00:34:40.060 |
and indeed they are, at least for sake of this conversation. 00:34:45.360 |
or within that word, attention, there's a continuum. 00:35:03.960 |
Some people can sense their heart beating pretty easily. 00:35:08.500 |
What we are feeling on the surface of our skin, 00:35:11.680 |
how hot or cold we feel, that's interoception. 00:35:15.100 |
In contrast, at the other end of the continuum 00:35:18.440 |
is so-called exteroception, spelled with an E. 00:35:24.760 |
that's outside or beyond the confines of our skin. 00:35:37.960 |
which not so incidentally is the prefrontal cortex, 00:35:48.760 |
or the extension of their belly while they breathe. 00:36:05.540 |
with how they are feeling from the skin inward 00:36:14.060 |
about to order a sandwich, and you're reading the menu, 00:36:17.360 |
The menu is outside the confines of your skin, 00:36:19.560 |
and little ideas or maybe big ideas come to mind 00:36:28.820 |
what you like, what you don't like, et cetera. 00:36:31.060 |
That's splitting interoception and exteroception. 00:36:34.560 |
But when we close our eyes, we stop, we slow down, 00:36:37.120 |
we focus on our breathing or that third eye center. 00:36:40.280 |
The majority of our perception then shifts to interoception. 00:36:53.920 |
the ACC, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the insula, 00:36:57.440 |
really ramp up their levels of neural activity. 00:37:16.400 |
and closing your eyes, your brain undergoes a massive shift 00:37:22.540 |
Now, that's not to say you can't be distracted 00:37:24.280 |
by external events, and in fact, many people are, 00:37:33.680 |
to one end of the continuum, 'cause there's no down up, 00:37:43.920 |
but many people are very interoceptively aware, 00:37:48.920 |
just naturally, even if they don't do a meditation practice. 00:37:52.360 |
Other people are not, and there's a pretty good measure 00:38:00.040 |
and that is your ability to count your heartbeats 00:38:08.480 |
You can actually try and estimate your number of heartbeats 00:38:28.640 |
heightened levels of interoceptive awareness, 00:38:33.640 |
oh, but to be really in touch with your body, 00:38:49.120 |
of any subtle shift in their heart rate or breathing 00:38:52.000 |
or change in the sensations within their stomach, 00:38:57.000 |
of their bodily state, that can be beneficial, right? 00:39:00.000 |
It can be adaptive or not, depending on the circumstances. 00:39:02.960 |
It's probably not adaptive to be very, very aware 00:39:08.240 |
If, for instance, you're doing public speaking, 00:39:09.920 |
you don't want to be thinking about what's going on 00:39:11.160 |
in your stomach or how quickly you're breathing. 00:39:12.920 |
I'm certainly trying to ignore all those signals, 00:39:17.320 |
but for somebody who has no awareness of what's going on, 00:39:26.500 |
who can ignore the fact that they're having a heart attack 00:39:28.460 |
or can ignore the fact that they have high blood pressure 00:39:31.380 |
and are caring about life focused on everything external 00:39:38.680 |
So we want to be very careful about placing valence, 00:39:45.240 |
on interoceptive awareness versus exteroceptive awareness. 00:39:52.600 |
if it's of the sort where you stop your movement 00:39:56.300 |
you are training for interoceptive awareness. 00:40:08.400 |
in fact, I would say some people ought to opt 00:40:14.800 |
actually a meditation like a walking meditation 00:40:28.380 |
who develop such a heightened state or awareness 00:40:35.400 |
for so aware of their breathing and of their heart 00:40:40.180 |
that it actually is intrusive for daily activities. 00:40:43.180 |
So I will ask you to ask this question of yourself now. 00:40:47.240 |
Are you somebody who tends to be very in touch 00:40:54.380 |
or are you somebody who tends to be less in touch with 00:41:08.120 |
And I think most people will answer that it depends. 00:41:11.240 |
It depends on whether or not you are in a social setting 00:41:16.940 |
So keep it in mind because it will become very beneficial 00:41:20.280 |
in building an optimal meditation practice for you. 00:41:23.640 |
But for now, just know there's this continuum of perception 00:41:30.160 |
opening your eyes dramatically increases exteroception 00:41:34.920 |
just automatically because so much of your brain, 00:41:40.440 |
for those of you that are low vision or no vision, 00:41:42.320 |
and those of you that are blind or have poor vision, 00:41:45.000 |
this entire process is translated to the auditory, 00:41:55.920 |
closing the eyes doesn't have this huge shift 00:42:12.020 |
or they put headphones on or noise canceling headphones, 00:42:14.440 |
then the world inside of them becomes very prominent 00:42:17.900 |
relative to the world outside of them for obvious reasons. 00:42:23.180 |
who tends to be more interoceptively aware or not, 00:42:29.520 |
And some of you might not be able to answer that question. 00:42:36.720 |
depending on the activities that you're doing. 00:42:40.480 |
where if somebody comes over to you and starts talking to you 00:42:47.180 |
that you're thinking about your heart beating 00:42:48.920 |
and whether or not you're flushing red, et cetera, 00:42:50.560 |
you're going to pay attention to what they say. 00:42:52.320 |
Many people, however, when somebody talks to them, 00:43:02.480 |
or whether or not they look right or sound right 00:43:04.880 |
or whether or not they have something in their teeth. 00:43:12.040 |
towards interoceptive awareness or exteroceptive awareness. 00:43:16.360 |
It will depend on whether or not you're out on a date 00:43:18.700 |
with somebody that you would loathe to find out later 00:43:23.720 |
or whether or not you're with somebody you're more familiar 00:43:27.520 |
or the other person would tell you this kind of thing. 00:43:39.280 |
We know that if you are more interoceptively aware 00:43:42.460 |
your insulin, ACC are active, but that's not very useful. 00:43:45.100 |
That's not helpful as a tool, that's just a fact. 00:43:55.960 |
from where you naturally sit in order to help you function 00:44:00.480 |
not just during the meditation, but at all times. 00:44:09.040 |
It's a very cool study, has a very cool name. 00:44:13.620 |
that will come up again and again in today's conversation. 00:44:15.720 |
That's something called the default mode network. 00:44:19.960 |
of different brain areas that essentially are active 00:44:24.520 |
And certainly is active when we are not focused 00:44:28.140 |
on one particular task or conversation or activity. 00:44:31.680 |
The default mode network can be thought of more or less 00:44:43.440 |
your perceptual spotlight can either be two spotlights 00:44:47.760 |
Well, similarly, human beings can think about the past, 00:44:50.980 |
surely, the present, definitely, and the future. 00:44:55.200 |
And it turns out we can also split our thoughts 00:45:06.500 |
I can split my thinking and my memory in that way. 00:45:09.220 |
I can also think about the present and the future. 00:45:11.120 |
I can also think about the future and the past, 00:45:14.140 |
although it's very difficult, although not impossible, 00:45:17.700 |
to split one's thinking and memory into the past, 00:45:22.580 |
Not easily done, but pretty easy to split one's attention 00:45:28.880 |
either the past, the present, and the future, 00:45:35.880 |
you can place your mind, your thinking, and your memory, 00:45:41.580 |
that'd be very, very present or the past and the present 00:45:48.520 |
while it involves a lot of different brain areas, 00:45:50.580 |
can be thought of simply as the network of brain areas 00:46:00.740 |
as I mentioned before, is now a classic paper, 00:46:12.580 |
but that's actually the title of the scientific paper, 00:46:21.180 |
but it's especially competitive to get manuscripts accepted 00:46:24.900 |
into science, into nature, and into the journal cell. 00:46:28.020 |
So it represents kind of one of the Super Bowl, 00:46:31.220 |
NBA championships, and Stanley Cup, if you will, 00:46:34.300 |
for you sports aficionados of scientific publishing. 00:46:36.640 |
This is a paper from Matthew Killingsworth and Dan Gilbert. 00:46:43.060 |
And this paper, "A Wandering Mind is an Unhappy Mind," 00:46:47.100 |
I'm going to paraphrase certain elements of it for you, 00:46:49.620 |
because they say essentially what I would like you to know, 00:46:54.920 |
So first of all, they start out with a statement, 00:47:05.560 |
"contemplating events that happened in the past, 00:47:07.640 |
"might happen in the future, or will never happen at all." 00:47:10.800 |
I agree with their assertion that human beings do that. 00:47:16.500 |
I don't think there's any evidence whatsoever 00:47:23.840 |
but I'd be happy to go toe to toe with you on that. 00:47:25.560 |
I am not aware of any data that prove one way or the other 00:47:39.040 |
which is that humans have this wandering of the mind 00:47:44.400 |
That is, there's nothing happening to create these thoughts 00:47:47.220 |
or anything happening in the immediate environment. 00:47:49.720 |
These thoughts are just happening on their own internally. 00:47:59.260 |
that smartphones became widely available and in use. 00:48:06.740 |
They contacted people on their iPhones many times per day. 00:48:11.880 |
And they did this for well over 2,200 adults. 00:48:15.960 |
They had a mix of male and female people in this study. 00:48:21.240 |
but there was a mean, of course, mean average, 00:48:23.640 |
but there were a range of different ages and so forth. 00:48:29.920 |
And they also asked them, "What are you doing right now?" 00:48:32.540 |
So they were looking for the match or mismatch 00:48:34.600 |
between what people were doing and what they were feeling. 00:48:41.360 |
And they came up with a kind of a bubble chart, if you will, 00:48:47.320 |
the more answers came back about one particular thing. 00:48:50.420 |
And they assessed whether or not people were happy or not 00:48:59.440 |
But the important points that came from the data, 00:49:14.340 |
people were generally thinking about something else, 00:49:16.680 |
except it turns out there's just one little bubble 00:49:21.540 |
People claimed, and I'm inclined to believe them, 00:49:24.780 |
that they tend to be very focused on making love 00:49:31.040 |
Now, why their iPhone was there with them at that moment, 00:49:34.300 |
That wasn't included in this description of the study. 00:49:37.380 |
But all the other activities, grooming and self-care, 00:49:47.700 |
people claimed that their mind wandered a lot. 00:49:49.900 |
And then they also assessed, of course, their mood 00:49:52.340 |
and how those people felt at any given moment, 00:50:06.860 |
when their minds were wandering than when they were not. 00:50:12.420 |
And then third, what people were thinking at a given moment 00:50:16.940 |
was far better a predictor of their happiness 00:50:22.900 |
and I think matches a lot of people's experience. 00:50:25.460 |
In fact, I think as you hear about this study, 00:50:27.460 |
many of you will probably just say, well, duh. 00:50:29.860 |
I mean, if you're working and you don't like your work 00:50:32.060 |
and you're thinking about something bad that happened, 00:50:34.200 |
well, then of course you're not going to be happy. 00:50:42.660 |
that people were thinking about something unpleasant. 00:50:49.460 |
that was pleasant, that also made them feel unhappy. 00:50:53.660 |
In other words, the mismatch between being in an activity 00:50:57.300 |
and having our mind elsewhere led people to report 00:51:00.980 |
themselves as feeling more unhappy in that moment. 00:51:04.620 |
what you find is that people are often not present 00:51:10.760 |
even if their thoughts are those of happy, joyful thoughts. 00:51:16.920 |
and I think runs counter to what most of us have heard 00:51:20.580 |
or have been taught, which is think good thoughts, 00:51:24.660 |
have a good internal landscape, create a good narrative. 00:51:37.980 |
That is the strongest predictor of being happy. 00:51:49.580 |
in conclusion, a human mind is a wandering mind, 00:51:55.620 |
The ability to think about what is not happening 00:52:00.620 |
is a cognitive achievement that comes at an emotional cost. 00:52:04.900 |
So I know I'm not alone in believing that this paper, 00:52:21.940 |
along that interoceptive, exteroceptive continuum 00:52:26.180 |
to what you happen to be experiencing in that moment. 00:52:30.540 |
And while most people think of a meditative practice 00:52:38.520 |
for any number of minutes, or maybe even an hour or longer, 00:52:46.460 |
in which you are actively focusing on things outside 00:52:50.380 |
or beyond the confines of your skin and internal landscape. 00:52:57.180 |
And if we are to take the work of Killingsworth and Gilbert, 00:53:00.700 |
this wandering mind is an unhappy mind, seriously. 00:53:03.860 |
And I know a number of other laboratories have 00:53:05.860 |
and have supported this research with their findings 00:53:19.260 |
being present to what we are doing in a given moment, 00:53:22.940 |
is one of the essential keys to happiness and improved mood, 00:53:31.820 |
and it's a tool that any and all of us can use, 00:53:33.860 |
whether or not you tend to be interoceptively dominant, 00:53:38.600 |
to your bodily sensations, or exteroceptively dominant. 00:53:42.120 |
And again, if you don't know the answer to that question, 00:53:46.140 |
You can just sit down or lie down, close your eyes, 00:53:58.820 |
Cars honking or going by, people in the room, 00:54:02.100 |
or whether or not you tend to be able to focus 00:54:03.940 |
on your internal landscape to the exclusion of exteroception 00:54:10.100 |
Now, of course, this will depend on context and situation, 00:54:19.180 |
every time you decide to do a meditation practice. 00:54:26.300 |
to determine what meditation you do at any given moment. 00:54:30.100 |
So let's say you are somebody who is a regular meditator, 00:54:32.620 |
or let's say you're somebody who's never meditated 00:54:34.620 |
and you'd like to develop a meditation practice. 00:54:37.220 |
I suggest that you do a test of whether or not 00:54:42.900 |
or exteroceptively dominant in that moment, okay? 00:54:48.460 |
This is a question about where you happen to be in a moment. 00:54:51.260 |
So let's say you're on a plane or you're in the car. 00:54:53.740 |
If you're in the car, please don't close your eyes 00:54:59.460 |
But stop, close your eyes, and assess whether or not 00:55:09.740 |
or whether or not your attention and perception 00:55:12.160 |
gets pulled to something external, to exteroception. 00:55:15.900 |
And again, that will vary depending on circumstance 00:55:28.760 |
you can divorce your perception from sensations 00:55:32.620 |
that occur at the level of your skin or internally. 00:55:35.620 |
Now, I should say that there's no technology, 00:55:48.280 |
there's no technology that can tell you, for instance, 00:55:51.420 |
whether or not you are interoceptively dominant 00:56:04.520 |
and you notice that you can equally split your attention 00:56:06.600 |
between internal sensations and external sensations, 00:56:18.200 |
well, that will dictate the sort of meditation 00:56:20.160 |
that you perhaps ought to perform in that moment. 00:56:23.300 |
Let me give an example of how you would do this. 00:56:30.180 |
and evaluate whether or not you can essentially rule out 00:56:35.100 |
or eliminate attention to all outside events. 00:56:38.500 |
Most people won't be able to do that entirely, 00:56:40.500 |
but try and focus your attention, for instance, 00:56:42.120 |
on your breathing or the typical third eye center, 00:56:44.080 |
you know, focusing at a spot right behind your forehead. 00:56:48.180 |
to the exclusion of what's happening around you, 00:57:04.420 |
like a tree or maybe even an object or a plant 00:57:08.860 |
or something else in your immediate environment, 00:57:31.880 |
or we could even say force yourself a little bit 00:57:35.780 |
to either inside your body or outside your body, 00:57:43.780 |
well, then you are actively training up the neural circuits. 00:57:50.640 |
the brain's ability to change in response to experience. 00:57:52.900 |
You are deliberately engaging a shift along that continuum. 00:57:57.900 |
To make this crystal clear, what I mean is this. 00:58:01.760 |
If I were to sit down and I want to do some meditation, 00:58:06.600 |
there's good evidence that even three minutes of meditation 00:58:10.900 |
including enhanced focus and enhanced anxiety management. 00:58:16.500 |
that I can really focus inward on what's happening 00:58:19.940 |
at the level of my skin and my internal organs, 00:58:26.340 |
or maybe it's just because my brain is in a state 00:58:28.920 |
that I'm particularly good at that at that moment, 00:58:33.900 |
Well, then I would opt for a three-minute meditation practice 00:58:44.380 |
because I want, and I think most people would like, 00:58:52.460 |
and they don't default to whatever happens to be easiest 00:58:59.060 |
and try and focus on what's going on internally, 00:59:01.060 |
and I kept getting distracted by things happening 00:59:04.540 |
or feeling like I need to reach for my phone, 00:59:06.240 |
or paying attention to the sounds in the room, 00:59:08.300 |
well, then I would actively engage a meditation practice, 00:59:11.180 |
in this case, a three-minute example, but it could be longer, 00:59:14.040 |
where I'm deliberately trying to focus my perception 00:59:17.780 |
on events at the level of the confines of my skin 00:59:22.600 |
Well, I love to use the phrase anytime with kids 00:59:36.220 |
if you can perform any activity or thought, et cetera, 00:59:43.620 |
It's the friction, it's the feeling that something is hard 00:59:46.340 |
that turns on the enormous variety of mechanisms 00:59:51.160 |
that allow you to potentially change your neural circuitry. 00:59:58.200 |
to your brain and body that something needs to change. 01:00:01.020 |
So I'm encouraging you to embark on meditative practices 01:00:17.700 |
For some of you, this will change across the day 01:00:19.980 |
where early in the day, you are very, very good 01:00:28.420 |
I actually believe based on the data that I've covered 01:00:30.560 |
and we'll get into a few more papers about this, 01:00:33.060 |
and my lab is actively working on this as well, 01:00:35.580 |
that a meditative practice can be made far more effective. 01:00:44.060 |
more shift in brain states and brain circuitry 01:00:54.740 |
of what our brain would naturally do in a given moment. 01:00:59.220 |
and you're finding that everything's very distracting, 01:01:09.060 |
you're looping thoughts about the past and present, 01:01:13.100 |
well, that would be a terrific time, an ideal time really, 01:01:22.800 |
on interoceptive bias or extra receptive bias, 01:01:27.020 |
you are going against, or I should say you're pushing back 01:01:32.300 |
I would argue it's going to be far more effective. 01:01:35.260 |
That is, you're going to reduce or shift the activity 01:01:46.180 |
toward being more interoceptive or extra receptive. 01:01:50.940 |
both for the immediate changes that you experience, 01:01:57.280 |
And it also can lead to, as we referred to earlier, 01:02:00.820 |
more neuroplasticity, more changes in the brain circuits 01:02:08.380 |
And I want to be very clear that I am not the first 01:02:17.100 |
In fact, I can't recommend this book highly enough. 01:02:24.940 |
This is a book by Daniel Goleman and Richard Davidson. 01:02:28.000 |
They've done a terrific work in many writings 01:02:30.180 |
and many TED Talks, et cetera, about meditation. 01:02:36.340 |
this book really captured what I believe to be 01:02:39.620 |
the most essential elements of the science of meditation 01:02:44.620 |
Today, we are focusing on much of what's covered 01:02:50.780 |
In fact, most of the papers that I'm going to talk about 01:03:01.080 |
trait changes being the more long lasting ones. 01:03:03.840 |
My read of this book and the literature that follows 01:03:07.320 |
is, again, that when you sit down to meditate, 01:03:13.200 |
to do that interoceptive, exteroceptive bias assessment. 01:03:16.740 |
Ask yourself whether or not you are more in your head 01:03:23.260 |
that runs counter to where you happen to be at. 01:03:30.500 |
And if you're more focused on what's going on around you, 01:03:36.440 |
with how to do an interoceptive biased meditation. 01:03:43.320 |
focus on that third eye center behind your forehead 01:03:46.120 |
or focus on your breathing or your bodily sensations. 01:03:58.620 |
So that could be, for instance, a point on the wall. 01:04:06.360 |
What you will find is that your visual system 01:04:10.280 |
when you concentrate your visual focus at that location. 01:04:12.720 |
I want to remind you that it is perfectly okay 01:04:19.960 |
There is no rule that says that you can't do those things. 01:04:22.600 |
This is not just beaming a particular location in space 01:04:27.580 |
I've been accused many times of not blinking very often. 01:04:35.440 |
So I'm accessing from a sort of an internal image in my head. 01:04:42.580 |
if you're going to do an exteroceptive biased meditation, 01:04:47.180 |
why you wouldn't look away from that location 01:04:50.220 |
In the same way that if you're focused on internal thoughts 01:04:53.360 |
with your eyes closed and focused on your breathing, 01:04:56.520 |
your thoughts will skip away from that breathing 01:05:01.640 |
In fact, and this is discussed in the book "Altered Traits," 01:05:07.540 |
one of the key elements of any meditative practice, 01:05:17.600 |
The more number of times that you have to yank yourself 01:05:20.960 |
back into attending or perceiving one specific things. 01:05:24.080 |
In other words, the more times your mind wanders 01:05:27.500 |
actually the more effective that practice is. 01:05:32.080 |
with laser precision and your mind never darts away 01:05:34.320 |
from that and you don't have to bring it back, 01:05:37.440 |
Nothing needs to change because your nervous system 01:05:40.100 |
will effectively know it's performing perfectly. 01:05:42.300 |
So if you're somebody who tries to do meditation, 01:05:50.820 |
or focus back on your breath or your third eye center, 01:05:53.440 |
each one of those are just opportunities to do better. 01:05:56.740 |
They are essential to the improvement process. 01:06:00.260 |
Think about them as ascending a staircase of refocusing. 01:06:03.460 |
Every time you refocus, you're going up one more level, 01:06:10.980 |
from the kind of judgmental process of thinking, 01:06:17.280 |
that the refocusing process will happen so quickly 01:06:22.140 |
And again, this is something that's borne out 01:06:30.600 |
is refocusing more quickly and consistently over time. 01:06:35.960 |
in very experienced meditators that was done in Japan 01:06:46.020 |
with many hundreds, if not thousands of hours of meditation 01:06:56.860 |
could really focus, and they did this by brain imaging, 01:07:07.060 |
their mind is really going to something else. 01:07:14.280 |
But it turns out that the more modern neuroimaging studies 01:07:19.600 |
such that they're staying in a very narrow trench of focus. 01:07:24.100 |
and going back in more quickly, more quickly, more quickly, 01:07:27.480 |
So rather than think about your ability to focus, 01:07:32.040 |
And the more number of times you have to refocus, 01:07:35.720 |
So earlier I mentioned doing this interoceptive biased 01:07:38.120 |
or exteroceptive biased meditation for three minutes. 01:07:42.500 |
Well, three minutes seems like a reasonable number 01:07:44.220 |
for most people to do consistently, you know, once a day. 01:07:48.380 |
And in fact, there are some studies of one minute meditations 01:07:58.660 |
But I think it's also clear that by three minutes, 01:08:06.300 |
I'm not pointing to any one particular data point here. 01:08:12.980 |
to direct one's perception, that is your attention, 01:08:17.220 |
to your internal state or to something external to you 01:08:20.900 |
is immensely beneficial if you do it consistently 01:08:32.420 |
that is not the one that you would default to 01:08:36.100 |
And some people have taken this to the extreme to say that, 01:08:38.660 |
you know, you can even just move about your day 01:08:43.380 |
To be honest, when I look at the whole of the data, 01:08:53.180 |
Now, I'm a big fan of some of the newer meditation apps 01:09:06.060 |
at least every other day and often every day. 01:09:08.180 |
And he convinced me to check out the Waking Up app 01:09:22.980 |
They're not a sponsor of this podcast, I should mention, 01:09:33.940 |
and what a specific meditation can do for you 01:09:40.380 |
Some of them are a minute long, two minutes long, 01:09:45.100 |
That app I think includes a variety of meditations 01:09:48.080 |
that really encompasses the huge range of possibilities 01:09:53.700 |
and that at least by my experience of the Waking Up app 01:09:57.200 |
has led to my most consistent meditation practice. 01:10:00.820 |
And of course, I would love to get Sam on the podcast 01:10:03.100 |
as a guest so we could talk about the sort of underpinnings 01:10:12.940 |
some of the very deep and somewhat abstract discussions. 01:10:21.340 |
but I absolutely love the Waking Up app, Sam, 01:10:33.980 |
because it turns out to be pretty interesting. 01:10:38.260 |
that's been given to another neural structure, 01:10:41.360 |
or I should say structure because it's not strictly neural, 01:10:47.260 |
I promise I'm not taking off on a tangent here 01:10:55.760 |
you tend to have mirror symmetric representations 01:11:00.260 |
Well, you have a prefrontal cortex on the right, 01:11:03.060 |
and they actually do slightly different things. 01:11:04.780 |
Language is sometimes lateralized to one side, 01:11:17.300 |
The pineal gland is the gland that makes melatonin, 01:11:20.800 |
which at night when it gets dark secretes melatonin, 01:11:26.840 |
it helps you fall asleep but not stay asleep. 01:11:30.820 |
asserted that the pineal was the seat of the soul 01:11:33.400 |
because it was the one structure in the brain 01:11:36.100 |
that he saw was not on both sides of the brain. 01:11:41.780 |
Now, I don't know if it's the seat of the soul or not. 01:11:43.960 |
I'm not in a position to make assessments like that, 01:12:08.260 |
or, believe it or not, two holes in the top of their skull 01:12:14.540 |
light can go directly into their brain through these holes 01:12:18.260 |
and activate the pineal to suppress melatonin 01:12:21.460 |
and control their wakefulness, sleep rhythms. 01:12:24.940 |
In birds, they don't have holes in their skull, 01:12:33.740 |
and communicates information about time of day 01:12:42.380 |
And so the pineal has been called the third eye 01:12:44.980 |
because it's a light-sensitive organ inside the brain. 01:12:48.260 |
In humans, the pineal sits deep, deep, deep to the surface, 01:13:01.220 |
or you're having neurosurgery or something of that sort, 01:13:10.860 |
and it absolutely should not see light directly. 01:13:14.760 |
So the idea that the pineal is the third eye in humans 01:13:20.580 |
So anytime someone says, "Oh, the pineal is your third eye," 01:13:25.360 |
that people are referring to when they talk about meditation. 01:13:28.420 |
Now, you'll see a number of different forms of art 01:13:32.520 |
where somebody will, it will be a picture of a face 01:13:36.760 |
or sometimes open, there'll be literally a third eye, 01:13:38.580 |
like a cyclops eye in the middle of the forehead. 01:13:41.520 |
That has been proposed for many thousands of years 01:13:47.980 |
Now, that's interesting because that real estate 01:13:54.120 |
which we know from lesion studies and stimulation studies. 01:14:04.660 |
In fact, and this is kind of an eerie result, 01:14:07.660 |
but if you inactivate, you turn off the prefrontal cortex 01:14:26.620 |
but their ability to distinguish between enemy and friend 01:14:45.020 |
This is also true for people that have prefrontal damage. 01:14:49.920 |
or a hard time suppressing behaviors, et cetera. 01:14:52.400 |
So the third eye center as the seat of consciousness 01:14:55.840 |
and our intention is something that makes sense generally 01:15:00.240 |
with what we know about the neuroscience and neurology, 01:15:04.000 |
that I think is especially important for all of you 01:15:06.200 |
that goes beyond anything about ancient traditions 01:15:10.160 |
and pits in the top of the head, and here's what it is. 01:15:22.280 |
Well, if I touch the top of my hand, I can feel that. 01:15:33.960 |
at the level of my stomach, is it full, is it empty, 01:15:38.320 |
or does it feel pleasant, et cetera, I can sense that. 01:15:41.680 |
And that's because we have sensory neurons on our skin 01:15:51.180 |
This is one of the reasons why you can remove the skull 01:15:54.160 |
and do brain surgery on somebody who's wide awake 01:15:57.500 |
and they don't need any anesthetic on the brain itself. 01:16:09.360 |
So normally we are perceiving and paying attention 01:16:13.180 |
to what we are sensing, either externally, sights and sounds, 01:16:20.700 |
But by focusing our perception and our attention, 01:16:32.360 |
we essentially are bringing that attentional, 01:16:36.120 |
to a location in which there is no sensation. 01:16:46.560 |
which is the prefrontal cortex, to be quite honest, 01:16:54.200 |
And what happens is when we are not thinking about 01:16:57.460 |
and perceiving our sensations, because there are none there, 01:17:00.760 |
our thoughts and our emotions and our memories 01:17:06.500 |
A better way to put it would be that they geyser up 01:17:09.380 |
and take on more prominence in our perception. 01:17:19.780 |
but as I'm speaking, I'm in contact with the chair 01:17:24.480 |
But if I focus my energy and attention on them, 01:17:30.440 |
Similarly, I'm thinking things all the time, you are too, 01:17:36.420 |
and I'm anticipating things all the time about the future. 01:17:39.240 |
But by focusing my attention on the one organ 01:17:42.760 |
for which I have no sensation, that is my brain, 01:18:03.600 |
if it's a meditative practice where you close your eyes 01:18:31.840 |
That's less thinking than it is perceiving senses, okay? 01:18:44.360 |
to the very area of your brain that directs attention, 01:18:49.460 |
The only things that will become present to you 01:18:52.200 |
are feelings, emotions, that is, thoughts, and memories. 01:19:00.340 |
in what seems to be a very disorganized fashion. 01:19:02.740 |
And the reason they arrive in somewhat disorganized fashion 01:19:06.760 |
is because normally we just don't perceive things that way. 01:19:14.240 |
to multiple things, our sensation and our thoughts. 01:19:17.420 |
When we put all of our perception into our thoughts, 01:19:20.080 |
we see how disorganized, how wandering they are, 01:19:23.320 |
and how, in fact, how random and intrusive those can be. 01:19:29.360 |
And much of what we talked about in that paper earlier, 01:19:42.120 |
They're not really present to what they're doing, 01:19:44.480 |
which leads me to the statement that I believe, 01:19:46.920 |
at least based on the data, that paper included, 01:19:53.680 |
They're focused more on what's going on internally 01:19:55.960 |
than they are focused on what's happening externally. 01:19:58.560 |
There are certainly people who, for the opposite, is true, 01:20:07.720 |
to do a meditation practice that allows us to focus inward 01:20:12.840 |
by all the stressors of life, et cetera, et cetera. 01:20:18.660 |
But as we do that, we tend to be very focused 01:20:27.160 |
can enhance one's level of presence and happiness, 01:20:32.880 |
that being mindful and aware of what's happening, 01:20:38.440 |
that includes what other people are saying and doing, 01:20:49.240 |
We've talked a little bit about the brain networks 01:20:53.680 |
which include prefrontal cortex, ACC, the insula. 01:21:06.960 |
that you can be right in the middle of that continuum. 01:21:11.140 |
and find that you are smack dab in the middle 01:21:13.440 |
of being able to attend to things outside of you, 01:21:20.880 |
that is either exteroceptive biased or interoceptive biased. 01:21:26.240 |
if you find that you are more, quote unquote, 01:21:29.840 |
well then focus on an exteroceptive biased meditation 01:21:35.960 |
whereas if you are more exteroceptively focused 01:21:38.600 |
at any given moment, well then I encourage you 01:21:40.680 |
to do an interoceptively focused meditation practice. 01:21:45.500 |
there's this issue of how long to do a practice. 01:21:49.640 |
but some of the practices we've covered on this podcast 01:21:57.160 |
that Dr. Wendy Suzuki from New York University's laboratory 01:22:05.760 |
which shows that a daily 13-minute meditation, 01:22:17.060 |
that meditation done daily for about eight weeks, 01:22:19.980 |
maybe shorter, but in that study, eight weeks, 01:22:22.720 |
greatly improved mood, improved ability to sleep, 01:22:25.500 |
improved cognitive ability and focus, memory. 01:22:28.160 |
A huge number of metrics were looked at very specifically. 01:22:31.720 |
So that's a terrific one, and you may be asking yourself, 01:22:35.420 |
Could you get away with five minutes or three minutes? 01:22:39.420 |
in stress reduction, improvement in sleep, et cetera 01:22:45.080 |
However, in trying to establish how long you should meditate, 01:22:53.120 |
And by consistently, that doesn't necessarily mean every day. 01:22:56.720 |
If you answer the question about consistency honestly, 01:23:04.020 |
well, then I would encourage you to go a little bit longer, 01:23:06.020 |
maybe 10 or 15 minutes, maybe even 30 minutes. 01:23:08.480 |
Again, understanding that you're going to have 01:23:09.880 |
to refocus repeatedly throughout that meditation, 01:23:14.220 |
on internal perceptions or external perceptions. 01:23:20.160 |
or 15 minutes per day, and you can meditate every day, 01:23:23.560 |
well, then I think you have a bit more flexibility 01:23:27.560 |
Maybe it's three minutes one day, one minute the next day, 01:23:32.480 |
Just like with exercise, the key component is consistency. 01:23:39.540 |
It's also borne out in all the recent studies 01:23:42.880 |
Since that book was published, consistency is key. 01:23:45.500 |
So ask yourself what you can do consistently, 01:23:54.560 |
So earlier, we decided we were going to parse 01:24:01.000 |
We've talked about interoceptive versus exteroceptive bias, 01:24:06.200 |
where you place your perception or your focus. 01:24:11.880 |
is the pattern of breathing that you embrace. 01:24:14.340 |
In fact, the pattern of breathing that you embrace 01:24:23.600 |
Well, these days, we hear a lot about breath work. 01:24:31.540 |
First of all, I think we need to credit Wim Hof, 01:24:46.160 |
that Wim Hof started to grow in recognition and popularity 01:24:52.200 |
which in the laboratory we call cyclic hyperventilation. 01:24:58.420 |
He named it, or people named it after him, Wim Hof. 01:25:04.640 |
is a Dutchman who is known to hold many world records 01:25:17.020 |
but who's also expert in the use of breathing 01:25:24.840 |
about different patterns of breath work, in particular, 01:25:29.880 |
deep, deliberate breathing, so big inhales, exhales, 01:25:39.540 |
It's very clear from studies both done on Wim specifically, 01:25:46.500 |
that that pattern of cyclic hyperventilation, 01:25:48.640 |
of deliberately breathing deeply and repetitively, 01:25:51.480 |
typically in through the nose, out through the mouth, 01:25:55.440 |
or causes adrenaline release from the brain and body. 01:26:03.040 |
but the liberation of adrenaline does a number of things 01:26:08.520 |
That more or less is what Wim Hof breathing is, 01:26:16.120 |
is not a pattern of breathing typical of most meditations 01:26:23.320 |
Now, that's not to say that cyclic hyperventilation 01:26:25.640 |
can't be incorporated into a meditation practice, 01:26:32.080 |
is typically considered its own practice, okay? 01:26:35.600 |
Its own breath work practice divorced from meditation. 01:26:49.400 |
and this could be in the form of cyclic breathing 01:26:52.520 |
of inhale, exhale, inhale, exhale, which is cyclic, 01:26:56.120 |
or in some cases, doubling up on inhales and then exhaling. 01:26:59.620 |
So inhale, inhale, exhale, inhale, inhale, exhale, 01:27:02.340 |
or controlling the duration of inhale, breath, hold, 01:27:06.180 |
exhale, breath, hold, repeat, so-called box breathing, 01:27:09.480 |
where the inhale, the hold, the exhale, and the hold 01:27:23.240 |
holding for two seconds, and seven seconds out. 01:27:25.740 |
Regardless of what cadence of breathing one uses, 01:27:29.300 |
there is a tendency during most meditative practices 01:27:32.820 |
to slow one's breathing and/or control one's breathing 01:27:38.440 |
This is essential because when we default our breathing, 01:27:44.840 |
to how long we are inhaling relative to our exhales, 01:27:56.440 |
Normally, when we're not thinking about breathing, 01:28:00.640 |
There's a motor command that's sent to inflate the lungs, 01:28:05.160 |
but in many breath work practices or meditation practices, 01:28:11.800 |
Well, when we do that, a number of things happen. 01:28:14.000 |
First of all, it forces us into interoception. 01:28:18.720 |
Because it's the diaphragm, the muscle that helps 01:28:27.040 |
or depth of breathing, as one would with box breathing 01:28:37.580 |
more often than not, we aren't focused on the actual air 01:28:40.520 |
leaving our nasal passages or mouth, maybe a little bit, 01:28:45.980 |
or we just default to focusing on the movement 01:28:52.280 |
All of that is to say that by deliberately focusing 01:29:01.020 |
So breathing and specific patterns of breathing 01:29:12.000 |
we shift to interoception and away from external events. 01:29:15.940 |
Doesn't mean we can't still pay attention to external events. 01:29:18.800 |
We can still exterocept, but at least some portion 01:29:22.000 |
of our perception of our attention shifts to interoception. 01:29:26.520 |
So we, of course, need to breathe to stay alive. 01:29:33.420 |
So of course, breathing is part of any meditative practice, 01:29:36.660 |
just like it's part of any living activity, even sleep, 01:29:44.080 |
is to direct our perception in a deliberate way, 01:29:47.820 |
using that prefrontal cortex to a specific location, 01:30:01.380 |
of a meditative practice is the pattern of breathing. 01:30:14.360 |
That is, should we be controlling the depth and the cadence? 01:30:20.900 |
about the capacity for specific patterns of breathing 01:30:30.960 |
And that is true regardless of whether or not 01:30:36.280 |
within our body or exteroceptive perceptions. 01:30:44.800 |
Well, there's again, no simple one size fits all rule there, 01:30:49.800 |
but there are some general rules of respiration physiology 01:30:53.600 |
that can help us access and develop a meditation practice 01:30:59.580 |
And since this is not an episode all about respiration, 01:31:18.200 |
or if you're going to do your meditation walking, 01:31:21.800 |
when you are about to begin your meditative practice, 01:31:27.080 |
Do you want to be more relaxed than you are at present? 01:31:33.320 |
Or do you want to be more alert than you are at present 01:31:42.220 |
Simple question, you can decide from session to session, 01:31:48.280 |
but just as you need to assess whether or not 01:31:50.180 |
you are leaning more interceptively or exteroceptively, 01:31:56.500 |
do you need to calm down or want to calm down? 01:32:01.680 |
Or maybe you want to go into a state of deep relaxation 01:32:08.620 |
using breath work and specific patterns of breathing. 01:32:12.080 |
And here is the general rule that is supported 01:32:15.060 |
by all the respiration physiology that I'm aware of. 01:32:25.180 |
we will do an episode all about respiration physiology 01:32:44.540 |
This is simply based on the way that the neural circuits, 01:32:47.880 |
like the pre-Botzinger nucleus and the parafacial nucleus, 01:32:50.180 |
they govern respiration physiology and alertness. 01:32:55.440 |
that release noradrenaline, norepinephrine, et cetera. 01:32:58.300 |
In contrast, if you emphasize longer duration 01:33:05.220 |
and/or more vigorous exhales relative to your inhales, 01:33:14.840 |
Now, you might be saying, okay, I understand what it is 01:33:28.540 |
So an example of inhale-biased breath work would be, 01:33:38.800 |
and it's a little bit longer than the exhale, 01:33:45.460 |
then you want to extend your exhales relative to your inhales 01:34:08.300 |
because those are two sides of the same seesaw 01:34:13.860 |
at the end of your meditation as where you started, 01:34:16.340 |
at least in terms of levels of alertness and calmness, 01:34:18.860 |
well, then you would just keep your inhales and your exhales 01:34:24.760 |
Now, the introduction of things like breath holds 01:34:33.060 |
deep inhale exhales, and then exhale all your air, 01:34:35.740 |
hold your breath for 15 to 60 seconds and then repeat 01:34:47.580 |
if you are going to do a complicated breathing practice, 01:34:54.620 |
shift much of your attention to the breathing practice, 01:35:01.020 |
Cyclic breathing is where inhales always follow exhales, 01:35:04.620 |
It actually relies on a specific brain center 01:35:12.160 |
However, if you are doubling up on your inhale, 01:35:18.180 |
a pattern of breathing my laboratory has studied extensively, 01:35:22.940 |
well, then that relies on a different brain center, 01:35:30.780 |
or you are deliberately emphasizing inhales or exhales 01:35:35.300 |
or the vigor of inhales and exhales, et cetera, 01:35:38.300 |
well, then some portion of your attention will be devoted 01:35:41.620 |
to making sure that you follow that breathing practice. 01:35:43.960 |
We are very good at going into cyclic breathing practices 01:35:47.420 |
by default and our attention can drift to other things, 01:35:50.500 |
interoceptive or exteroceptive, doesn't matter. 01:35:56.440 |
or something we see or hear in the room, et cetera. 01:36:00.780 |
and the breathing pattern is non-cyclic or complex 01:36:09.260 |
again, from those so-called top-down mechanisms 01:36:20.660 |
to be devoted to the breathing practice itself. 01:36:40.140 |
the less you will be able to focus on other things. 01:36:46.020 |
So for instance, if you're somebody who's very much caught 01:36:48.860 |
in your own head, right, we talked about this earlier, 01:36:55.300 |
well, then that meditation practice that you do 01:36:56.980 |
really should be focused on exteroceptive bias. 01:36:59.320 |
You should really focus on something external to you. 01:37:05.440 |
where inhales follow exhales follow inhales follow exhales. 01:37:08.820 |
If, however, you are finding that you're sort of caught 01:37:12.020 |
in the landscape of things happening around you 01:37:14.340 |
and you want to ground yourself as it's sometimes called, 01:37:17.020 |
that's a loose language, not a scientific language. 01:37:20.900 |
and that's a whole thing people always writing to me 01:37:22.580 |
is grounding a real thing, walking barefoot on the earth 01:37:25.320 |
and magnetic fields and gravitational fields. 01:37:30.740 |
there isn't a lot of science for it, to be frank. 01:37:33.420 |
Does feel nice to walk on the ground, however. 01:37:35.720 |
But if you are somebody who's kind of feeling pulled out 01:37:41.420 |
and you want to bring your awareness into your body 01:37:44.860 |
and sort of calm down, well, then I would encourage you 01:37:48.200 |
to, yes, use a deliberate, somewhat unnatural 01:38:03.220 |
that this has been discussed in detail such as this before. 01:38:09.400 |
My laboratory has been working on this extensively. 01:38:11.140 |
I'm always looking for new colleagues and collaborators. 01:38:17.760 |
who's been a guest on the Huberman Lab Podcast 01:38:22.860 |
In fact, he's our associate chair of psychiatry, 01:38:27.140 |
We have an active research program focused on these issues. 01:38:32.140 |
that a breath work practice itself can be meditative. 01:38:47.940 |
that an interoceptive biased breath work practice 01:38:53.120 |
which is to make you more interoceptively aware. 01:38:56.060 |
And if you think back to earlier in the episode, 01:38:58.500 |
for many people, that will be a wonderful thing 01:39:03.340 |
or ought to seek because it can help people gain awareness. 01:39:09.240 |
and they're not realizing it till the end of the day, 01:39:12.220 |
more interoceptive awareness throughout the day 01:39:16.060 |
If, however, you are somebody who is overly focused 01:39:20.740 |
well, then more exteroceptive awareness is important. 01:39:29.120 |
what particular types of meditative practices 01:39:39.500 |
or sleep-based challenges or focus-based challenges. 01:39:43.140 |
I haven't listed off all the positive benefits of meditation 01:39:47.700 |
yet in this episode, but they are many, many, many. 01:40:01.320 |
There are known benefits of a regular meditation practice 01:40:04.900 |
There are known benefits of a regular meditation practice 01:40:09.940 |
even improving outcomes in cancer, reducing pain, 01:40:13.420 |
improving mood, reducing the symptoms of ADHD 01:40:17.060 |
and clinically diagnosed ADHD and on and on and on. 01:40:25.600 |
that some meditation practice done regularly, 01:40:29.880 |
has tremendous, even outsized benefits on our health, 01:40:32.760 |
even relative to some drug treatments that's been shown, 01:40:37.420 |
I've been more focused on what sorts of brain 01:40:40.180 |
and body changes occur when we do a meditation practice, 01:40:43.800 |
what really constitutes a meditation practice. 01:40:46.620 |
We have this thing about a continuum of perception. 01:40:51.540 |
Well, there's another component that I'd like to raise now, 01:40:54.620 |
which we could say is the third major component. 01:40:56.460 |
The first one that I raised was interoceptive 01:41:04.140 |
Is it going to be default or deliberate breathing? 01:41:07.100 |
Is it going to be natural cadence or unnatural cadence? 01:41:17.340 |
that hasn't really been formalized in the literature, 01:41:19.260 |
but that Dr. Spiegel and I are working hard to formalize 01:41:22.260 |
through some research and through an upcoming review 01:41:35.140 |
So now all of you know what interoception is, 01:41:56.140 |
in dissociation or disassociation, and guess what? 01:42:05.660 |
Some people will say disassociation, like I disassociate. 01:42:12.300 |
Both of those refer to essentially the same thing. 01:42:32.000 |
people report feeling out of body or out of the experience, 01:42:43.580 |
in terms of people who are in a traumatic accident 01:42:46.940 |
or they see someone killed right in front of them. 01:42:49.220 |
First responders will talk about dissociating 01:42:52.440 |
I don't want to provide gruesome imagery here 01:42:54.360 |
'cause I know people can be pretty sensitive to this, 01:42:59.540 |
and just seeing carnage or incredible damage to bodies 01:43:06.000 |
Dissociation lies at the opposite end of a continuum 01:43:12.440 |
is on the opposite end of a continuum with exteroception, 01:43:15.220 |
but it also is on the opposite end of a continuum 01:43:20.100 |
We can provide some better definitions perhaps 01:43:24.020 |
And here I'm actually reading from an upcoming review. 01:43:29.080 |
but nonetheless, interoception refers to a process 01:43:33.180 |
meaning your brain and connections with your body, 01:43:34.840 |
senses, interprets, integrates, and regulates signals 01:43:40.500 |
and thereby provides moment-to-moment mapping 01:43:49.000 |
basically the process of perceiving what's happening 01:43:51.040 |
at the level of the surface of your skin or inward. 01:44:13.200 |
but really, if we think about health and mental health 01:44:23.900 |
We don't want to be dissociated from life's experiences, 01:44:26.700 |
but we also don't want everything that happens in the world 01:44:30.460 |
to profoundly impact our heart rate and our breathing. 01:44:35.880 |
Okay, there are instances in which being yanked around 01:44:58.060 |
but being too dissociated or being too feeling, 01:45:04.980 |
to everything that happens is also problematic. 01:45:08.980 |
that have challenges with what's called narrative distancing. 01:45:13.060 |
That is, they see someone in a movie getting hit 01:45:15.120 |
and they almost flinch as if they are getting hit. 01:45:18.280 |
They see someone who's scared or happy in a movie 01:45:32.160 |
remember way back at the beginning of the episode, 01:45:34.040 |
that ACC, that anterior cingulate cortex and the insula? 01:45:45.080 |
that person in your environment who's breaking down crying, 01:45:48.400 |
yes, they're sad, it's important to be sympathetic, 01:45:56.680 |
And then, of course, there are areas of your brain 01:45:59.520 |
that are also leaning on, and here I'm using metaphor, 01:46:04.160 |
but they're leaning on the insula and ACC and saying, 01:46:07.640 |
"Hey, there's somebody that I care about that's upset. 01:46:13.700 |
Or, "They're scared, so I'm also going to be scared." 01:46:27.160 |
So why am I raising yet another continuum, right? 01:46:35.400 |
Well, if we want to think about how meditation 01:46:38.540 |
can serve our mental health and our ability to focus, 01:46:42.700 |
there's a very particular mental model that we can arrive at 01:46:47.980 |
that incorporates this interoceptive/dissociative continuum. 01:46:57.780 |
and those feelings in your body nearly completely account 01:47:09.660 |
but your bodily response to that is essentially shut down. 01:47:21.620 |
Again, sadly, this is often what victims of trauma report, 01:47:25.300 |
that they are able to just go through the motions 01:47:30.440 |
They aren't feeling the elevated heart rate or breathing. 01:47:47.220 |
and then a meditative practice that can be used 01:47:52.980 |
or the healthy location along that continuum. 01:47:55.220 |
Let's first imagine the ideal mental health state, 01:47:59.980 |
and here I want to acknowledge nobody achieves 01:48:03.500 |
or at least maintains this mental health state. 01:48:08.260 |
along this interoceptive to dissociative continuum 01:48:13.220 |
or you represent a sphere that can roll back and forth 01:48:20.380 |
At the other end, you're completely dissociated. 01:48:25.940 |
we take that continuum and we fold up the sides 01:48:35.980 |
I realize a number of people are listening to this, 01:48:43.540 |
my fingers of my hands are apart, so it looks like a V, 01:48:47.800 |
Your state is like a ball bearing at the base of that. 01:48:50.180 |
You are in a trench of perfectly balanced interoception 01:48:56.420 |
you can register what's going on in the outside world, 01:48:59.080 |
but your feelings are not overwhelmed or overtaken 01:49:10.940 |
Wouldn't that be lovely if we could be like that 01:49:13.600 |
And frankly, nobody is like that all the time. 01:49:22.980 |
of self versus others and internal versus external states 01:49:35.340 |
and your state is more or less like a ball bearing 01:49:56.160 |
your mind drifts a little bit while watching a movie 01:50:00.660 |
or while your child is complaining about something 01:50:13.500 |
would involve this kind of U-shaped model as well, 01:50:16.300 |
where it's kind of, can shift back and forth, 01:50:21.700 |
all the way to dissociated in any kind of extreme way. 01:50:24.960 |
The ball bearing stays down near the base of that U. 01:50:27.560 |
Then, of course, there are states that we all, frankly, 01:50:33.860 |
where the continuum of interoception and dissociation 01:50:38.480 |
where you are a ball bearing at one location or another, 01:50:41.420 |
depending on whether or not you're watching a movie 01:50:46.420 |
or in an activity with your partner or a friend, et cetera, 01:50:49.180 |
has you very engrossed, maybe matching their state, right? 01:50:53.280 |
where matching one state is actually healthy and good. 01:50:56.180 |
And then there are a number of conditions in life 01:51:00.700 |
like you're getting yelled at and they're angry, 01:51:05.020 |
you're not in the best place along that continuum. 01:51:09.940 |
they find themselves somewhere along that continuum, 01:51:12.640 |
and a number of practices, including meditation, 01:51:15.760 |
including exercise, including getting a good night's sleep, 01:51:28.780 |
of that flat continuum into more of a U or concave shape 01:51:32.620 |
so that that ball bearing, meaning your state of awareness 01:51:39.540 |
versus paying attention to what's going on around you, 01:51:41.940 |
is somewhere, again, biased toward the middle 01:51:44.700 |
by curling up the edges of that continuum on either end, 01:51:59.880 |
is no longer shaped like a deep trench, like a V. 01:52:05.880 |
It's not flat with the edges curled up a little bit 01:52:17.560 |
of pure interoception, just feeling beyond any ability 01:52:23.400 |
just feeling one's feelings, being angry, being sad, 01:52:26.680 |
being, or even happy, being so extremely happy or manic 01:52:34.840 |
inappropriate for what's going on around you, 01:52:36.400 |
or dropping to the other side of the continuum 01:52:39.000 |
where you're so dissociated that you're not engaged 01:52:45.080 |
That shape is one that I think almost all clinicians, 01:52:51.080 |
if not all clinicians, and most people would say 01:52:53.680 |
is pathologic because you are either completely checked out 01:52:57.940 |
or you are completely absorbed in what's going on 01:53:11.560 |
but it does incorporate a lot of what we think about 01:53:15.680 |
and we talk about the ability to be mentally stable, 01:53:18.600 |
to feel one's feelings, but to still be actively engaged 01:53:25.420 |
from interoceptive awareness to dissociation, 01:53:40.100 |
Well, we know for sure that being sleep-deprived, 01:53:44.100 |
for instance, tends to take us away from that trench shape 01:53:52.440 |
and starts to make that continuum more convex. 01:53:56.680 |
like we're completely checked out and exhausted 01:54:01.800 |
We are yanked around by whatever experience is happening. 01:54:08.720 |
the fundamental or foundational layer of mental health, 01:54:12.640 |
because it tends to put us in a healthier place. 01:54:15.260 |
That is, when we're getting enough quality sleep 01:54:24.480 |
And when I say pulls us apart, that's not a real term. 01:54:31.660 |
Less bowl-shaped and more convex, more hill-shaped, 01:54:41.480 |
In addition, a meditative practice done regularly 01:54:51.500 |
or it can allow us to become more exteroceptively aware, 01:54:55.420 |
which is really just another form of dissociation. 01:55:12.640 |
that hasn't been discussed a whole lot in the literature. 01:55:18.460 |
If you look in the clinical psychiatry literature, 01:55:21.200 |
there's a wonderful collection of studies and reviews 01:55:23.840 |
that will say that interoceptive awareness is terrific 01:55:30.140 |
that they are not able to engage in the world. 01:55:32.340 |
Similarly, you'll find a beautiful literature, 01:55:46.360 |
but because they can disengage or they're dissociated from it 01:55:52.380 |
or dissociation can be very adaptive and beneficial 01:55:59.980 |
so they're not getting pulled into every argument, 01:56:03.520 |
they don't necessarily think that it's their fault, 01:56:07.640 |
use their prefrontal cortex and say, "Hey, wait, 01:56:16.000 |
Okay, so in thinking about the positive effects 01:56:33.740 |
having your default mode network be one of mind wandering 01:56:37.580 |
actually is correlated with being more unhappy. 01:56:41.140 |
That was the earlier study that we talked about, 01:56:45.840 |
Now, of course, meditation can make us more present, 01:56:50.620 |
to whether or not we are becoming more present 01:56:59.220 |
and we don't pay attention to whether or not our bias 01:57:14.200 |
who has a tremendous amount of interoceptive awareness, 01:57:20.140 |
may not be good, and actually there's some evidence 01:57:26.020 |
I've talked about this previously in the podcast, 01:57:28.100 |
but in that very study from Wendy Suzuki's lab 01:57:34.740 |
it's also very clear that for a number of people 01:57:47.260 |
because they are becoming more interoceptively aware, 01:57:55.940 |
and falling asleep involves turning off your thoughts 01:57:58.820 |
and your focus, and focusing purely on sensation, 01:58:05.240 |
This is why I'm a big fan of using non-sleep deep rest 01:58:10.140 |
to non-sleep deep rest and yoga nidra protocols, 01:58:12.380 |
I've talked about them on the podcast before, 01:58:14.640 |
but those protocols are not meditation per se, 01:58:24.420 |
whereas meditation tends to be a focusing practice. 01:58:33.800 |
where you focus on things that are outside your body 01:58:38.720 |
who tends to focus too much on their inner landscape 01:58:42.380 |
can help get them out of their head and body, 01:58:46.740 |
but for people that are not in touch with their emotions, 01:58:50.300 |
it actually can drive them down the exact path 01:58:56.220 |
and we want to make sure that we are parsing meditation 01:58:58.700 |
in a rational way that matches the neural circuitry involved 01:59:03.680 |
and more importantly, for sake of practical purposes, 01:59:07.140 |
that you are asking yourselves the right question, 01:59:09.700 |
are you interoceptively or exteroceptively biased? 01:59:13.340 |
or do you tend to sort of feel everything in a big way? 01:59:18.340 |
I've heard this term of hypersensitive people 01:59:21.540 |
and some of those are clinical terms, some of them are not, 01:59:25.140 |
and you also need to assess where you happen to be at 01:59:27.260 |
on a given day, which will be dictated, of course, 01:59:29.820 |
by how well you slept, life experience, et cetera. 01:59:33.340 |
So this interoceptive to dissociative continuum 01:59:40.800 |
And again, the solution or the answer of what to do 01:59:47.740 |
of whether or not you are more inward focused 01:59:52.340 |
Just do the opposite of where your bias lies. 01:59:55.100 |
That is, if you're tilted towards interoception, 02:00:02.460 |
that sounds sort of pejorative, it sounds bad, right? 02:00:05.480 |
But again, if you are somebody who's more focused 02:00:09.200 |
and you want to gain more interoceptive awareness 02:00:16.400 |
that's third eye center practice or breathing focused. 02:00:21.600 |
is that they've heard before or they've experienced 02:00:36.780 |
And I came up with an answer that was, frankly, 02:00:45.860 |
in order to sleep better and maybe even reduce 02:00:51.040 |
something that I think most people would want. 02:00:55.900 |
probably should enjoy sleeping, I certainly do, 02:01:05.100 |
First of all, I want to point to the recent study. 02:01:09.180 |
And then again, this is one that I've raised a few times 02:01:13.980 |
Brief Daily Meditation Enhances Attention, Memory, 02:01:17.380 |
Mood, and Emotion Regulation in Non-Experienced Meditators. 02:01:29.020 |
at New York University and has run a laboratory 02:01:48.960 |
I should just mention that the control group in this study 02:02:03.220 |
which is not to say that podcasts aren't useful. 02:02:07.100 |
Unfortunately, it was not the Huberman Lab Podcast, 02:02:09.920 |
which I like to think at least increases understanding 02:02:12.420 |
of certain key concepts of science and science-based tools. 02:02:22.380 |
but it didn't change the brain in any fundamental way 02:02:32.680 |
is that they mentioned that if people in the experiment 02:02:40.640 |
they had trouble sleeping again, which makes sense 02:02:42.600 |
because meditation, at least in its most common form 02:02:56.460 |
that doing two 20 minute sessions per day of meditation 02:03:06.560 |
First of all, understanding what sleep need is 02:03:17.960 |
with six hours of sleep, but would do better with eight. 02:03:20.560 |
Some people would actually manage probably better 02:03:30.400 |
you can check out any one of three different episodes 02:03:40.560 |
And then, of course, we've done episodes on sleep 02:03:42.600 |
with expert guests like Dr. Matthew Walker from UC Berkeley. 02:03:45.800 |
All of those can be found at hubermanlab.com. 02:03:50.140 |
With that said, this assertion that has been made 02:03:53.580 |
many times over, and certainly in the popular press, 02:03:55.680 |
that regular meditation can reduce one's overall sleep need 02:04:02.480 |
Some groups find that indeed that is the case, 02:04:05.260 |
and the interpretation is that the stress reduction 02:04:08.280 |
that's brought about by regular meditative practice, 02:04:12.960 |
tends to be one or more typically two 20-minute per day 02:04:17.720 |
That's quite a lot, I think, for most people. 02:04:19.760 |
If you think about 40 minutes, isn't that much time overall, 02:04:23.520 |
but very few people will stick to that twice a day, 02:04:26.140 |
20-minute meditation practice very consistently. 02:04:35.540 |
brought about by that type of meditation practice, 02:04:38.800 |
is good at offsetting some of the cortisol increases 02:04:43.280 |
associated with reduced sleep and leading people 02:04:47.340 |
to be able to function cognitively and physically 02:04:53.140 |
had they not been doing the meditation practice. 02:04:59.180 |
if people meditate regularly, that's reducing stress. 02:05:01.720 |
The reduction in stress is reducing cortisol. 02:05:05.680 |
but it should be restricted to early part of the day. 02:05:17.580 |
against the unhealthy pattern of cortisol release. 02:05:25.200 |
and/or the total amount of sleep that they need is reduced. 02:05:31.420 |
and interpreted it as saying, well, if you can't sleep, 02:05:36.340 |
So one night you don't sleep or you have trouble sleeping, 02:05:38.540 |
you just meditate the next day and you'll be fine. 02:05:41.340 |
Well, certainly that is not supported by the literature. 02:05:46.020 |
it's one that I've talked about on this podcast 02:05:47.640 |
many times before, but if you haven't heard me talk about it, 02:05:53.040 |
It is a practice of doing not so much a focus meditation, 02:05:59.160 |
and actually trying to turn off that prefrontal cortex 02:06:04.260 |
Yoga nidra scripts can be found on YouTube and elsewhere. 02:06:15.860 |
You can just simply go to YouTube and put in NSDR 02:06:18.300 |
and my last name, Huberman, there's one there. 02:06:27.680 |
not as many as been done on traditional meditation, 02:06:30.260 |
or I should say third eye-centered meditation 02:06:38.620 |
and reduce cortisol, reduce a stress hormone, 02:06:41.680 |
at least as much, and by my read of the literature, 02:06:45.100 |
significantly more than with traditional meditation. 02:06:48.420 |
And there's a nice paper that we will provide a link to, 02:06:51.220 |
which is entitled "Yoga Nidra Practice Shows Improvement 02:06:58.020 |
Basically, this study looks at, as the title suggests, 02:07:07.020 |
or would carry over for people who don't have insomnia. 02:07:15.420 |
"Salivary cortisol reduced statistically significantly 02:07:20.660 |
There was a statistically significant reduction 02:07:29.520 |
that we believe would be paralleled by a very similar, 02:07:39.160 |
let's just call it the sort of mystical language 02:07:41.540 |
It focuses more on the physiology and the body scans. 02:07:44.440 |
I want to acknowledge that yoga nidra has been around 02:07:47.460 |
for thousands of years and was certainly there before NSDR. 02:07:52.300 |
and this was brought up also in altered traits, 02:07:59.260 |
In fact, this was recognized by Jon Kabat-Zinn 02:08:02.980 |
when he created what he called mindfulness-based 02:08:10.860 |
which was simply mindfulness meditation to reduce stress, 02:08:13.320 |
but he called it MBSR, mindfulness-based stress reduction, 02:08:26.800 |
to any specific utility of one practice versus another. 02:08:32.320 |
If you want to get better at falling and staying asleep 02:08:38.300 |
or if you are generally challenged with sleep issues, 02:08:45.120 |
meaning data that show that a stress hormone cortisol 02:08:51.060 |
as well as certain neurotransmitters can be replenished, 02:08:54.860 |
as well as, and this is key and covered in this paper 02:08:57.700 |
that I've mentioned a few moments ago on yoga nidra, 02:09:07.880 |
done frankly any time of day is going to be beneficial. 02:09:17.660 |
to improve your mood, and perhaps most importantly, 02:09:20.960 |
to be able to maneuver yourself in a deliberate way 02:09:30.580 |
and to really shift your default mode network 02:09:35.640 |
to somebody who can focus and who frankly is happier, 02:09:38.960 |
well then a more traditional third eye center type meditation 02:09:42.780 |
or a more traditional exteroceptive focused meditation 02:09:49.800 |
either focusing inward or focusing on a point 02:09:54.280 |
by whether or not you tend to be interoceptively biased 02:10:02.460 |
and you want to replace sleep that you've lost, 02:10:06.840 |
like Matthew Walker don't come after me with, 02:10:12.460 |
Probably with an alarm clock and I don't know, 02:10:15.300 |
blankets and a pillow or something of that sort. 02:10:18.060 |
In all seriousness, it's very clear that replacing sleep 02:10:28.680 |
if not downright useful for replacing sleep that you've lost. 02:10:33.680 |
Certainly the small amount of data that exists now 02:10:42.380 |
that a 30-minute yoga nidra, aka NSDR practice, 02:10:48.140 |
which puts people in a position to be more action-oriented 02:10:51.960 |
and focused, et cetera, when they come out of the yoga nidra. 02:11:02.340 |
what people think about when we talk about meditation. 02:11:04.540 |
Of course, this is an episode about meditation. 02:11:08.200 |
is that many people meditate to enhance their sleepability, 02:11:14.360 |
It appears that meditation is probably not ideal for that 02:11:22.460 |
for adjusting the default mode network toward more happiness 02:11:26.920 |
and for placing oneself in that healthy model 02:11:35.300 |
and I like to think that I've given you some key decisions 02:11:44.660 |
And maybe you're somebody who just answers that question 02:11:46.480 |
by saying, "Look, I'm not going to meditate regularly. 02:11:48.520 |
I just want to do the thing that's going to allow me 02:11:52.200 |
and is going to allow me to adjust my state of mind 02:11:55.620 |
when I'm not where I want to be for whatever reason, 02:12:00.480 |
And for those people, I would say a practice like NSDR, 02:12:05.720 |
as will a more traditional form of meditation. 02:12:08.940 |
I also want to just remind everybody that an app 02:12:12.260 |
that guides meditation, also with some information 02:12:23.000 |
I know millions of other people have as well, 02:12:30.440 |
on these continuums of interoception and exteroception 02:12:37.000 |
of meditation practice you should do in a given moment. 02:12:39.500 |
Whether or not you should focus your vision inward 02:12:43.760 |
and your attention outward, being a key component. 02:12:45.960 |
Whether or not you should do cyclic breathing, 02:12:48.840 |
which will allow your focus to be off your breathing 02:12:52.380 |
somewhat easier than if you do non-cyclic breathing, 02:12:56.720 |
Whether or not your breathing is going to be natural or not. 02:13:02.220 |
is designed to enhance your level of focus or to relax you. 02:13:08.820 |
that doesn't necessarily mean that it won't be relaxing. 02:13:23.120 |
of replenishing yourself, replacing sleep that you've lost, 02:13:29.420 |
On previous podcasts, I've talked about hypnosis, 02:13:32.980 |
and particularly the episode with Dr. David Spiegel, 02:13:38.000 |
but just understand that hypnosis is distinct 02:13:40.520 |
from breath work, from yoga nidra, from NSDR, 02:13:45.420 |
some of those components, like focusing your attention, 02:13:48.280 |
it involves actually directing your visual attention 02:13:53.140 |
it involves some breathing of a particular kind, 02:14:00.620 |
is really designed to fix or address a specific problem. 02:14:05.100 |
Whereas meditation, NSDR, yoga nidra, et cetera, 02:14:13.280 |
sleep issues, et cetera, but they generally are not directed 02:14:22.620 |
especially in the clinical context, not stage hypnosis, 02:14:24.880 |
but the clinical context for which there's a lot of research 02:14:27.820 |
to show it can, for instance, help with quitting smoking, 02:14:34.020 |
for smoking cessation with something like the Reverie app, 02:14:38.900 |
or for reducing insomnia, or for reducing pain, 02:14:43.020 |
or for any number of things, including trauma, et cetera, 02:14:45.180 |
hypnosis is really great at dealing with specific issues 02:14:51.040 |
Meditation tends to be focused on other things, 02:14:54.680 |
I'm guessing some of you are probably wondering 02:14:56.340 |
where to start, or if you're already an avid meditator, 02:15:03.860 |
a particular form of meditation that incorporates 02:15:08.040 |
all of the features that I've talked about up until now 02:15:21.500 |
And the time component has to do with a very simple fact, 02:15:35.240 |
as more or less the second hand on your clock of existence, 02:15:39.620 |
whereas when we tend to focus on things far away from us, 02:15:42.840 |
we tend to parse or carve up time within bigger bins. 02:15:47.680 |
If you've ever seen an airplane flying at a distance, 02:15:53.960 |
it's probably going five or 600 miles an hour, 02:15:58.760 |
Believe it or not, how you slice the time domain 02:16:10.200 |
The more closely your attention is placed on yourself, 02:16:17.200 |
or you think about the other side of the world, 02:16:21.480 |
well, then you're actually slicing time more broadly. 02:16:26.160 |
Fine slicing would be like slow motion, higher frame rate. 02:16:32.440 |
So even though things look like they're moving more slowly, 02:16:34.360 |
it's because your fidelity, your precision of measuring time 02:16:40.040 |
It's as if you're only have the hour's hand on the clock. 02:16:49.320 |
that incorporates everything that I've talked about today. 02:17:07.280 |
And it's one that we're starting to do some research on, 02:17:16.200 |
that it can even lead to some interesting insights, 02:17:18.480 |
both during the meditation and outside the meditation. 02:17:22.800 |
ideally you would do this outside or at a window, 02:17:25.080 |
but what you do is you essentially close your eyes. 02:17:28.600 |
I'm not going to close my eyes and do the meditation, 02:17:30.560 |
You close your eyes and you focus your attention 02:17:32.240 |
either on your third eye center or your breathing, 02:17:34.560 |
and you try and put 100% of your perceptual awareness 02:17:43.240 |
So you're 100% or trying to be 100% in interoception. 02:17:50.400 |
You focus on the surface of your body someplace. 02:17:52.920 |
I find that holding out my hand at sort of arms distance 02:17:59.640 |
So I'm splitting my attention now between my hand, 02:18:02.320 |
and I'm also going to pay attention to my breath 02:18:04.360 |
for the duration of three full inhales and exhales 02:18:10.160 |
So you're splitting interoception and exteroception 02:18:19.680 |
in your immediate environment, maybe 10, 15 feet away, 02:18:23.360 |
and you focus your attention on that location 02:18:27.700 |
so that you're still paying attention to your breathing. 02:18:29.200 |
You do that for the duration of three breaths, 02:18:31.520 |
but now you are in exteroception and interoception. 02:18:35.300 |
Then you focus your attention at some distance further away, 02:18:40.140 |
Now, this is why it's useful to do out of a window 02:18:44.840 |
You focus on the furthest point, maybe a horizon, 02:18:48.140 |
some furthest point for the duration of three breaths 02:18:50.860 |
while also paying attention to your breathing. 02:18:54.980 |
if you find it to be challenging to focus on both. 02:18:58.260 |
And then, and this is where it can be a little tricky, 02:19:00.560 |
but then what you actually focus on is the fact, 02:19:07.020 |
on this big ball that's floating out in space, right? 02:19:14.200 |
while also acknowledging that you are a small body, 02:19:17.040 |
literally, on this very seemingly large body, the earth, 02:19:32.780 |
And you might want to, and you do that for three breaths. 02:19:35.000 |
You focus on your interoception for three breaths. 02:19:41.320 |
but typically I will just do it for one segment 02:19:48.880 |
some distance in front of me, horizon, whole globe, 02:20:04.180 |
is that it has you deliberately step your awareness, 02:20:12.060 |
along that interoceptive, exteroceptive continuum. 02:20:19.180 |
as they'll say in the yoga classes, aware of, 02:20:21.980 |
I guess would be the more scientific way to state it, 02:20:26.980 |
and put your awareness completely outside yourself. 02:20:29.620 |
But most people will find that challenging to do 02:20:31.480 |
if they're already paying attention to their breath. 02:20:34.200 |
So I find it easier to just split my awareness 02:20:38.960 |
But by stepping through these different locations 02:20:41.420 |
and then deliberately placing your perception, 02:20:48.340 |
what you do is you essentially are practicing 02:20:54.040 |
that the human mind has to deliberately place 02:20:59.880 |
along the interoceptive, exteroceptive continuum. 02:21:06.520 |
tend to get locked at one location along that continuum. 02:21:17.220 |
Or if you're very focused on things out in the world, 02:21:19.100 |
you oftentimes can forget about your internal sensations 02:21:23.820 |
And being functional in work, in life, in relationship, 02:21:27.080 |
in all aspects, including your ability to fall asleep, 02:21:30.940 |
involves stepping yourself along these different locations, 02:21:45.280 |
just by way of how your visual system and the time domain 02:21:54.340 |
And so much of what involves being a functional human being 02:22:03.140 |
to a question somebody asks and then back again, 02:22:05.480 |
or from text messaging to listening to a lecture 02:22:09.200 |
or a podcast, or from listening to a lecture or podcast 02:22:12.640 |
and then going back into a mode of commuting, 02:22:18.640 |
or connect with family or friends, et cetera. 02:22:25.080 |
I should say the maladaptive behaviors and emotions 02:22:28.520 |
that show up in life are really not about any set 02:22:31.760 |
of behaviors or emotions being wrong or right, 02:22:39.960 |
which again is just fancy nerd speak for saying, 02:22:45.160 |
is a wonderful byproduct of a meditation practice, 02:22:57.240 |
through a practice deliberately so that you are flexibly 02:23:00.240 |
and dynamically able to engage in conversation 02:23:04.480 |
or focus and then disengage from the work you're focusing on 02:23:07.180 |
and actually have a conversation or be in the world 02:23:17.500 |
I realize this might sound a little bit vague. 02:23:19.440 |
For that reason, I encourage you not to think about it 02:23:27.620 |
I think it is a good one for people that find 02:23:32.880 |
and interoceptive meditation might be enjoyable to them 02:23:39.160 |
and other people who might find that that tends 02:23:43.020 |
I think it also ought to be very useful for people 02:23:48.500 |
more in the dissociative end of the continuum 02:23:50.760 |
and need to bring in a bit more of interoceptive awareness, 02:23:54.300 |
but either can't do that or uncomfortable doing that 02:23:59.780 |
or comfortable with feeling so much of their internal state 02:24:04.120 |
or that's just simply not the way they want to feel. 02:24:06.260 |
Now, as we round up, I do want to acknowledge 02:24:08.440 |
that there are an enormous number of rooms within the house 02:24:18.060 |
intention setting and mantras and an enormous number 02:24:21.860 |
of different features of meditation practices 02:24:32.940 |
For that reason, in future episodes and not long from now, 02:24:36.200 |
I'm going to be sitting down with experts in meditation 02:24:40.660 |
but other experts in meditation that certainly are versed 02:24:46.920 |
to specific research studies can certainly point us 02:24:49.960 |
toward the utility of things like mantras and intentions 02:24:58.980 |
and I hope you will join me for those as well. 02:25:01.080 |
If you're learning from and are enjoying this podcast, 02:25:05.180 |
That's a terrific zero cost way to support us. 02:25:10.020 |
on Spotify and Apple and on both Spotify and Apple, 02:25:20.200 |
please put those in the comment section on YouTube. 02:25:30.360 |
If you're not already following me on Instagram, 02:25:34.360 |
It's Huberman Lab on Instagram, Facebook, and Twitter. 02:25:37.780 |
And all three of those places I cover science 02:25:39.940 |
and science-based tools, some of which overlaps 02:25:42.000 |
with the content of the Huberman Lab podcast, 02:25:44.120 |
but much of which is distinct from the content 02:25:47.900 |
Thanks again for joining me for today's discussion 02:25:50.080 |
about the science and practice of meditation.