back to index

Understand & Improve Memory Using Science-Based Tools | Huberman Lab Podcast #72


Chapters

0:0 Memory, Improving Memory
2:45 Eight Sleep, Thesis, InsideTracker
7:54 Sensory Stimuli, Nervous System & Encoding Memory
11:12 Context & Memory Formation
13:46 Tool: Repetition, Improving Learning & Memory
17:11 Co-Activation and intensity Neuron Activation
20:50 Different Types of Memory
25:40 Memory Formation in the Brain, Hippocampus
28:0 Hippocampus, Role in Memory & Learning, Explicit vs. Implicit Memory
31:49 Emotion & Memory Enhancement
36:44 Tool: Emotion Saliency & Improved Memory
41:42 Conditioned-Placed Avoidance/Preference, Adrenaline
47:14 Adrenaline & Cortisol
49:35 Accelerating the Repetition Curve & Adrenaline
53:3 Tool: Enhancing Learning & Memory - Caffeine, Alpha-GPC & Stimulant Timing
60:50 Tool: Enhancing Learning & Memory - Sleep, Non-Sleep Deep Rest (NSDR)
64:48 Tool: Enhancing Learning & Memory - Deliberate Cold Exposure, Adrenaline
68:42 Timing of Adrenaline Release & Memory Formation
72:36 Chronically High Adrenaline & Cortisol, Impact on Learning & Memory
75:12 Adrenaline Linked with Learning: Not a New Principle
77:25 Amygdala, Adrenaline & Memory Formation, Generalization of Memories
82:20 Tool: Cardiovascular Exercise & Neurogenesis
87:0 Cardiovascular Exercise, Osteocalcin & Improved Hippocampal Function
89:59 Load-Bearing Exercise, Osteocalcin & Cognitive Ability
94:41 Tool: Timing of Exercise, Learning & Memory Enhancement
97:29 Photographic Memory
98:49 “Super Recognizers,” Facial Recognition
101:46 Tool: Mental Snapshots, Photographs & Memory Enhancement
109:12 Déjà Vu
113:24 Tool: Meditation, Daily Timing of Meditation
122:21 How to Enhance Memory
125:51 Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Patreon, Momentous Supplements, Instagram, Twitter, Neural Network Newsletter

Whisper Transcript | Transcript Only Page

00:00:00.000 | - Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
00:00:02.260 | where we discuss science and science-based tools
00:00:04.900 | for everyday life.
00:00:05.900 | I'm Andrew Huberman,
00:00:10.200 | and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
00:00:13.060 | at Stanford School of Medicine.
00:00:14.880 | Today, we are discussing memory,
00:00:16.660 | in particular, how to improve your memory.
00:00:19.340 | Now, the study of memory is one
00:00:20.660 | that dates back many decades.
00:00:22.700 | And by now, there's a pretty good understanding
00:00:25.360 | of how memories are formed in the brain,
00:00:27.860 | the different structures involved
00:00:29.300 | and some of the neurochemicals involved.
00:00:31.220 | We will talk about some of that today.
00:00:33.620 | Often overlooked, however,
00:00:34.820 | is that memories are not just about learning.
00:00:37.580 | Memories are also about placing your entire life
00:00:40.160 | into a context.
00:00:41.580 | And that's because what's really special about the brain,
00:00:44.180 | and in particular, the human brain,
00:00:46.040 | is its ability to place events in the context
00:00:48.500 | of past events, the present, and future events,
00:00:52.660 | and sometimes even combinations of the past and present,
00:00:54.980 | or present and future, and so on.
00:00:57.460 | So when we talk about memory,
00:00:58.820 | what we're really talking about
00:01:00.300 | is how your immediate experiences relate
00:01:03.020 | to previous and future experiences.
00:01:06.220 | Today, I'm going to make clear how that process occurs.
00:01:09.320 | Even if you don't have a background in biology or psychology,
00:01:11.820 | I promise to put it into language
00:01:13.140 | that anyone can access and understand.
00:01:15.300 | And we are going to talk about the science
00:01:17.620 | that points to specific tools
00:01:19.700 | for enhancing learning and memory.
00:01:22.400 | We're also going to talk about unlearning and forgetting.
00:01:25.100 | There are, of course, instances
00:01:26.260 | in which we would like to forget things.
00:01:28.380 | And that too is a biological process
00:01:30.660 | for which great tools exist to, for instance,
00:01:34.540 | eliminate or at least reduce the emotional load
00:01:37.320 | of our previous experience that you really did not like,
00:01:39.980 | or that perhaps even was traumatic to you.
00:01:43.000 | So today, you're going to learn about the systems
00:01:46.100 | in the brain and body that establish memories.
00:01:49.320 | You're going to learn why certain memories
00:01:51.340 | are easier to form than others.
00:01:53.300 | And I'm going to talk about specific tools
00:01:55.640 | that are grounded in not just one,
00:01:57.900 | not just a dozen, but well over a hundred studies
00:02:00.860 | in animals and humans that point to specific protocols
00:02:04.620 | that you can use in order to stamp down
00:02:07.380 | learning of particular things more easily.
00:02:09.980 | And you can also leverage that same knowledge
00:02:12.620 | to better forget or unload the emotional weight
00:02:15.260 | of experiences that you did not like.
00:02:17.540 | We are also going to discuss topics
00:02:19.060 | like deja vu and photographic memory.
00:02:21.980 | And for those of you that do not have a photographic memory,
00:02:24.980 | and I should point out
00:02:25.820 | that I do not have a photographic memory either.
00:02:28.480 | Well, you will learn how to use your visual system
00:02:31.360 | in order to better learn visual and auditory information.
00:02:35.220 | There are protocols to do this
00:02:36.560 | grounded in excellent peer-reviewed research.
00:02:39.100 | So while you may not have a true photographic memory,
00:02:41.940 | by the end of the episode, you will have tools in hand,
00:02:44.140 | or I should say tools in mind or in eyes and mind
00:02:47.760 | to be able to encode and remember specific events
00:02:51.340 | better than you would otherwise.
00:02:53.340 | Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize
00:02:54.880 | that this podcast is separate
00:02:56.220 | from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
00:02:58.380 | It is, however, part of my desire and effort
00:03:00.240 | to bring zero cost to consumer information
00:03:02.060 | about science and science-related tools
00:03:04.060 | to the general public.
00:03:05.460 | In keeping with that theme,
00:03:06.520 | I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
00:03:09.380 | Our first sponsor is Eight Sleep.
00:03:11.860 | Eight Sleep makes smart mattress covers
00:03:13.540 | with cooling, heating, and sleep tracking.
00:03:16.420 | Many times on this podcast,
00:03:17.660 | I've talked about the incredible relationship
00:03:19.700 | between temperature and sleep,
00:03:21.380 | as well as temperature and wakefulness.
00:03:24.300 | Many people aren't aware of this,
00:03:25.460 | but waking up in the morning
00:03:26.980 | is in part the consequence of your body heating up
00:03:30.180 | and falling asleep at night and remaining in deep sleep
00:03:33.140 | is in part the consequence of your body temperature
00:03:35.480 | dropping by one to three degrees.
00:03:38.160 | So it's vitally important that the temperature
00:03:40.060 | of your sleeping environment is controlled.
00:03:42.660 | I've had trouble over the years falling and staying asleep,
00:03:46.180 | or I should say falling asleep
00:03:47.300 | hasn't been so much of an issue for me,
00:03:48.700 | but waking up two or three or four hours later
00:03:50.860 | has been an issue.
00:03:52.060 | Oftentimes I'm too warm, I need to open a window,
00:03:54.140 | I need to adjust the temperature of the room and so on.
00:03:57.140 | Eight Sleep mattress covers are terrific
00:03:59.140 | because you can program the specific patterns of temperature
00:04:01.900 | that you want in your bed, or I should say below you
00:04:04.500 | coming from the mattress throughout the night.
00:04:07.160 | So I've programmed my Eight Sleep
00:04:08.860 | to put my mattress into a state of coolness
00:04:11.780 | in order to fall asleep,
00:04:13.140 | and then to get slightly cooler as the night goes on
00:04:15.980 | to get into even deeper sleep,
00:04:17.580 | and then to warm up as morning approaches.
00:04:20.860 | As mentioned before, Eight Sleep mattresses
00:04:22.480 | can be used for cooling and heating.
00:04:24.060 | So some of you may actually need to heat your mattress
00:04:26.660 | and Eight Sleep's terrific for that.
00:04:28.280 | For me, I use Eight Sleep to keep the mattress really cool.
00:04:31.100 | And I have to say,
00:04:32.300 | even though I thought I was sleeping pretty well before,
00:04:34.700 | I have not experienced the sort of sleep
00:04:36.280 | that I've been getting with Eight Sleep until now
00:04:38.980 | by creating this descending temperature
00:04:41.700 | in the beginning part of the night,
00:04:42.900 | cooler, cooler, cooler, and then warming up towards morning
00:04:45.780 | even has a nice little vibration function.
00:04:47.500 | So I wake up now to a slightly vibrating bed
00:04:49.940 | and the sleep that I'm getting is just amazing.
00:04:52.340 | And I feel so much better during the day as a consequence.
00:04:55.300 | If you'd like to try Eight Sleep,
00:04:56.660 | you can go to eightsleep.com/huberman
00:04:59.220 | and check out the pod pro cover
00:05:01.020 | and save $150 at checkout.
00:05:03.500 | Eight Sleep currently ships within the USA, Canada,
00:05:05.980 | and the UK.
00:05:06.980 | Again, that's eightsleep.com/huberman
00:05:09.220 | to save $150 at checkout.
00:05:11.380 | Today's episode is also brought to us by Thesis.
00:05:14.220 | Thesis makes custom nootropics.
00:05:16.340 | And to be quite honest, I don't like the word nootropics
00:05:19.060 | because nootropics means smart drugs.
00:05:21.580 | And there really isn't a neuroscience of smart.
00:05:25.620 | Of course, there is this notion of intelligence,
00:05:27.700 | but we now know there are lots of different forms
00:05:29.180 | of intelligence.
00:05:30.280 | Also, as a neuroscientist,
00:05:31.660 | we don't really think about intelligence.
00:05:33.300 | We think about the particular types of operations
00:05:36.000 | that you want your brain to perform in different contexts.
00:05:38.980 | So for instance, some tasks and life experiences
00:05:42.480 | require that you remember information better.
00:05:45.580 | Other tasks and life experiences and forms of learning
00:05:48.780 | involve focus and task switching.
00:05:50.840 | Others, creativity, and so on and so on.
00:05:53.320 | So the word nootropics is too much of a catchall
00:05:55.620 | or throwaway phrase to appeal to me.
00:05:58.460 | Thesis understands this.
00:06:00.060 | And what they've done is they've created nootropics
00:06:02.960 | that are each designed toward a particular end goal.
00:06:06.240 | For instance, they have blends for focus,
00:06:09.040 | blend for motivation, a blend for clarity.
00:06:11.300 | They use only the highest quality ingredients,
00:06:13.700 | things like Alpha GPC and phosphatidylserine.
00:06:16.480 | I've been using Thesis for close to six months now,
00:06:18.440 | and I can confidently say their nootropics
00:06:20.320 | have been a game changer for me.
00:06:21.860 | I often take the clarity formula
00:06:24.300 | prior to long bouts of work, meaning cognitive work,
00:06:26.780 | and the energy formula prior to physical workouts.
00:06:29.280 | To get your own personalized nootropic starter kit,
00:06:31.500 | you can go online to takethesis.com/huberman,
00:06:35.140 | take a three-minute quiz,
00:06:36.460 | and Thesis will send you four different formulas
00:06:38.520 | to try in your first month.
00:06:40.180 | Again, that's takethesis.com/huberman
00:06:42.440 | and use the code Huberman at checkout
00:06:44.100 | to get 10% off your first box.
00:06:46.260 | Today's podcast is also brought to us by Inside Tracker.
00:06:49.320 | Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform
00:06:51.720 | that analyzes data from your blood and DNA
00:06:54.220 | to help you better understand your body
00:06:55.780 | and help you reach your health goals.
00:06:57.780 | I've long been a believer in getting regular blood work done
00:07:00.620 | for the simple reason that many of the factors
00:07:03.020 | that impact your immediate and long-term health
00:07:05.260 | can only be assessed from a quality blood test.
00:07:07.820 | One of the challenges with a lot of blood tests
00:07:09.580 | and DNA tests out there, however,
00:07:11.300 | is that you'll get the numbers back
00:07:12.820 | and get information about your blood lipids
00:07:14.440 | and hormones and so forth,
00:07:15.960 | but there are no directives
00:07:16.960 | as to what to do with that information.
00:07:18.820 | Inside Tracker has solved that problem
00:07:20.460 | by creating a very easy to use dashboard.
00:07:22.940 | So you'll get your numbers back
00:07:24.100 | and you can see what's in range and what's out of range,
00:07:26.020 | and then they will point you
00:07:27.640 | to specific nutritional supplementation and behavioral tools
00:07:31.600 | that will allow you to bring those numbers
00:07:33.360 | into the proper ranges for you.
00:07:35.220 | I find that immensely beneficial
00:07:37.220 | because just having a bunch of data
00:07:38.880 | without any knowledge of what to do with that data
00:07:40.960 | is more overwhelming than having no data at all, frankly.
00:07:43.700 | If you'd like to try Inside Tracker,
00:07:45.140 | you can go to insidetracker.com/huberman
00:07:48.000 | to get 20% off any of Inside Tracker's plans.
00:07:50.760 | Again, that's insidetracker.com/huberman to get 20% off.
00:07:54.780 | Okay, let's talk about memory
00:07:56.340 | and let's talk about how to get better
00:07:57.840 | at remembering things.
00:07:59.420 | In order to address both of those things,
00:08:01.220 | we need to do a little bit of brain science 101 review,
00:08:04.460 | and I promise this will only take two minutes,
00:08:06.580 | and I promise that even if you don't have a background
00:08:08.860 | in biology, it will make sense.
00:08:10.640 | We are constantly being bombarded with physical stimuli,
00:08:14.780 | patterns of touch on our skin,
00:08:17.420 | light to our eyes, light to our skin for that matter,
00:08:21.100 | smells, tastes, and sound waves.
00:08:23.820 | In fact, if you can hear me saying this right now,
00:08:25.620 | well, that's the consequence of sound waves
00:08:28.020 | arriving into your ears through headphones, a computer,
00:08:30.660 | or some other speaker device.
00:08:32.660 | Each one of and all of those sensory stimuli
00:08:35.560 | are converted into electricity and chemical signals
00:08:39.120 | by your so-called nervous system,
00:08:40.400 | your brain, your spinal cord,
00:08:41.580 | and all their connections with the organs of the body
00:08:43.860 | and all the connections of your organs of the body
00:08:46.560 | back to your brain and spinal cord.
00:08:48.460 | One of the primary jobs of your nervous system, in fact,
00:08:50.620 | is to convert physical events in the world
00:08:53.020 | that are non-negotiable, right?
00:08:54.920 | Photons of light are photons of light.
00:08:56.680 | Sound waves are sound waves.
00:08:57.820 | There's no changing that,
00:08:59.140 | but your nervous system does change that.
00:09:01.140 | It converts those things into electrical signals
00:09:04.280 | and chemical signals,
00:09:05.720 | which are the language of your nervous system.
00:09:08.180 | Now, just because you're being bombarded
00:09:09.620 | with all this sensory information
00:09:10.880 | and it's being converted into a language
00:09:12.840 | that neurons and the rest of your nervous system
00:09:14.820 | can understand does not mean that you are aware of it all.
00:09:18.360 | In fact, you are only going to perceive a small amount
00:09:22.020 | of that sensory information.
00:09:23.760 | For instance, if you can hear me speaking right now,
00:09:26.400 | you are perceiving my voice,
00:09:28.420 | but you are also most likely neglecting the feeling
00:09:31.620 | of the contact of your skin
00:09:32.680 | with whichever surface you happen to be sitting
00:09:34.980 | or standing on.
00:09:36.740 | So it is only by perceiving a subset,
00:09:39.740 | a small fraction of the sensory events in our environment
00:09:42.820 | that we can make sense of the world around us.
00:09:44.400 | Otherwise we would just be overwhelmed
00:09:46.280 | with all the things that are happening
00:09:47.620 | in any one given moment.
00:09:49.480 | Now, memory is simply a bias in which perceptions
00:09:54.480 | will be replayed again in the future.
00:09:57.200 | Anytime you experience something,
00:09:58.980 | that is the consequence of specific chains of neurons
00:10:01.560 | that we call neural circuits being activated.
00:10:04.320 | And memory is simply a bias in the likelihood
00:10:07.540 | that that specific chain of neurons
00:10:09.580 | will be activated again.
00:10:12.080 | So for instance, if you can remember your name,
00:10:14.420 | and I certainly hope that you can,
00:10:16.440 | well, that means that there are specific chains of neurons
00:10:19.620 | in your brain that represent your name.
00:10:22.140 | And when those neurons connect with one another
00:10:25.220 | and communicate electrically with one another
00:10:27.380 | in a particular sequence, you remember your name.
00:10:30.660 | Were that particular chain of neurons to be disrupted,
00:10:34.220 | you would not be able to remember your name.
00:10:36.400 | Now, this might seem immensely simple,
00:10:37.960 | but it raises this really interesting question,
00:10:40.820 | which we talked about before,
00:10:42.960 | which is why do we remember certain things and not others?
00:10:45.720 | Because according to what I've just said,
00:10:49.040 | as you go through life,
00:10:50.140 | you're experiencing things all the time.
00:10:51.860 | You're constantly being bombarded with sensory stimuli.
00:10:54.860 | Some of those sensory stimuli you perceive,
00:10:56.880 | and only some of those perceptions
00:10:59.260 | get stamped down as memories.
00:11:02.020 | Today, I'm going to teach you how certain things
00:11:04.480 | get stamped down as memories.
00:11:05.880 | And I'm going to teach you how to leverage that process
00:11:08.800 | in order to remember the information that you want
00:11:11.160 | far better.
00:11:12.300 | Now, even though I've told you that a memory
00:11:13.840 | is simply a bias in the likelihood
00:11:15.340 | that a particular chain of neurons will be activated
00:11:17.560 | in a particular sequence again and again,
00:11:20.980 | it doesn't operate on its own.
00:11:23.260 | In fact, most of what we remember
00:11:25.620 | takes place in a context of other events.
00:11:29.200 | So for instance, you can most likely remember your name,
00:11:32.780 | and yet you're probably not thinking about
00:11:35.500 | when it was that you first learned your name.
00:11:38.040 | This generally happens
00:11:39.420 | when we are very, very young children.
00:11:41.580 | And yet I'm guessing you could probably remember
00:11:45.220 | a time when someone mispronounced your name
00:11:47.580 | or made fun of your name.
00:11:49.260 | Or as the case was for me,
00:11:51.140 | I got to the third grade and there were two Andrews.
00:11:53.860 | And sadly for me, I lost the coin flip
00:11:56.900 | that allowed me to keep Andrew.
00:11:58.140 | And from about third grade until about 12th grade,
00:12:02.460 | people called me Andy, which I really did not prefer.
00:12:04.980 | So if you call me Andy in the comments,
00:12:07.060 | I'll delete your comment.
00:12:07.900 | Just kidding, doesn't bother me that much.
00:12:09.980 | But eventually I reclaimed Andrew as my name.
00:12:13.440 | Well, it was mine to begin with and throughout,
00:12:15.460 | but I started going by Andrew again.
00:12:18.280 | Why do I say this?
00:12:19.120 | Well, there's a whole context to my name for me.
00:12:22.140 | And there may or may not be a whole context
00:12:24.520 | to your name for you.
00:12:25.620 | But presumably if you asked your parents
00:12:27.000 | why they named you your given name,
00:12:28.960 | you'll get a context, et cetera.
00:12:30.940 | That context reflects the activation
00:12:33.800 | of other neural circuits that are also related
00:12:35.980 | to other events in your life, not just your name,
00:12:38.500 | but probably your siblings names and who your parents are
00:12:41.580 | and on and on and on.
00:12:43.020 | And so the way memory works is that each individual thing
00:12:47.200 | that we remember or that we want to remember
00:12:48.980 | is linked to something by either a close,
00:12:51.700 | a medium or a very distant association.
00:12:54.540 | This turns out to be immensely important.
00:12:56.900 | I know many of you will read or will encounter programs
00:13:01.080 | that are designed to help you enhance your memory.
00:13:03.340 | You have these phenoms that can remember 50 names
00:13:06.500 | in a room full of people,
00:13:08.060 | or they can remember a bunch of names of novel objects
00:13:11.060 | or maybe even in different languages.
00:13:12.820 | And oftentimes that's done by association.
00:13:15.520 | So people will come up with little mental tricks
00:13:18.020 | to either link the sound of a word
00:13:21.260 | or the meaning of a word in some way that's meaningful
00:13:23.520 | for them and will enhance their memory.
00:13:25.520 | That can be done and is impressive when we see it.
00:13:28.080 | And for those of you who can do that, congratulations.
00:13:30.340 | Most of us can't do that,
00:13:32.060 | or at least it requires a lot of effort and training.
00:13:35.220 | However, there are things that we can do
00:13:37.520 | that leverage the natural biology of our nervous system
00:13:40.700 | to enhance learning and memory of particular perceptions
00:13:43.700 | and particular information.
00:13:45.900 | Let's first just talk about the most basic ways
00:13:48.420 | that we learn and remember things
00:13:49.860 | and how to improve learning and memory.
00:13:52.180 | And the most basic one is repetition.
00:13:55.140 | Now the study of memory and the role of repetition
00:13:58.100 | actually dates back to the late 1800s, early 1900s,
00:14:01.660 | when Ebbinghaus developed the first
00:14:03.780 | so-called learning curves.
00:14:06.020 | Now learning curves are simply what results
00:14:08.840 | when you quantify how many repetitions of something
00:14:11.480 | are required in order to remember something.
00:14:15.100 | In fact, it's been said that Ebbinghaus liberated
00:14:17.960 | the understanding of learning from the philosophers
00:14:20.560 | by generating these learning curves.
00:14:22.480 | What do you mean by that?
00:14:23.320 | Well, before Ebbinghaus came along,
00:14:26.960 | learning and memory were thought to be philosophical ideas.
00:14:31.420 | Ebbinghaus came along and said,
00:14:33.480 | well, let's actually take some measurements.
00:14:35.980 | Let's measure how well I can remember a sequence of words
00:14:39.200 | or a sequence of numbers if I just repeat them.
00:14:41.660 | So what Ebbinghaus did is he would take a sequence
00:14:43.780 | of numbers or words on a page and he would read them.
00:14:47.700 | And then he would take a separate sheet of paper
00:14:50.740 | and we have to presume he didn't cheat.
00:14:52.720 | And he would write down as many of them as he could
00:14:55.140 | and he would try and keep them in the same sequence.
00:14:57.140 | Then he would compare to the original list
00:14:58.980 | and he would see how many errors he made.
00:15:00.780 | And you do this over and over and over again.
00:15:03.340 | And as you would expect,
00:15:04.740 | early in the training and the learning,
00:15:08.620 | it took a lot more repetitions to get the sequence correct.
00:15:11.340 | And over time, it took fewer sequences.
00:15:13.900 | And he referred to that difference
00:15:15.580 | in the initial number of repetitions that he had to perform
00:15:19.300 | versus the later number of repetitions
00:15:22.340 | that he had to perform as a so-called savings.
00:15:25.140 | So he literally thought of the brain
00:15:26.940 | as having to generate a kind of a currency of effort.
00:15:30.660 | And he talked about savings as the reduction
00:15:34.000 | in the amount of effort that he had to put forward
00:15:36.340 | in order to learn information.
00:15:37.740 | And what he got was a learning curve.
00:15:38.900 | And you can imagine what that learning curve looked like.
00:15:40.640 | It was at a very sharp peak at the beginning
00:15:42.580 | that dropped off over time.
00:15:44.180 | And of course he remembered
00:15:45.260 | all this meaningless information.
00:15:47.240 | But even though the information
00:15:48.660 | might've been meaningless, the experiment itself
00:15:51.660 | and what Ebbinghaus demonstrated was immensely meaningful
00:15:54.860 | because what it said was that with repetition,
00:15:57.760 | we can activate particular sequences of neurons.
00:16:01.600 | And that repeated activation lays down
00:16:04.600 | what we call a memory.
00:16:06.080 | And that might all seem like a big duh,
00:16:07.820 | but prior to Ebbinghaus, none of that was known.
00:16:09.960 | Now I should also say Ebbinghaus,
00:16:12.140 | because of when he was alive,
00:16:13.540 | was not aware of these things that we call neural circuits.
00:16:16.200 | It was in 1906 that Golgi and Cajal got the Nobel prize
00:16:20.360 | for actually showing that neurons are independent cells
00:16:22.800 | connected by synapses,
00:16:23.920 | these little gaps between them where they communicate.
00:16:26.220 | So he may have been aware of that,
00:16:27.780 | but the whole notion of neural circuits
00:16:29.140 | hadn't really come about.
00:16:30.440 | Nevertheless, what the Ebbinghaus learning curves
00:16:32.760 | really established was that sheer repetition,
00:16:36.340 | just repeating things over and over and over again
00:16:39.220 | is sufficient to learn.
00:16:41.500 | Something that no doubt had been observed before,
00:16:43.560 | but it had never been formally quantified.
00:16:46.860 | Now, if we look at that result,
00:16:50.260 | there's something really important
00:16:51.420 | that lies a little bit cryptic.
00:16:53.140 | That's not so obvious to most people,
00:16:55.540 | which is the information that he was trying to learn
00:16:58.900 | wasn't any more interesting the second time
00:17:01.440 | than it was the first,
00:17:02.780 | probably was even less interesting
00:17:04.700 | and less and less interesting with each repetition.
00:17:06.820 | And yet it was sheer repetition
00:17:09.140 | that allowed him to remember.
00:17:11.900 | Now, sometime later in the early to mid 1920s,
00:17:15.020 | a psychologist in Canada named Donald Hebb
00:17:17.500 | came up with what was called Hebb's postulate.
00:17:20.060 | And Hebb's postulate, broadly speaking,
00:17:23.060 | is this idea that if a sequence of neurons is active
00:17:28.060 | at the same time or at roughly the same time,
00:17:31.500 | that that would lead to a strengthening of the connections
00:17:33.880 | between those neurons.
00:17:35.380 | And many, many decades of experimentation later,
00:17:38.460 | we now know that postulate to be true.
00:17:41.020 | Neurons themselves are not smart.
00:17:44.780 | They don't have knowledge.
00:17:46.420 | So every memory is the consequences, I told you before,
00:17:49.660 | of the repeated activation of a particular chain of neurons.
00:17:53.300 | And what Ebbinghaus showed through repetition
00:17:55.640 | and what Donald Hebb proposed,
00:17:57.980 | and it was eventually verified through experimentation
00:18:00.620 | on animals and humans,
00:18:02.340 | was that if you encourage the co-activation of neurons,
00:18:06.480 | meaning have neurons fire at roughly the same time,
00:18:11.220 | they will strengthen their connections.
00:18:12.960 | It leads to a bias in the probability
00:18:14.940 | that those neurons will be active again.
00:18:17.160 | Now, this is vitally important
00:18:18.500 | because nowadays we hear a lot about how memories
00:18:21.820 | are the consequence of new neurons added in the brain,
00:18:25.460 | or that every time you learn something,
00:18:28.340 | a new connection in your brain forms.
00:18:30.780 | Well, sorry to break it to you,
00:18:31.820 | but that's simply not the case.
00:18:34.100 | Most of the time, and I want to emphasize most, not all,
00:18:36.800 | but most of the time when we learn something,
00:18:38.940 | it's because existing neurons, not new neurons,
00:18:42.400 | but existing neurons strengthen their connection
00:18:45.460 | through co-activation over and over and over
00:18:48.940 | through repetition, or, and this is a very important or,
00:18:53.060 | or through very strong activation once and only once.
00:18:58.060 | In fact, there's something called one trial learning
00:18:59.900 | whereby we experience something
00:19:01.980 | and we will remember that thing forever.
00:19:05.300 | This is often most associated with negative events,
00:19:08.100 | and I'll explain why in a few minutes,
00:19:10.000 | but it can also be associated with positive events,
00:19:12.940 | like the first time you saw your romantic partner
00:19:16.960 | or something that happened with that romantic partner,
00:19:19.660 | or the first time that you saw your child,
00:19:22.380 | or any other positive event,
00:19:23.920 | as well as any other extremely negative event.
00:19:26.820 | So again, both repetition,
00:19:28.740 | and I guess we could label it intensity,
00:19:31.900 | but what we really mean when we say intensity
00:19:33.620 | is strong activation of neurons can lay down these traces,
00:19:37.240 | these circuits that are far more likely to be active again,
00:19:41.620 | than had there not been repetition
00:19:43.820 | or not some strong activation of those circuits.
00:19:47.220 | So with that in mind,
00:19:48.320 | let's return to the original contrarian question
00:19:51.860 | that I raised before,
00:19:52.720 | which is why do we remember anything?
00:19:55.760 | Every day you wake up,
00:19:57.380 | your neurons in your brain and body are active,
00:20:00.140 | different neural circuits are active,
00:20:01.780 | and yet you only remember a small fraction
00:20:04.140 | of the things that happen each day,
00:20:05.540 | and yet you retain a lot of information from previous days
00:20:08.060 | and the days before those and so on.
00:20:10.580 | It is only with a lot of repetition
00:20:14.260 | or with extremely strong activation
00:20:17.100 | of a given neural circuit that we will create new memories.
00:20:20.060 | And so in a few minutes,
00:20:20.900 | I'll explain how to get extremely strong activation
00:20:24.000 | of particular neural circuits.
00:20:25.980 | Repetition is pretty obvious, repetition is repetition,
00:20:29.080 | but in a few minutes,
00:20:30.620 | I'll illustrate a whole set of experiments
00:20:32.980 | and a whole set of tools
00:20:34.340 | that point to how you can get extra strong activation
00:20:38.140 | of a given neural circuit as it relates to learning
00:20:40.620 | so that you will remember that information,
00:20:43.180 | perhaps not just with one trial of learning,
00:20:45.840 | but certainly with far fewer repetitions
00:20:48.500 | than would be required otherwise.
00:20:50.380 | Before we go any further,
00:20:51.500 | I want to preface the discussion by saying
00:20:53.020 | that there are a lot of different kinds of memory.
00:20:55.020 | In fact, were you to take a voyage
00:20:56.700 | into the neuroscience and or psychology of memory,
00:21:00.180 | you would find an immense number of different terms
00:21:02.460 | to describe the immense number of different types of memory
00:21:05.760 | that researchers focus on.
00:21:07.880 | But for sake of today's discussion,
00:21:09.680 | really just want to focus on short-term memory,
00:21:11.760 | medium-term memory and long-term memory.
00:21:14.400 | And while there's still debate,
00:21:16.320 | as is always the case with scientists, frankly,
00:21:18.500 | about the exact divisions between short-term,
00:21:20.380 | medium and long-term memory,
00:21:22.120 | we can broadly define short-term memory and long-term memory
00:21:26.740 | and we can describe a couple of different types of those
00:21:29.220 | that I think you can relate to in your everyday life.
00:21:32.020 | The most common form of short-term memory
00:21:33.660 | that we're going to focus on is called working memory.
00:21:36.180 | Working memory is your ability to keep a chain of numbers
00:21:39.720 | in mind for some period of time,
00:21:41.880 | but the expectation really isn't
00:21:44.180 | that you would remember those numbers the next day
00:21:46.780 | and certainly not the next week.
00:21:48.220 | So a good example would be a phone number.
00:21:49.780 | If I were to tell you a phone number, 4932938,
00:21:53.480 | well, you could probably remember it 4932938,
00:21:56.980 | but if I came back tomorrow and asked you
00:21:59.420 | to repeat that chain of numbers, most likely you would not,
00:22:02.420 | unless of course we used a particular tool
00:22:05.180 | to stamp down that memory into your mind
00:22:07.180 | and commit it to long-term memory.
00:22:10.040 | Now, of course, in this day and age,
00:22:11.580 | most people have phone numbers programmed into their phone.
00:22:14.820 | They don't really have to remember the exact numbers.
00:22:16.620 | It's usually done by contact identity and so forth.
00:22:20.900 | So a different example
00:22:21.960 | that some of you are probably more familiar with
00:22:24.060 | would be those security codes.
00:22:25.900 | So you try and log on to an app or a website
00:22:28.180 | and it asks you for a security code
00:22:29.860 | that's been sent to your text messages
00:22:31.580 | and then you can either plug that in directly in some cases
00:22:33.940 | or you have to remember that short sequence
00:22:35.340 | of anywhere usually from six to seven,
00:22:37.580 | sometimes eight numbers.
00:22:38.780 | Your ability to do that, to switch back and forth
00:22:40.860 | between web pages or apps and plug in that number
00:22:45.560 | by remembering the sequence and plugging it in
00:22:47.780 | by texting or keying it in on your keyboard,
00:22:50.800 | that's a really good example of working memory.
00:22:53.100 | Long-term memory of the sort
00:22:54.380 | that we're going to be talking a lot about today
00:22:56.420 | is your ability to commit certain patterns of information,
00:22:59.320 | either cognitive information or motor information, right?
00:23:02.620 | The ability to move your limbs in a particular sequence
00:23:05.560 | over long periods of time,
00:23:07.380 | such that you could remember it a day or a week or a month
00:23:10.260 | or maybe even a year or several years later.
00:23:12.900 | So we've got short-term memory and long-term memory
00:23:14.600 | and we've got this working memory,
00:23:15.900 | which is kind of keeping something online,
00:23:18.140 | but then discarding it, okay?
00:23:19.980 | Not online on a computer, but online within your brain.
00:23:22.780 | There are also two major categories of memory
00:23:25.380 | that I'd like you to know about.
00:23:26.540 | One is explicit memory.
00:23:28.460 | So this is not necessarily explicit of the sort
00:23:31.140 | that you're used to thinking about,
00:23:32.960 | but rather the fact that you can declare you know something.
00:23:36.420 | So you have an explicit memory of your name.
00:23:39.500 | Presumably you have an explicit memory of the house
00:23:42.380 | or the apartment that you grew up in.
00:23:44.340 | You know something and you know you know it
00:23:46.700 | and you can declare it.
00:23:47.700 | So I can ask you,
00:23:48.620 | what was the color of the first car that you owned?
00:23:51.780 | Or what is the color of your romantic partner's hair?
00:23:56.660 | These sorts of things.
00:23:57.480 | That's an explicit declarative memory,
00:23:59.740 | but you also have explicit procedural memories.
00:24:04.100 | Now, procedural memories, as the name suggests,
00:24:06.300 | involve action sequences.
00:24:08.980 | The simplest one, it's almost ridiculously simple,
00:24:11.620 | is walking.
00:24:12.460 | If I say, how is it that you walk from one room
00:24:15.300 | to the other, you'd probably say,
00:24:16.460 | well, I go that direction, then I turn left.
00:24:17.960 | I say, no, no, no, no.
00:24:18.820 | How is it exactly that you do it?
00:24:20.700 | And say, well, I move my left foot,
00:24:22.220 | then my right foot, then my left foot.
00:24:24.100 | And you could describe that.
00:24:25.860 | So it's an explicit procedural memory.
00:24:27.940 | So much so that if you were going to teach a young toddler
00:24:30.660 | how to walk, you would probably say,
00:24:32.660 | okay, good, good, try.
00:24:33.840 | Okay, and you, you know,
00:24:34.940 | probably that's going to be pre-language for the toddler,
00:24:37.620 | but you're going to encourage them to move one leg
00:24:40.040 | than the other.
00:24:40.880 | And you're going to encourage and reward them
00:24:43.100 | for moving one leg than the other
00:24:44.700 | because you have an explicit procedural memory
00:24:47.420 | of how to walk.
00:24:48.980 | Okay, almost ridiculously simple,
00:24:50.580 | maybe even truly ridiculously simple.
00:24:52.660 | But nonetheless, when you think about it in the context
00:24:55.340 | of neural circuits and neural firing, pretty amazing.
00:24:58.220 | Even more amazing is the fact that all explicit memories,
00:25:03.220 | both declarative and procedural explicit memories
00:25:07.860 | can be moved from explicit to implicit.
00:25:11.980 | What do I mean by that?
00:25:12.980 | Well, in the example of walking,
00:25:15.520 | you might've chuckled a little bit
00:25:16.600 | or kind of shook your head and said,
00:25:18.180 | that's a ridiculous thing to ask.
00:25:19.740 | How do I walk from one room to the next?
00:25:21.180 | I just walk, I just do it.
00:25:22.900 | Ah, well, what is just do it?
00:25:24.980 | What it is is that you have an implicit understanding,
00:25:28.380 | meaning your nervous system knows how to walk
00:25:31.660 | without you actually having to think about
00:25:35.460 | what you know about how to walk.
00:25:37.100 | You just get up out of your chair
00:25:38.380 | or you get up out of bed and you walk.
00:25:40.460 | In the brain, you have a structure.
00:25:43.260 | In fact, you have one on each side of your brain.
00:25:45.980 | It's called the hippocampus.
00:25:47.460 | The hippocampus literally means seahorse.
00:25:49.900 | Anatomists like to name brain structures after things
00:25:52.780 | that they think those brain structures resemble.
00:25:55.480 | When I look at the hippocampus,
00:25:57.100 | frankly, it doesn't look like a seahorse,
00:25:58.960 | which either reflects my lack of understanding
00:26:01.440 | of what a seahorse really looks like, a visual deficit,
00:26:03.780 | or I think it's fair to say that those anatomists
00:26:06.240 | were using a little bit of creative elaboration
00:26:09.780 | when thinking about what the hippocampus looks like.
00:26:12.220 | Nonetheless, it is a curved structure.
00:26:15.360 | It has many layers.
00:26:17.200 | It's been described by my colleague, Robert Sapolsky,
00:26:19.700 | and by others as looking more like a jelly roll
00:26:22.620 | or a cinnamon roll is what it looks like to me.
00:26:25.740 | And if you were to take one cinnamon roll,
00:26:28.020 | chop it down the middle.
00:26:29.700 | So now you've got two half cinnamon rolls
00:26:32.020 | and rather than put them back together
00:26:33.920 | in the configuration they were before,
00:26:35.520 | you just slide one down so that you've got essentially
00:26:39.100 | two C's, two C-shaped halves of the cinnamon roll
00:26:43.380 | and you push them together,
00:26:45.420 | slightly offset from one another.
00:26:47.180 | Well, that's what the hippocampus looks like to me.
00:26:49.340 | And I think that's a far better description
00:26:51.540 | of its actual physical structure.
00:26:53.300 | But I guess if you were to use that physical structure
00:26:56.560 | as the name, well, then you'd have to open up
00:26:57.780 | a brain atlas and it would be called
00:26:59.700 | two half C cinnamon rolls stuffed halfway together.
00:27:02.100 | So that's not very good.
00:27:02.940 | So I guess seahorse will work.
00:27:04.620 | Hippocampus is the name of this structure
00:27:07.140 | and it is the site in your brain.
00:27:10.180 | And again, you have one on each side of your brain
00:27:13.140 | in which explicit declarative memories are formed.
00:27:17.460 | It is not where those memories are stored and maintained.
00:27:21.220 | It is where they are established in the first place.
00:27:23.980 | In contrast, implicit memories, right?
00:27:28.120 | These subconscious memories are formed and stored
00:27:32.540 | elsewhere in the brain, mainly by areas like the cerebellum,
00:27:36.180 | but also the neocortex,
00:27:37.300 | the kind of outer shell of your brain.
00:27:39.180 | The cerebellum is, it literally means mini brain.
00:27:42.180 | And it does in fact look like a mini brain
00:27:44.300 | and is in the back of the brain.
00:27:45.660 | And the neocortex is the outer part of the brain
00:27:47.920 | that covers all the other stuff.
00:27:49.900 | So the hippocampus is vitally important
00:27:53.420 | for establishing these new declarative memories
00:27:56.180 | of what you know and what you know how to do.
00:28:00.260 | Now, in order to really understand
00:28:02.060 | the role of the hippocampus in memory,
00:28:03.660 | in particular explicit declarative
00:28:06.220 | and explicit procedural memory,
00:28:08.020 | and to really understand how that's distinct
00:28:10.340 | from implicit declarative and implicit procedural memories,
00:28:15.340 | we have to look to a clinical case.
00:28:17.820 | And the clinical case that I'm referring to
00:28:19.300 | is a patient who went by the name HM.
00:28:22.140 | Patients go by their initials
00:28:23.800 | in order to maintain confidentiality of their real identity.
00:28:27.500 | HM had what's called intractable epilepsy.
00:28:31.500 | So he would have these really dramatic
00:28:33.780 | so-called grand mal seizures or drop seizures.
00:28:37.120 | For those of you that know somebody with epilepsy
00:28:39.540 | or that have epilepsy, you might be familiar with this.
00:28:43.680 | You can have petit mal seizures, which are minor seizures.
00:28:47.100 | You can have tonic-clonic seizures,
00:28:48.540 | which are sometimes not even detectable.
00:28:50.320 | You can have absent seizures where people will just stop.
00:28:52.820 | It's almost as if their brain goes on pause
00:28:55.020 | and they'll just stop there.
00:28:55.860 | It was reported actually that Einstein had absent seizures,
00:28:59.120 | although I don't know that
00:28:59.960 | that's ever really been confirmed neurologically.
00:29:02.900 | Grand mal seizures are extremely severe,
00:29:06.800 | and that's what HM had.
00:29:08.180 | So he could just be going about his day
00:29:10.140 | and maybe even cooking or doing something,
00:29:12.500 | driving, operating any kind of machinery,
00:29:14.740 | and then all of a sudden he would just have a drop seizure
00:29:17.500 | so he would just physically drop
00:29:18.940 | and go into a grand mal seizure.
00:29:20.360 | So convulsing of the whole body,
00:29:22.180 | loss of consciousness, et cetera.
00:29:24.280 | Or he would feel it coming on.
00:29:26.320 | Oftentimes people with epilepsy
00:29:27.660 | can feel the epilepsy seizure coming on
00:29:29.700 | kind of like a wave from the back of the brain.
00:29:32.260 | And sometimes they can get to a safe circumstance,
00:29:34.340 | but not always.
00:29:35.180 | And so the frequency and the intensity of his seizures
00:29:38.240 | were so robust that the neurosurgeons and neurologists
00:29:41.840 | decided that they needed to locate the origin,
00:29:44.380 | what they call the foci of those seizures
00:29:47.580 | and remove that brain tissue.
00:29:49.460 | Because the way seizures work is they spread out
00:29:52.040 | from that focus or that foci of brain tissue.
00:29:56.500 | And unfortunately for HM,
00:29:58.980 | the focus of his seizures was the hippocampus.
00:30:02.220 | So after a lot of deliberation, a neurosurgeon,
00:30:05.480 | in fact, one of the most famous neurosurgeons
00:30:07.580 | in the world at that time
00:30:08.920 | made what are called electrolytic lesions,
00:30:11.860 | actually burned out the hippocampus in the brain of HM.
00:30:16.180 | And as a consequence, he lost all explicit memory.
00:30:20.840 | Now, the consequence of this was that he couldn't exist
00:30:23.480 | in normal, everyday life, like most people.
00:30:26.080 | So he had to live mostly, not entirely,
00:30:28.840 | but mostly in a kind of hospital setting.
00:30:31.940 | And I've talked to several people who have,
00:30:34.980 | I should say, who met HM directly
00:30:37.900 | because he's no longer alive,
00:30:39.940 | but an interaction with him might look like the following.
00:30:44.140 | He would walk up to you just fine.
00:30:45.880 | You wouldn't know that he had any kind of brain damage.
00:30:49.220 | He could walk fine.
00:30:50.420 | He could speak fine.
00:30:51.420 | And he'd say, "Hi, I'm Andrew."
00:30:53.340 | And he'd say, "Hi, I'm," whatever his name happened to be.
00:30:55.940 | He wouldn't say HM, but he'd probably say his real name.
00:30:58.720 | And then perhaps someone new would walk into the room.
00:31:02.200 | He might turn around, look at that person,
00:31:03.860 | as any of us might do, then turn around back to me
00:31:08.140 | and say, "Hi, what's your name?"
00:31:10.360 | And if I were to say, "Well, I just told you my name
00:31:12.500 | and you just told me your name.
00:31:14.140 | Do you remember that?"
00:31:14.980 | He'd say, "I'm sorry, I don't remember any of that.
00:31:16.780 | What's your name?"
00:31:17.620 | So you had to go through this over and over again.
00:31:19.200 | So a complete lack of explicit declarative memory.
00:31:22.820 | Now he did have some memory for previous events in his life
00:31:27.820 | that dated way back, okay?
00:31:30.180 | Again, hinting at the idea that memories
00:31:33.360 | are not necessarily stored in the hippocampus.
00:31:36.420 | They're just formed in the hippocampus.
00:31:37.900 | So once they've moved out of the hippocampus
00:31:39.460 | to other brain areas, he could still keep those memories.
00:31:42.260 | They're in a different database, if you will.
00:31:43.960 | They're in a different pattern of firing
00:31:45.420 | of other neural circuits, but he couldn't form new memories.
00:31:48.280 | Now there's some very important and interesting twists
00:31:51.220 | on what HM could and could not do
00:31:53.420 | in terms of learning and memory
00:31:55.180 | that teach us a lot about the brain.
00:31:56.800 | In fact, I think most neuroscientists would agree
00:31:59.100 | that this unfortunate case of HM's epilepsy
00:32:02.140 | and the subsequent neurosurgery that he had
00:32:04.600 | taught us much of what we know, or at least think about,
00:32:07.300 | in terms of human learning and memory.
00:32:09.660 | For instance, as I mentioned before,
00:32:11.580 | he still had implicit knowledge.
00:32:13.920 | He knew how to walk.
00:32:14.800 | He knew how to do certain things like make a cup of coffee.
00:32:17.820 | He knew the names of people that he had met
00:32:21.380 | much earlier in his life and so on.
00:32:23.900 | And yet he couldn't form new memories.
00:32:26.060 | Now in violation to that last statement,
00:32:29.380 | there were some elements of HM's emotionality
00:32:32.920 | that suggests that there was some sort of residual capacity
00:32:36.500 | to learn new information,
00:32:38.060 | but it wasn't what we normally think of
00:32:40.500 | as explicit declarative or procedural memory.
00:32:43.080 | For instance, it's been reported, or it's been said,
00:32:46.980 | I should say, because I don't know that the studies
00:32:49.000 | were ever done with intense physiological measurements,
00:32:51.860 | that if you were to tell HM a joke
00:32:54.820 | and he thought it was funny, he would laugh really hard.
00:32:58.180 | Though he liked jokes, so you'd tell him, you'd say,
00:32:59.760 | "HM, I want to tell you a joke."
00:33:01.760 | You'd tell him a joke and he'd laugh really hard.
00:33:03.940 | Then you could leave the room, come back,
00:33:05.980 | and tell him the same joke again.
00:33:07.980 | Now, keep in mind, he did not remember
00:33:09.700 | that you told him the joke previously.
00:33:11.980 | And the second time he would laugh a little bit less.
00:33:14.580 | And then you'd leave the room, come back again,
00:33:17.220 | say, "Hi, I'm Andrew."
00:33:18.700 | And he'd say, "Oh, nice to meet you."
00:33:19.620 | Because as you recall, because you can recall things,
00:33:23.040 | but he couldn't recall things.
00:33:24.500 | He didn't know that he just met you,
00:33:25.820 | or at least he couldn't remember it.
00:33:27.240 | You tell him the joke a third time or a fourth time.
00:33:29.340 | And with each subsequent telling of the joke,
00:33:31.620 | he found it a little less funny, just as,
00:33:34.920 | keep this in mind, folks.
00:33:36.360 | If you tell a joke and you get a big laugh,
00:33:39.180 | don't tell it again, at least not immediately,
00:33:42.060 | not to the same person or the same crowd,
00:33:44.360 | because the second time it's a little less funny,
00:33:46.600 | and the third time it's a little less funny.
00:33:47.980 | And that actually has to do with a whole element of dopamine
00:33:51.180 | and its relationship to surprise.
00:33:53.360 | And that's the topic of a future podcast
00:33:55.260 | where we talk all about humor and novelty in the brain.
00:33:58.420 | But the point being that certain forms of memory
00:34:02.140 | seem to exist in a kind of phantom-like way
00:34:05.000 | within HM's brain.
00:34:06.400 | What do I mean by that?
00:34:07.300 | Well, this underscores the fact that he had
00:34:09.880 | an implicit memory of having heard the joke before.
00:34:13.620 | And it suggests that humor, or at least what we find funny,
00:34:16.720 | is somehow more related to procedures,
00:34:19.300 | similar to walking or motor ability,
00:34:21.540 | than it is to the precise content of that joke.
00:34:25.940 | That's a little bit of an abstract concept,
00:34:27.480 | but the point is that HM lacked explicit declarative memory.
00:34:31.320 | He couldn't tell you what he had just heard.
00:34:34.020 | He could not learn new information.
00:34:36.120 | And he couldn't tell you how to do something
00:34:38.600 | unless he had learned how to do that something
00:34:40.940 | many years prior.
00:34:42.880 | Now, there've been a lot of other patients besides HM
00:34:45.620 | that have had brain lesions due to epilepsy,
00:34:49.200 | or I should say, due to surgeries to treat epilepsy,
00:34:51.980 | due to strokes, due to sadly gunshot wounds
00:34:55.540 | and other forms of what we call infarct,
00:34:58.540 | infarct, I-N-F-A-R-C-T, infarct is the word we use
00:35:03.540 | to describe damage to a particular brain region.
00:35:07.180 | And many different patients with many different patterns
00:35:10.240 | of infarct have taught us a lot about how memory
00:35:13.960 | and other aspects of the brain work.
00:35:15.700 | HM really teaches us that what we know
00:35:21.280 | and what we are able to do is the consequence of things
00:35:24.740 | that we are aware of and learnings that have been passed off
00:35:28.200 | into subconscious knowledge, that our body knows,
00:35:31.000 | our brain knows, but we don't know exactly
00:35:33.920 | how we know that thing.
00:35:35.560 | And I tell you the story about HM's ability
00:35:37.980 | to understand a joke,
00:35:39.800 | but that with repeated telling of a joke,
00:35:42.340 | it has less and less and less of an impact
00:35:44.460 | in creating a sense of laughter, of humor in HM,
00:35:49.460 | not as just an anecdote to flesh out his story,
00:35:53.280 | but because emotion itself turns out to be the way
00:35:57.240 | in which we can enhance memories,
00:35:59.620 | even if those are memories for things that are not funny,
00:36:02.760 | are not intensely sad, are not immensely happy,
00:36:07.120 | or don't evoke a really strong emotional response
00:36:10.160 | or even any emotional response.
00:36:12.980 | And the reason for that is that emotions,
00:36:15.760 | just like perception, just like sensation,
00:36:18.620 | are the consequence of particular neurochemicals
00:36:21.080 | being present in our brain and body.
00:36:22.720 | And as I'm going to tell you next,
00:36:24.780 | there are particular neurochemicals
00:36:26.920 | that you can leverage in order
00:36:28.600 | to learn specific information faster
00:36:31.280 | and to remember it for a much longer period of time,
00:36:34.160 | maybe even forever.
00:36:35.520 | And you can do that by leveraging the relationship
00:36:37.880 | in your nervous system between your brain and your body
00:36:41.500 | and your body back to your brain.
00:36:44.240 | So let's talk about tools for enhancing memory.
00:36:46.840 | Now there's one tool that is absolutely clear works,
00:36:51.360 | and it's always worked, it works now,
00:36:53.860 | and it will work forever.
00:36:56.240 | And that's repetition.
00:36:57.480 | The more often that you perform something
00:37:00.480 | or that you recite something,
00:37:02.340 | the more likely you are to remember it in the future.
00:37:05.680 | And while that might seem obvious,
00:37:07.380 | it's worth thinking about what's happening
00:37:09.580 | when you repeat something.
00:37:11.000 | But when I say what's happening, I mean at the neural level.
00:37:13.840 | What's happening is that you're encouraging the firing
00:37:16.660 | of particular chains of neurons
00:37:18.880 | that reside in a particular circuit, right?
00:37:21.180 | So a particular sequence of neurons playing neuron A, B, C, D
00:37:24.960 | played in that particular sequence
00:37:26.120 | over and over and over again.
00:37:27.920 | And with more repetitions,
00:37:29.440 | you get more strengthening of those nerve connections.
00:37:33.300 | Now, repetition works, but the problem for most people
00:37:37.680 | is that they either don't have the patience,
00:37:40.020 | they don't have the time,
00:37:40.880 | and sometimes they literally don't have the time
00:37:42.400 | because they've got a deadline
00:37:44.120 | on something that they're trying to remember and learn,
00:37:47.220 | or they simply would like to be able
00:37:49.380 | to remember things better in general,
00:37:51.780 | remember them more quickly.
00:37:53.820 | This process of accelerating repetition-based learning
00:37:57.640 | so that your learning curve doesn't go
00:37:59.680 | from having to perform something 1,000 times
00:38:02.580 | and then gradually over time, it's 1,750 times a day,
00:38:06.320 | 500 times a day, 300 times a day,
00:38:08.560 | and down to no repetitions, right?
00:38:10.400 | You can just perform that thing
00:38:12.000 | the first time and every time.
00:38:13.800 | Well, there is a way to shift that curve
00:38:16.820 | so that you can essentially establish stronger connections
00:38:20.920 | between the neurons that are involved
00:38:22.840 | in generating that memory or behavior more quickly.
00:38:26.800 | How do you do that?
00:38:27.660 | Well, in order to answer that,
00:38:29.220 | we have to look at the beautiful work
00:38:31.760 | of James McGaugh and Larry Cahill.
00:38:34.680 | James McGaugh and Larry Cahill did a number of experiments
00:38:37.260 | over several decades, really,
00:38:39.240 | based on a lot of animal literature,
00:38:41.460 | but mainly focused on humans
00:38:44.140 | that really established what's required
00:38:46.960 | to get better at remembering things
00:38:49.600 | and to do so very quickly.
00:38:51.620 | I want to talk about one experiment that they did
00:38:53.860 | that was particularly important,
00:38:55.980 | and we will provide a link to this paper.
00:38:57.540 | It's some years old now, but the results still hold up.
00:39:00.700 | In fact, the results establish an entire field
00:39:03.420 | of memory in neuroscience and psychology.
00:39:06.000 | What they did is they had human subjects
00:39:08.900 | come into the laboratory and to read a short paragraph
00:39:12.060 | of about 12 sentences,
00:39:14.440 | and the key thing is that some subjects read a paragraph
00:39:18.620 | that was pretty mundane.
00:39:20.920 | The content, the information within the paragraph
00:39:23.460 | was all related to the content of the previous sentence,
00:39:26.340 | so it was a cogent paragraph, right?
00:39:27.980 | It just wasn't meaningless scramble of words,
00:39:30.980 | but it described a kind of mundane set of circumstances.
00:39:35.140 | Maybe it would be a story about someone
00:39:36.740 | who walked into a room, sat down at a desk,
00:39:39.520 | wrote for a little bit, then got up and had lunch,
00:39:42.080 | just kind of mundane information, not very interesting.
00:39:45.700 | Another group of subjects read also
00:39:48.620 | a 12-sentence paragraph, but that paragraph
00:39:51.940 | included a subset of sentences
00:39:54.600 | that had a lot of emotionally intense language,
00:39:58.040 | or that had language that could evoke
00:40:00.520 | an emotionally intense response in the person reading it.
00:40:03.320 | So it might've talked about a car accident
00:40:05.420 | or a very intense surgery,
00:40:07.600 | but it also could be positive stuff,
00:40:09.740 | things like a birthday party
00:40:12.240 | or a celebration of some other kind or a big sports win.
00:40:15.560 | So in other words, you have two conditions of this study,
00:40:18.420 | people either read a boring paragraph
00:40:20.700 | or they read a really emotionally laden paragraph.
00:40:23.500 | And again, the emotions could either be positive
00:40:25.700 | or negative emotions.
00:40:27.900 | Subjects left the laboratory and sometime later,
00:40:30.300 | they were called back to the laboratory.
00:40:32.040 | And I should say at no point in the experiment
00:40:34.700 | did they know they were part of a memory experiment, okay?
00:40:37.040 | They don't know why they're reading this paragraph.
00:40:39.020 | They came in either for class credit or to get paid.
00:40:41.700 | It's typically how these things are done
00:40:42.900 | on college campuses or elsewhere.
00:40:45.180 | They come back into the lab
00:40:46.860 | and they would get a pop quiz.
00:40:49.620 | They would be asked to recall the content of the paragraph
00:40:53.520 | that they had read previously.
00:40:55.700 | As is probably expected, perhaps even obvious to you,
00:40:58.880 | the subjects that read the emotionally intense paragraph
00:41:03.700 | remembered far more of the content of that paragraph
00:41:06.500 | and were far more accurate
00:41:08.900 | in their remembering of that information.
00:41:11.660 | Now, that particular finding wasn't very novel.
00:41:15.460 | Many people had previously described
00:41:17.380 | how emotionally intense events are better remembered
00:41:20.540 | than non-emotionally intense events.
00:41:22.780 | In fact, way back in the 1600s, Francis Bacon,
00:41:25.600 | who's largely credited with developing the scientific method
00:41:29.320 | said, quote, "Memory is assisted by anything
00:41:32.040 | "that makes an impression on a powerful passion,
00:41:34.200 | "inspiring fear, for example, or wonder, shame, or joy."
00:41:38.400 | Francis Bacon said that in 1620.
00:41:40.860 | So Jim McGaugh and Larry Cahill
00:41:42.700 | were certainly not the first to demonstrate
00:41:46.180 | or to conceive of the idea
00:41:47.700 | that emotionally laden experiences
00:41:50.060 | are more easily remembered than other experiences.
00:41:53.460 | However, what they did next was immensely important
00:41:56.800 | for our understanding of memory
00:41:58.300 | and for our building of tools
00:42:00.900 | to enhance learning and memory.
00:42:03.400 | What they did was they evaluated the capacity for stress
00:42:08.220 | and for particular neurochemicals associated with stress
00:42:11.460 | to improve our ability to learn information,
00:42:14.380 | not just information that is emotional,
00:42:17.620 | but information of all kinds.
00:42:19.940 | So I'm going to describe some experiments
00:42:21.380 | done in animal models just very briefly,
00:42:23.420 | and then experiments done on human subjects,
00:42:26.620 | because McGaugh worked mainly on animals,
00:42:28.900 | also human subjects,
00:42:30.080 | Larry Cahill almost exclusively on human subjects.
00:42:32.960 | If you take a rat or a mouse and put it in an arena
00:42:38.480 | where at one location the animal receives an electrical shock
00:42:42.440 | and then you come back the next day,
00:42:44.600 | you remove the shock evoking device
00:42:47.280 | and you let the animal move around that arena,
00:42:49.480 | that animal will quite understandably avoid the location
00:42:52.600 | where it was shocked, so-called conditioned place aversion.
00:42:56.300 | That effect of avoiding that particular location
00:43:00.640 | occurs in one trial.
00:43:01.780 | That's a good example of one trial learning.
00:43:03.920 | So somehow the animal knows
00:43:05.960 | that it was shocked at that location.
00:43:08.160 | It remembers that it is a hippocampal dependent learning.
00:43:12.260 | So animals that lack a hippocampus
00:43:14.160 | or who have their hippocampus pharmacologically
00:43:16.240 | or otherwise incapacitated
00:43:18.600 | will not learn that new bit of information.
00:43:21.880 | But for animals that do,
00:43:24.060 | they remember it after the first time and every time,
00:43:27.680 | unless you are to block the release of certain chemicals
00:43:32.080 | in the brain and body.
00:43:32.960 | And the chemicals I'm referring to are epinephrine,
00:43:36.280 | adrenaline, and to some extent the corticosterones,
00:43:40.080 | things like cortisol.
00:43:41.720 | Now we know that the effect of getting one trial learning
00:43:44.360 | somehow involves epinephrine,
00:43:46.320 | at least in this particular experimental scenario,
00:43:49.160 | because if researchers do the exact same experiment
00:43:52.880 | and they have done the exact same experiment,
00:43:54.860 | but they introduce a pharmacological blocker of epinephrine
00:43:59.400 | so that epinephrine is released in response to the shock,
00:44:02.440 | but it cannot actually bind to its receptors
00:44:05.200 | and have all of its biological effects.
00:44:07.360 | Well, then the animal is perfectly happy to tread back
00:44:10.400 | into the area where it received the shock.
00:44:12.680 | It's almost as if it didn't know,
00:44:14.280 | or we have to assume that didn't remember
00:44:17.140 | that it received the shock at that location.
00:44:19.680 | So it all seems pretty obvious when you hear it,
00:44:21.380 | something bad happens in a location,
00:44:22.640 | you don't go back to that location.
00:44:24.140 | So that's conditioned place avoidance,
00:44:26.380 | but it turns out that the opposite is also true,
00:44:29.160 | meaning for something called conditioned place preference,
00:44:32.400 | you can take an animal, put it into an arena,
00:44:34.760 | feed it or reward it somehow at one location in that arena.
00:44:38.760 | So you can give a hungry rat or mouse food
00:44:41.040 | at one particular location, take the animal out,
00:44:44.280 | come back the next day, no food is introduced,
00:44:46.580 | but it'll go back to the location where it received the food
00:44:49.160 | or you can do any variant of this.
00:44:50.680 | You can make the arena a little bit chilly
00:44:52.240 | and provide warmth at that location,
00:44:54.260 | or you can take a male animal and turns out male rats
00:44:59.040 | and mice will mate at any point or a female animal
00:45:01.360 | that's at the particular so-called receptive phase
00:45:03.200 | of her mating cycle and give them an opportunity
00:45:05.320 | to mate at a given location,
00:45:06.520 | they'll go back to that location and wait and wait.
00:45:08.180 | This is perhaps why people go back to the same bar,
00:45:10.520 | the bar, seat at the bar or the same restaurant
00:45:12.560 | and wait because of the one time they, you know,
00:45:14.880 | things worked out for them, whatever the context was.
00:45:18.240 | Conditioned place preference.
00:45:19.880 | Conditioned place preference,
00:45:22.960 | as with conditioned place avoidance,
00:45:25.200 | depends on the release of adrenaline, right?
00:45:28.240 | It's not just about stress,
00:45:30.200 | it's about a heightened emotional state in the brain
00:45:33.480 | and body, okay, this is really important.
00:45:35.460 | It's not just about stress.
00:45:37.240 | You can get one trial learning for positive events,
00:45:39.520 | conditioned place preference,
00:45:40.920 | and you can get one trial learning for negative events.
00:45:45.560 | Here I say positive, negative,
00:45:47.280 | I'm putting what's called valence on,
00:45:48.720 | I'm making a value judgment about whether or not
00:45:50.880 | the animal liked it or didn't like it,
00:45:52.340 | and we have to presume what the animal liked or didn't like
00:45:55.200 | and how it felt,
00:45:56.040 | but this turns out all to be true for humans as well.
00:45:59.840 | We know that because McGaugh and Cahill did experiments
00:46:03.420 | where they gave people a boring paragraph to read
00:46:07.000 | and only a boring paragraph to read,
00:46:09.360 | but one group of subjects was asked to read the paragraph
00:46:13.320 | and then to place their arm into very, very cold water.
00:46:17.600 | In fact, it was ice water.
00:46:19.160 | We know that placing one's arm into ice water,
00:46:21.520 | especially if it's up to the shoulder or near to it,
00:46:25.080 | evokes the release of adrenaline in the body.
00:46:27.480 | It's not an enormous release,
00:46:28.780 | but it's a significant increase.
00:46:30.360 | And yes, they measured adrenaline release.
00:46:32.920 | In some cases,
00:46:33.760 | they also measured for things like cortisol, et cetera.
00:46:36.440 | And what they found is that if one evokes the release
00:46:40.980 | of adrenaline through this arm into ice water approach,
00:46:45.760 | the information that they read previously,
00:46:47.880 | just a few minutes before, was remembered.
00:46:51.120 | It was retained as well as emotionally intense information,
00:46:55.400 | but keep in mind that information that they read
00:46:57.060 | was not interesting at all,
00:46:58.120 | or at least it wasn't emotionally laden.
00:47:00.980 | This had to be the effect of adrenaline released
00:47:03.800 | into the brain and body,
00:47:05.180 | because if they blocked the release or the function
00:47:09.840 | of adrenaline in the brain and or body,
00:47:13.260 | they could block this effect.
00:47:14.620 | Now, the biology of epinephrine and cortisol
00:47:17.840 | are a little bit complex,
00:47:18.900 | but there's some nuance there that's actually interesting
00:47:21.080 | and important to us.
00:47:22.020 | First of all, adrenaline is released in the body
00:47:25.300 | and in the brain.
00:47:26.140 | It's released in the body from the adrenals,
00:47:28.060 | remember epinephrine and adrenaline are the same thing.
00:47:31.040 | Cortisol is also released from the adrenal glands,
00:47:33.100 | these two little glands that ride atop our kidneys,
00:47:35.600 | but it can't cross into the brain.
00:47:37.460 | It only has what we call peripheral effects,
00:47:39.280 | quickening of the heart rate, right?
00:47:41.080 | Changes the patterns of blood flow,
00:47:43.640 | changes our patterns of breathing,
00:47:45.040 | in general makes our breathing more shallow and faster,
00:47:47.800 | in general makes our heartbeat more quickly, et cetera.
00:47:51.360 | Within our brain, we have a little brain area
00:47:53.240 | called locus coeruleus, which is in the back of the brain,
00:47:55.620 | which has the opportunity to sprinkler the rest of the brain
00:47:58.600 | with the neuromodulator epinephrine, adrenaline,
00:48:01.360 | as well as norepinephrine, a related neuromodulator,
00:48:04.240 | and to essentially wake up or create a state of alertness
00:48:07.520 | throughout the brain, so it's a very general effect.
00:48:10.600 | The reason we have two sites of release
00:48:12.840 | is because these neurochemicals
00:48:15.760 | do not cross the blood-brain barrier,
00:48:17.900 | and so waking up the body with adrenaline
00:48:20.520 | and waking up the brain are two separate
00:48:22.360 | so-called parallel phenomena.
00:48:24.500 | Cortisol can cross the blood-brain barrier
00:48:27.200 | because it's lipophilic,
00:48:28.600 | meaning it can move through fatty tissue,
00:48:31.640 | and we'll get into the biology of that in another episode,
00:48:34.240 | but cortisol in general is released
00:48:36.280 | and has much longer term effects,
00:48:37.920 | and as I've just told you,
00:48:38.960 | can permeate throughout the brain and body.
00:48:40.760 | Adrenaline has more local effects,
00:48:42.380 | or at least is segregated between the brain and the body.
00:48:45.220 | This will turn out to be important later.
00:48:47.280 | The important thing to keep in mind
00:48:48.480 | is that it is the emotionality evoked by an experience,
00:48:53.020 | or to be more precise,
00:48:55.020 | it is the emotional state that you are in
00:48:57.620 | after you experience something
00:49:00.960 | that dictates whether or not
00:49:02.200 | you will learn it quickly or not.
00:49:04.600 | This is absolutely important
00:49:06.880 | in terms of thinking about tools to improve your memory,
00:49:09.520 | and no, I am not going to suggest
00:49:11.320 | that every time you want to learn something,
00:49:12.620 | you plunge your arm into ice water.
00:49:15.200 | Why won't I suggest that?
00:49:16.480 | Well, it will induce the release of adrenaline,
00:49:20.600 | but there are better ways to get that adrenaline release.
00:49:22.980 | Before I explain exactly what those tools are,
00:49:25.460 | I want to tamp down the biology of how all this works,
00:49:28.560 | because in that understanding,
00:49:30.580 | you will have access to the best possible tools
00:49:33.400 | to improve your memory.
00:49:35.240 | First of all, McGaugh and Cahill
00:49:37.360 | were excellent experimentalists.
00:49:38.880 | They did not just establish
00:49:40.820 | that you could quicken the formation of a memory
00:49:44.720 | by accessing material that was very emotionally laden
00:49:48.000 | or creating an emotional high adrenaline state
00:49:51.120 | after interacting with some thing,
00:49:54.320 | some word, some person, some information.
00:49:57.160 | They also tested whether or not
00:49:58.960 | that whole effect could be blocked
00:50:00.900 | by blocking the emotional state or by blocking adrenaline.
00:50:04.160 | So what they did is they had people read paragraphs
00:50:05.880 | that either had a lot of emotional content
00:50:09.000 | or they had people read paragraphs that were pretty boring,
00:50:11.980 | but then had them put their arm into ice water.
00:50:14.600 | And I should say they did other experiments too
00:50:16.320 | to increase adrenaline.
00:50:17.400 | There were even some shock experiments
00:50:18.700 | that were done by other groups,
00:50:20.240 | any number of things to evoke the release of adrenaline,
00:50:22.180 | even people taking drugs that increase adrenaline.
00:50:24.720 | But then they also did what are called blocking experiments.
00:50:29.020 | They did experiments where they had people
00:50:31.340 | get into a highly emotional state
00:50:33.280 | from reading highly emotional material,
00:50:35.640 | or they got people to get into
00:50:36.800 | a highly emotional neurochemical state
00:50:38.600 | by reading boring material
00:50:39.880 | and then taking a drug to increase adrenaline
00:50:42.480 | or ice bath or a shock.
00:50:44.320 | And then they also administered a drug
00:50:47.120 | called a beta blocker to block the effect of adrenaline
00:50:50.840 | and related chemicals in the brain and body.
00:50:52.960 | And what they found is that
00:50:55.360 | even if people were exposed to something really emotional
00:50:58.920 | or had a lot of adrenaline in their system
00:51:02.200 | because they received a drug
00:51:03.240 | to increase the amount of adrenaline,
00:51:04.520 | two manipulations that normally would increase memory,
00:51:06.640 | keep that in mind,
00:51:08.040 | if they gave them a beta blocker,
00:51:10.480 | which reduced the response to that adrenaline, right?
00:51:13.540 | So no quickening of the heart rate,
00:51:14.840 | no quickening of the breathing,
00:51:16.840 | no increase in the activity of locus coeruleus
00:51:20.080 | and these kind of wake up signals to the rest of the brain.
00:51:22.700 | Well, then the material wasn't remembered better at all.
00:51:26.000 | What this tells us is that yes, Francis Bacon was right.
00:51:29.080 | McGaugh and Cahill were right.
00:51:30.560 | Hundreds, if not thousands of philosophers
00:51:32.680 | and psychologists and neuroscientists were right
00:51:35.680 | in stating and in thinking that high emotional states
00:51:40.380 | help you learn things.
00:51:42.060 | But what McGaugh and Cahill really showed
00:51:45.000 | and what's most important to know
00:51:47.000 | is that it is the presence of high adrenaline,
00:51:51.840 | high amounts of norepinephrine and epinephrine
00:51:54.600 | and perhaps cortisol as well, as you'll soon see,
00:51:58.160 | that allows a memory to be stamped down quickly.
00:52:01.920 | It is not the emotion.
00:52:05.360 | It is the neurochemical state that you go into
00:52:08.120 | as a consequence of the emotion.
00:52:10.480 | And it's very important to understand
00:52:11.720 | that while those two things are related,
00:52:13.400 | they are not one and the same thing.
00:52:15.740 | Because what that means is that were you to evoke
00:52:20.040 | the release of epinephrine, norepinephrine and cortisol,
00:52:23.640 | or even just one or two of those chemicals,
00:52:27.100 | after experiencing something,
00:52:29.200 | you are stamping down the experience
00:52:32.120 | that you just previously had.
00:52:34.200 | This is fundamentally important and far and away different
00:52:38.120 | than the idea that we remember things
00:52:41.280 | because they are important to us
00:52:42.840 | or because they evoke emotion.
00:52:44.240 | That's true, but the real reason, the neurochemical reason,
00:52:47.620 | the mechanism behind all that
00:52:49.560 | is these neurochemicals have the ability
00:52:52.480 | to strengthen neural connections
00:52:54.720 | by making them active just once.
00:52:57.440 | There's something truly magic
00:52:58.920 | about that neurochemical cocktail
00:53:00.740 | that removes the need for repetition.
00:53:03.180 | Okay, so let's apply this knowledge.
00:53:04.720 | Let's establish a scientifically grounded set of tools,
00:53:08.240 | meaning tools that take into account
00:53:10.880 | the identity of the neurochemicals
00:53:13.040 | that are important for enhancing learning
00:53:15.520 | and the timing of the release of those chemicals
00:53:18.060 | in order to enhance learning.
00:53:20.220 | When I first learned about the results of McGaugh and Cahill,
00:53:23.520 | I was just blown away.
00:53:25.660 | I was also pretty upset, but not with them.
00:53:28.360 | I was upset with myself because I realized
00:53:30.440 | that the way that I'd been approaching learning and memory
00:53:33.000 | was not optimal.
00:53:34.480 | In fact, it was probably in the opposite direction
00:53:37.780 | to the enhanced protocol for learning and memory
00:53:40.680 | that I'm going to teach you today.
00:53:43.080 | My typical mode of trying to learn something
00:53:44.920 | while I was in college or while I was in graduate school,
00:53:47.960 | or as a junior professor or even a tenured professor
00:53:51.200 | was to sit down to whatever it is
00:53:53.600 | I was going to try and learn, perhaps even memorize,
00:53:56.600 | or if it was a physical skill,
00:53:58.040 | move to whatever environment
00:53:59.540 | I was going to learn that physical skill in.
00:54:01.520 | And prior to that, to make sure that I was hydrated,
00:54:05.680 | 'cause that's important to me,
00:54:06.880 | and certainly can contribute to your brain's ability
00:54:09.720 | to function and your body's ability to function
00:54:11.960 | and general patterns of alertness, but also to caffeinate.
00:54:16.080 | I would have a nice strong cup of coffee or espresso.
00:54:19.420 | I would have a nice strong cup of yerba mate.
00:54:22.020 | And I still drink coffee or yerba mate very regularly.
00:54:27.000 | I drink them in moderation, I think, certainly for me,
00:54:30.360 | but typically I would drink those things
00:54:33.520 | before I would engage in any kind of attempt
00:54:36.220 | to learn or memorize or to acquire a new skill.
00:54:39.240 | Now, caffeine in the form of coffee or yerba mate
00:54:43.120 | or any other form of caffeine
00:54:45.120 | does create a sense of alertness in our brain and body.
00:54:47.400 | And it does that through two major mechanisms.
00:54:49.680 | The first mechanism is by blocking the effects of adenosine.
00:54:54.080 | Adenosine is a molecule that builds up in the brain and body
00:54:56.360 | the longer that we are awake.
00:54:57.880 | And it's largely what's responsible
00:54:59.600 | for our feelings of sleepiness and fatigue
00:55:02.140 | when we've been awake for a very long time.
00:55:05.240 | Caffeine essentially acts to block the effects of adenosine.
00:55:10.240 | It's a competing agonist, not to get technical,
00:55:13.600 | but it binds to the receptor for adenosine
00:55:16.240 | for some period of time and prevents adenosine
00:55:18.120 | from having its normal pattern of action
00:55:20.120 | and thereby reduces our feelings of fatigue.
00:55:23.800 | But it also increases state of alertness.
00:55:26.580 | So while it's reducing fatigue,
00:55:28.800 | it's also pushing on neurochemical systems
00:55:31.000 | in order to directly increase our alertness.
00:55:32.720 | And it does that in large part by increasing
00:55:35.760 | the transmission of epinephrine,
00:55:37.600 | adrenaline in the brain and body.
00:55:39.560 | It also has this interesting effect
00:55:41.040 | of upregulating the number and or efficiency,
00:55:44.040 | or we say the efficacy of dopamine receptors,
00:55:48.180 | such that when dopamine is present
00:55:50.720 | and as a molecule that increases motivation
00:55:52.680 | and craving and pursuit,
00:55:54.080 | that dopamine can have a more potent effect
00:55:56.080 | than it would otherwise.
00:55:57.200 | So caffeine really hits these three systems.
00:55:59.240 | It hits other systems too,
00:56:00.240 | but it mainly reduces fatigue by reducing adenosine,
00:56:03.720 | increases alertness by increasing epinephrine release
00:56:07.200 | or adrenaline release,
00:56:08.200 | I should say both from the adrenals in your body
00:56:10.140 | and from locus coeruleus within the brain.
00:56:12.720 | And it can in parallel to all that increase the action
00:56:16.920 | or the efficacy of the action of dopamine.
00:56:20.160 | So my typical way of approaching learning and memory
00:56:22.780 | would be to drink some caffeine and then focus really hard
00:56:25.760 | on whatever it is that I'm trying to learn,
00:56:27.480 | trying to eliminate distractions and then hope, hope, hope,
00:56:31.440 | or try, try, try to remember that information
00:56:33.800 | as best as I could.
00:56:35.440 | And frankly, I felt like it was working pretty well for me.
00:56:37.680 | And typically if I leveraged other forms of pharmacology
00:56:40.420 | in order to enhance learning and memory,
00:56:43.100 | things like alpha GPC or phosphatidylserine,
00:56:47.440 | I would do that by taking those things
00:56:50.160 | before I sat down to learn a particular set of information
00:56:54.000 | or before I went off to learn a particular physical skill.
00:56:58.040 | Now, for those of you out there listening to this,
00:57:00.260 | you're probably thinking, well, okay,
00:57:03.120 | the results of McGaugh and Cahill pointed to the fact
00:57:05.880 | that having adrenaline released after learning something
00:57:10.560 | enhanced learning of that thing.
00:57:12.540 | But a lot of these things like caffeine or alpha GPC
00:57:15.880 | can increase epinephrine and adrenaline or dopamine
00:57:20.080 | or other molecules in the brain and body
00:57:23.440 | that can enhance memory for a long period of time.
00:57:25.700 | So it makes sense to take it first or even during learning
00:57:28.720 | and then allow that increase to occur.
00:57:31.020 | And the increase will occur over a long period of time
00:57:33.040 | and will enhance learning and memory.
00:57:35.260 | And while that is partially true, it is not entirely true.
00:57:39.560 | And it turns out it's not optimal.
00:57:41.820 | Work that was done by the McGaugh laboratory
00:57:44.160 | and other laboratories evaluated
00:57:47.400 | the precise temporal relationship
00:57:50.140 | between neurochemical activation of these pathways
00:57:54.100 | and learning and memory.
00:57:55.280 | What they did is they had animals and or people,
00:57:57.540 | depending on the experiment, take a drug, could be caffeine,
00:58:01.520 | could be in pill form,
00:58:03.620 | something that would increase adrenaline
00:58:06.700 | or related molecules that create this state of alertness
00:58:10.180 | that are related to emotionality.
00:58:12.100 | And they had them do it either an hour before,
00:58:14.420 | 30 minutes before, 10 minutes before,
00:58:17.380 | five minutes before learning
00:58:20.100 | or during the bout of learning, right?
00:58:23.100 | The reading of the information
00:58:24.240 | or the performing of the skill that one is trying to learn,
00:58:26.220 | or five minutes, 10 minutes, 15 minutes,
00:58:28.820 | 30 minutes, et cetera, afterwards.
00:58:30.900 | So they looked very precisely at when exactly is best
00:58:33.980 | to evoke this adrenaline release.
00:58:35.940 | And it turns out that the best time window
00:58:38.260 | to evoke the release of these chemicals,
00:58:40.660 | if the goal is to enhance learning
00:58:42.820 | and memory of the material is either immediately after
00:58:46.660 | or just a few minutes, five, 10, maybe 15 minutes
00:58:50.840 | after you're repeating that information,
00:58:53.920 | you're trying to learn that information.
00:58:55.380 | Again, this could be cognitive information
00:58:57.180 | or this could be a physical skill.
00:58:59.200 | Now, this really spits in the face of the way
00:59:01.680 | that most of us approach learning and memory.
00:59:03.980 | Most of us, if we use stimulants like caffeine or alpha GPC,
00:59:08.980 | we're taking those before or during an attempt to learn,
00:59:13.140 | not afterwards.
00:59:15.460 | These results point to the fact
00:59:16.940 | that it is after the learning and memory
00:59:19.800 | that you really want to get that big increase
00:59:22.140 | in epinephrine and the related molecules
00:59:24.460 | that will tamp down memory.
00:59:25.880 | So what this means is that if you are currently using
00:59:28.540 | caffeine or other compounds,
00:59:30.540 | and we'll talk about what those are
00:59:31.840 | and safety issues and so forth in a moment,
00:59:34.620 | if you're using those compounds
00:59:36.780 | in order to enhance learning and memory
00:59:38.420 | by taking them before or during a learning episode,
00:59:42.560 | well, then I encourage you to try and take them either late
00:59:46.040 | in the learning episode
00:59:47.340 | or immediately after the learning episode.
00:59:49.880 | Now, given everything I've told you up until now,
00:59:51.560 | why would I say late in the learning episode
00:59:53.440 | or immediately after?
00:59:54.560 | Well, when you ingest something by drinking it
00:59:57.160 | or you take it in capsule form,
00:59:58.680 | there's a period of time
00:59:59.520 | before that gets absorbed into the body
01:00:01.040 | and different substances such as caffeine,
01:00:03.800 | alpha GPC, et cetera,
01:00:05.160 | are absorbed in from the gut and into the bloodstream
01:00:08.040 | and reach the brain and trigger these effects
01:00:10.100 | in the brain and body at different rates.
01:00:11.600 | And so it's not instantaneous.
01:00:13.280 | Some have effects within minutes,
01:00:14.880 | others within tens of minutes and so on.
01:00:18.320 | It's really going to depend on the pharmacology
01:00:20.600 | of those things.
01:00:21.440 | And it's also going to depend on whether or not
01:00:22.720 | you have food in your gut,
01:00:23.720 | what else you happen to have circulating
01:00:25.160 | in your bloodstream, et cetera.
01:00:26.800 | But at a very basic level,
01:00:28.240 | we can confidently say that there are not one, not dozens,
01:00:32.640 | but as I mentioned before,
01:00:33.680 | hundreds of studies in animals and in humans
01:00:35.880 | that point to the fact that triggering the increase
01:00:37.740 | of adrenaline late in learning
01:00:39.960 | or immediately after learning is going to be most beneficial
01:00:42.640 | if your goal is to retain that information
01:00:44.760 | for some period of time
01:00:45.920 | and to reduce the number of repetitions required
01:00:48.140 | in order to learn that information.
01:00:49.920 | Now, I want to acknowledge that on previous episodes
01:00:52.340 | of this podcast and in appearing on other podcasts,
01:00:56.120 | I've talked a lot about things like non-sleep deep rest
01:00:58.920 | and naps and sleep as vital to the learning process.
01:01:01.960 | And I want to emphasize
01:01:02.960 | that none of that information has changed, right?
01:01:05.420 | I don't look at any of that information differently
01:01:07.320 | as the consequence of what I'm talking about today.
01:01:09.760 | It is still true that the strengthening of connections
01:01:12.680 | in the brain, the literal neuroplasticity,
01:01:15.460 | the changing of the circuits occurs during deep sleep
01:01:17.840 | and non-sleep deep rest.
01:01:19.680 | And it is also true,
01:01:20.960 | and I've mentioned these results earlier,
01:01:22.840 | that two papers were published in Cell Reports,
01:01:25.720 | Cell Press Journal, excellent journal
01:01:27.480 | over the last few years showing that brief naps
01:01:30.840 | of about 20 to up to 90 minutes in some period of time
01:01:35.800 | after an attempt to learn can enhance the rate of learning
01:01:39.720 | and memory.
01:01:41.360 | However, those bouts of sleep,
01:01:44.720 | the deep sleep that night, I should say,
01:01:46.320 | or those brief naps, or even the so-called NSDR,
01:01:49.680 | as we call it, non-sleep deep rest that was used
01:01:52.200 | to enhance the learning and memory
01:01:54.800 | of particular pieces of information,
01:01:57.360 | either cognitive or physical information or both,
01:02:00.960 | that still can be performed,
01:02:03.420 | but it can be performed some hours later, even an hour later
01:02:08.440 | it can be performed two hours later or four hours later.
01:02:11.020 | Remember, it's in these naps and in deep sleep
01:02:13.360 | that the actual reconfiguration of the neural circuits
01:02:15.700 | occurs, the strengthening of those neural circuits occurs.
01:02:18.940 | It is not the case that you need to finish a bout
01:02:21.440 | of learning and drop immediately into a nap or sleep.
01:02:23.520 | Some people might do that,
01:02:24.800 | but if you're really trying to optimize and enhance
01:02:27.300 | and improve your memory, the data from McGaugh and Cahill
01:02:31.080 | and many other laboratories that stemmed out
01:02:33.660 | from their initial work really point to the fact
01:02:36.120 | that the ideal protocol would be focus on the thing
01:02:40.040 | you're trying to learn very intensely.
01:02:41.600 | There are also some other things like error rates, et cetera.
01:02:44.220 | Please see our episodes on learning.
01:02:45.780 | We have a newsletter on how to learn better.
01:02:48.320 | You can access that at hubermanlab.com.
01:02:50.000 | It's a zero cost newsletter.
01:02:51.720 | You can grab that PDF.
01:02:53.240 | It lists out the things to do during the learning bout.
01:02:56.800 | Still try and get excellent sleep.
01:02:59.280 | Again, fundamentally important for mental health,
01:03:01.120 | physical health, and performance.
01:03:02.580 | And we can now extend from performance to saying,
01:03:05.300 | including learning and memory, nap,
01:03:08.540 | if it doesn't interrupt your nighttime sleep,
01:03:10.880 | naps of anywhere from 10 to 90 minutes
01:03:13.380 | or non-sleep-depress protocols
01:03:14.980 | will enhance learning and memory.
01:03:16.400 | But we can now add to that that spiking adrenaline,
01:03:20.720 | provided it can be done in a safe way,
01:03:22.800 | is going to reduce the number of repetitions
01:03:25.280 | required to learn.
01:03:26.120 | And that should be done at the very tail end
01:03:28.520 | or immediately after a learning bout,
01:03:30.720 | which is compatible with all the other protocols
01:03:33.360 | that I mentioned.
01:03:34.200 | And the reason I'm revisiting the stuff about sleep
01:03:36.200 | and non-sleep-depress
01:03:37.540 | is I think that some people got the impression
01:03:39.040 | that they need to do that immediately after learning.
01:03:41.080 | And today I'm saying to the contrary,
01:03:42.640 | immediately after learning,
01:03:43.780 | you need to go into a heightened state of emotionality
01:03:46.540 | and alertness.
01:03:47.400 | Now it's vitally important to point out
01:03:49.840 | that you do not need pharmacology.
01:03:52.560 | You don't need caffeine.
01:03:54.460 | You don't need alpha GPC.
01:03:56.680 | You don't need any pharmacologic substance
01:03:58.820 | to spike adrenaline,
01:04:00.620 | unless that's something that you already are doing
01:04:03.300 | or that you can do safely
01:04:05.000 | or that you know that you can do safely.
01:04:06.680 | And I always say, and I'll say it again,
01:04:09.160 | I'm not a physician, so I'm not prescribing anything.
01:04:11.120 | I'm a professor, so I profess things.
01:04:13.200 | You need to do what's safe for you.
01:04:14.800 | So if you're somebody who's not used to drinking caffeine
01:04:17.140 | and you suddenly drink four espresso
01:04:19.040 | after trying to learn something,
01:04:20.580 | you are going to have a severe increase in alertness
01:04:25.580 | and probably even anxiety.
01:04:26.760 | If you're panic attack prone,
01:04:28.260 | please don't start taking stimulants
01:04:30.240 | in order to learn things better.
01:04:31.720 | Please be safe.
01:04:32.560 | I don't just say that to protect me.
01:04:33.580 | I say that to protect you.
01:04:35.020 | And I should mention that if you're not accustomed
01:04:38.280 | to taking something,
01:04:39.520 | you always want to first check with your doctor, of course,
01:04:42.200 | but also move into that gradually, right?
01:04:45.040 | Start with the lowest effective dose,
01:04:47.700 | the minimal effective dose.
01:04:49.240 | And sometimes the minimal effective dose is zero milligrams.
01:04:53.240 | It's nothing.
01:04:54.400 | Why do I say that?
01:04:55.240 | Well, we already talked about results
01:04:57.480 | where they put people's arms into an ice bath
01:05:01.260 | in order to evoke adrenaline release.
01:05:02.880 | You are welcome to do that if you want.
01:05:04.320 | In fact, that's a pretty low cost, zero pharmacology,
01:05:07.640 | at least exogenous pharmacology way
01:05:09.280 | to approach this whole thing.
01:05:11.040 | That's a way of evoking your own natural epinephrine.
01:05:13.960 | And it turns out also dopamine release.
01:05:16.480 | You could take a cold shower.
01:05:18.200 | You could do an ice bath or get into a cold circulating bath.
01:05:21.880 | We've done several episodes on the utility of cold
01:05:24.920 | for health and performance.
01:05:25.800 | You can find those episodes at huberunelab.com.
01:05:28.080 | Also the episode with my colleague at Stanford
01:05:30.800 | from the biology department, Dr. Craig Heller,
01:05:33.060 | lots of protocols in particular,
01:05:34.780 | in the episode on cold for health and performance
01:05:37.700 | that describe how best to use the cold shower
01:05:41.980 | or the ice bath or the circulating cold bath
01:05:44.620 | in order to evoke epinephrine and dopamine release.
01:05:47.320 | The point is that the time in which you would want
01:05:50.980 | to do those protocols is after,
01:05:53.100 | ideally immediately after you're learning about,
01:05:56.280 | meaning when you're sitting down to learn new information
01:05:58.500 | or after trying to learn some new physical skill.
01:06:01.280 | Now, whether or not that's compatible
01:06:02.460 | with the other reasons you're doing deliberate cold exposure
01:06:05.880 | and whether or not that's compatible
01:06:07.280 | with the other things you're doing,
01:06:08.320 | that depends on the contour of your lifestyle,
01:06:10.080 | your training, your academic goals,
01:06:11.440 | your learning goals, et cetera.
01:06:12.840 | But if your specific purpose is to enhance learning
01:06:15.880 | and memory, you want to spike adrenaline afterwards.
01:06:17.920 | And so what I'm telling you is you can do that with caffeine.
01:06:20.680 | You can do that with alpha GPC.
01:06:22.780 | You can do that with a combination of caffeine and alpha GPC
01:06:25.640 | if you can do that safely.
01:06:27.360 | Some of you I know are using other forms of pharmacology.
01:06:29.780 | I did a long episode all about ADHD.
01:06:32.260 | I have to just really declare my stance very clearly
01:06:35.660 | that I am not a fan.
01:06:37.780 | I am actually opposed to people using prescription drugs
01:06:42.220 | who are not prescribed those drugs, right?
01:06:44.480 | In order to enhance alertness,
01:06:45.960 | I think there's a big addictive potential.
01:06:47.720 | There also is a potential to really disrupt
01:06:50.140 | one's own pharmacology around the dopaminergic system.
01:06:53.820 | However, some of you I know are prescribed things
01:06:57.000 | like Ritalin, Adderall, and modafinil and things of that sort
01:07:00.960 | in order to increase alertness and focus.
01:07:02.960 | So for those of you that are prescribed those things
01:07:04.820 | from a board certified physician,
01:07:07.680 | you're going to have to decide if you're going to take them
01:07:09.240 | before trying to learn or after trying to learn.
01:07:12.040 | You also have to take into consideration
01:07:13.380 | that some of those drugs are very long acting,
01:07:15.440 | some are shorter acting and time that
01:07:17.960 | according to what you're trying to learn and when.
01:07:20.200 | So that's pharmacology.
01:07:21.800 | But as I've mentioned, there are the behavioral protocols.
01:07:24.800 | You can use cold and cold is an excellent stimulus
01:07:28.800 | because first of all, it doesn't involve pharmacology.
01:07:32.880 | Second of all, you can generally access it
01:07:35.680 | at low to zero cost, especially the cold shower approach.
01:07:39.360 | And third, you can titrate it.
01:07:41.960 | You can start with warmer water.
01:07:43.680 | You can make it very, very cold if that's your thing
01:07:45.980 | and you're able to tolerate that safely.
01:07:47.480 | You can make it moderately cold.
01:07:50.780 | How cold should it be in order to evoke adrenaline release?
01:07:53.560 | Well, it should be uncomfortably cold,
01:07:55.520 | but cold enough that you feel like you really want to get out
01:07:58.960 | but can stay in safely.
01:08:00.340 | That's going to evoke adrenaline release.
01:08:02.560 | If it quickens your breathing,
01:08:04.120 | if it makes you go wide-eyed,
01:08:06.040 | that's increasing adrenaline release.
01:08:07.520 | In fact, those effects of going wide-eyed
01:08:09.800 | and quickening of the breathing
01:08:10.800 | and the challenges and thinking clearly,
01:08:12.720 | those are the direct effects of adrenaline
01:08:14.360 | on your brain and body.
01:08:15.840 | And of course, there are other ways to increase adrenaline.
01:08:18.160 | You could go out for a hard run.
01:08:20.720 | You could do any number of things
01:08:22.820 | that would increase adrenaline in your body.
01:08:25.060 | Which things you choose is up to you.
01:08:27.440 | But from a very clear, solid grounding in research data,
01:08:32.400 | we can confidently say that spiking adrenaline
01:08:35.100 | after interacting with some material,
01:08:37.620 | physical or cognitive material that you're trying to learn
01:08:39.840 | is going to be the best time to spike that adrenaline.
01:08:42.380 | Now, I realize that I'm being a bit redundant today
01:08:44.240 | or perhaps a lot redundant in repeating over and over
01:08:47.700 | that the increase in epinephrine should occur
01:08:51.380 | either very late in an attempt to learn something
01:08:54.280 | or immediately after an attempt to learn something.
01:08:58.180 | I also want to emphasize the general contour
01:09:00.940 | of pharmacologic effects and of behavioral tools
01:09:03.640 | to create adrenaline.
01:09:04.880 | What do I mean by that sentence?
01:09:06.260 | What I mean is that McGaugh and colleagues
01:09:09.240 | explored a huge number of different compounds and approaches,
01:09:11.720 | everything from the hand into the ice bath
01:09:13.760 | to injecting adrenaline, to caffeine,
01:09:17.400 | to drugs that block the effects of adrenaline and caffeine,
01:09:22.320 | drugs like mucimol and picrotoxin, please don't take those.
01:09:25.160 | These are drugs that reduce or enhance
01:09:27.560 | the amount of adrenaline.
01:09:28.560 | And the overall takeaway is that anything
01:09:31.640 | that increases adrenaline will increase learning and memory
01:09:35.280 | and will reduce the number of repetitions
01:09:36.880 | required to learn something,
01:09:39.160 | regardless of whether or not that something
01:09:40.880 | has an emotional intensity or not.
01:09:44.800 | Provided that that spike in adrenaline occurs
01:09:47.740 | late in the learning or immediately after,
01:09:49.580 | and anything that reduces epinephrine and adrenaline
01:09:53.720 | will impair learning.
01:09:55.760 | And that's the key and novel piece of information
01:09:58.560 | that I'm adding now, which is if you're taking beta blockers,
01:10:01.960 | for instance, or if you're trying to learn something
01:10:05.560 | and it's not evoking much of an emotional response
01:10:08.960 | and you're not using any pharmacology or other methods
01:10:11.640 | to enhance adrenaline release after learning that thing,
01:10:14.780 | well, you're not going to learn it very well.
01:10:17.100 | In fact, McGaugh and Cahill did beautiful experiments
01:10:20.560 | in humans looking at how much adrenaline is increased
01:10:24.980 | by varying the emotional intensity of different things
01:10:29.280 | that they were trying to get people to learn
01:10:31.160 | or by changing the dosage of epinephrine
01:10:34.160 | or by changing the amount of epinephrine blocker
01:10:36.480 | that they injected, lots and lots of studies.
01:10:39.420 | The key thing to take away from those studies
01:10:41.160 | is that for some people,
01:10:42.680 | adrenaline was increased 600 to 700%.
01:10:46.280 | So six to seven fold over baseline
01:10:48.900 | in the amount of circulating epinephrine or adrenaline.
01:10:51.700 | And keep in mind, sometimes that increase
01:10:53.400 | was due to the actual thing they were trying to learn
01:10:55.500 | being very emotional, positive or negative emotion.
01:10:58.600 | And sometimes it was because they were using
01:11:00.260 | a pharmacologic approach or the ice bath approach.
01:11:02.900 | I don't think they ever used a cold shower approach,
01:11:04.560 | but that would have been a very effective one,
01:11:06.200 | we can be sure.
01:11:07.380 | However, other people had a zero to 10% increase
01:11:13.020 | or a very small increase in epinephrine.
01:11:15.140 | What we can confidently say on the basis of all those data
01:11:19.200 | is that the more epinephrine release,
01:11:22.660 | the better that people remembered the material.
01:11:25.820 | Over and over again, this was shown
01:11:27.200 | whether or not it was for cognitive materials,
01:11:29.100 | so learning a language, learning a passage of words,
01:11:31.760 | learning mathematics,
01:11:33.120 | or whether or not it was for physical learning.
01:11:35.780 | I want to emphasize something about physical learning
01:11:37.700 | because I know a number of you
01:11:39.100 | are probably drinking a cup of coffee
01:11:41.100 | or having a cup of yerba mate
01:11:42.620 | or maybe even an energy drink
01:11:43.900 | and taking some alpha GPC or something
01:11:45.620 | before physical exercise.
01:11:48.300 | I'm not saying that's a bad thing to do
01:11:49.820 | or that you wouldn't want to do that,
01:11:51.020 | but that's really to increase alertness.
01:11:53.360 | It won't enhance learning, at least not as well
01:11:55.800 | as doing those things after the physical exercise.
01:11:59.060 | Now, again, many of you, including myself,
01:12:01.620 | exercise for sake of the physical benefits
01:12:03.580 | of that exercise, so cardiovascular resistance training,
01:12:06.500 | but we're not really focused on learning and memory.
01:12:09.860 | So I emphasize this
01:12:12.460 | just so it's immensely clear to everybody.
01:12:14.420 | If you want to use those approaches
01:12:16.800 | of increasing adrenaline prior to
01:12:18.940 | or during physical training
01:12:20.820 | or cognitive work for that matter, be my guest.
01:12:23.380 | I think that's perfectly fine provided that's safe for you.
01:12:26.300 | It's only by moving it to late or after the learning
01:12:29.840 | that you're really shifting the role
01:12:32.020 | of that adrenaline increase
01:12:34.060 | to enhancing memory specifically.
01:12:36.540 | And as a cautionary note,
01:12:38.340 | don't think that you can push this entire system
01:12:40.860 | to the extreme over and over again,
01:12:42.740 | or chronically as we say, and get away with it.
01:12:45.420 | In other words, you're not going to be able to take
01:12:48.380 | a alpha GPC and a double espresso,
01:12:52.180 | do your focus bout of work, cognitive or physical work,
01:12:56.120 | and then spike adrenaline again afterwards
01:12:58.780 | and remember that stuff even better, right?
01:13:00.580 | I'm not encouraging you.
01:13:01.700 | In fact, I'm discouraging you
01:13:03.640 | from chronically increasing adrenaline
01:13:06.960 | both during and after a given bout of work
01:13:11.860 | if the goal is to learn.
01:13:13.440 | Why do I say that?
01:13:14.380 | Well, work from McGaugh and Cahill and others
01:13:17.600 | has shown that it's not the absolute amount
01:13:21.380 | of adrenaline that you release in your brain and body
01:13:24.040 | that matters for enhancing memory.
01:13:26.560 | It's the amount of adrenaline that you release
01:13:29.680 | relative to the amount of adrenaline
01:13:32.240 | that was in your system just prior,
01:13:34.300 | in particular in the hour or two prior.
01:13:36.320 | So again, it's the delta, as we say, it's the difference.
01:13:38.920 | So if you're going to chronically increase adrenaline,
01:13:41.180 | you're not going to learn as well.
01:13:43.160 | The real key is to have adrenaline modestly low,
01:13:45.860 | perhaps even just as much as you need
01:13:48.140 | in order to be able to focus on something,
01:13:50.000 | pay attention to it, and then spike it afterwards.
01:13:52.860 | This is immensely important
01:13:54.900 | because while much of what we're talking about
01:13:58.220 | is actually a form of inducing a neurochemical acute stress,
01:14:01.980 | meaning a brief and rapid onset of stress,
01:14:06.300 | well, chronic stress,
01:14:08.460 | the chronic elevation of epinephrine and cortisol
01:14:11.860 | is actually detrimental to learning.
01:14:13.620 | And there's an entire category of literature,
01:14:16.500 | mainly from the work of the great,
01:14:18.380 | and sadly the late Bruce McEwen
01:14:20.660 | from the Rockefeller University
01:14:22.000 | and some of his scientific offspring,
01:14:23.580 | like the great Robert Sapolsky,
01:14:25.660 | showing that chronic stress,
01:14:27.780 | chronic elevation of epinephrine
01:14:29.340 | actually inhibits learning and memory
01:14:31.460 | and also can inhibit immune system function,
01:14:33.380 | whereas acute, right, sharp increases in adrenaline
01:14:37.700 | and cortisol actually can enhance learning
01:14:39.980 | and indeed can enhance the immune system.
01:14:41.920 | So if you really want to leverage this information,
01:14:44.780 | you might consider getting your brain and body
01:14:47.580 | into a very calm and yet alert state,
01:14:50.780 | so a high attentional state that will allow you
01:14:52.780 | to focus on what it is that you're trying to learn.
01:14:55.180 | We know focus is vital for encoding information
01:14:58.320 | and for triggering neuroplasticity,
01:15:00.260 | but remaining calm throughout that time
01:15:02.980 | and then afterwards spiking adrenaline
01:15:05.860 | and allowing adrenaline to have these incredible effects
01:15:08.740 | on reducing the number of repetitions required to learn.
01:15:11.620 | So if you're like me,
01:15:12.700 | you're learning about this information,
01:15:14.180 | this beautiful work of McGaugh and Cahill and others
01:15:16.660 | and thinking, wow, I should perhaps consider
01:15:19.740 | spiking my adrenaline in one form or another
01:15:23.060 | at the tail end or immediately following
01:15:25.320 | in attempt to learn something.
01:15:27.220 | And yet we are not the first to have this conversation
01:15:30.020 | nor were McGaugh and Cahill or any other researchers
01:15:33.020 | that I've discussed today,
01:15:34.260 | the first to start using this technique.
01:15:37.780 | In fact, there is a beautiful review
01:15:40.120 | that was published just this year, May of 2022
01:15:42.740 | in the journal Neuron, Cell Press Journal,
01:15:44.460 | excellent journal called "Mechanisms of Memory Under Stress."
01:15:48.980 | And I just want to read to you the first opening paragraph
01:15:52.120 | of this review, which is, as the name suggests,
01:15:54.380 | all about memory and stress.
01:15:56.760 | So here I'm reading and I quote,
01:15:58.660 | "In medieval times, communities threw young children
01:16:01.580 | in the river when they wanted them
01:16:02.900 | to remember important events.
01:16:04.860 | They believed that throwing a child in the water
01:16:06.700 | after witnessing historic proceedings
01:16:08.980 | would leave a lifelong memory for the events in the child."
01:16:12.540 | Believe it or not, this is true.
01:16:14.400 | This is a practice that somehow people arrived at.
01:16:18.980 | I don't know if they were aware of what adrenaline was,
01:16:22.180 | probably not, but somehow in medieval times,
01:16:26.040 | it was understood that spiking adrenaline
01:16:28.860 | or creating a robust emotional experience
01:16:32.160 | after an experience that one hoped a child would learn
01:16:36.720 | would encourage the child's nervous system,
01:16:39.040 | and they didn't even know what a nervous system was,
01:16:40.420 | but would encourage the brain and body of that child
01:16:43.440 | to remember those particular events.
01:16:45.860 | Very counterintuitive, if you ask me.
01:16:48.880 | I would have thought that the kid would remember
01:16:50.680 | only being thrown into the river.
01:16:52.120 | My guess is that they remembered that,
01:16:53.520 | but that the idea here anyway
01:16:55.440 | is that they also remember the things
01:16:56.820 | that preceded being thrown into the river.
01:16:59.140 | So both interesting and amusing,
01:17:02.540 | and somewhat, I should say, thought stimulating, really,
01:17:06.620 | that this is a practice that has been going on
01:17:09.620 | for many hundreds of years,
01:17:11.680 | and we are not the first to start thinking
01:17:14.140 | about using cold water as an adrenaline stimulus,
01:17:16.780 | nor are we the first to start thinking
01:17:18.100 | about using cold water-induced adrenaline
01:17:20.780 | as a way to enhance learning and memory.
01:17:22.580 | This has been happening since medieval times.
01:17:25.320 | So up until now, I've been talking about
01:17:27.180 | a pretty broad contour of these experiments.
01:17:29.880 | I've been talking about the underlying pharmacology,
01:17:31.960 | the role of epinephrine, and so forth.
01:17:33.320 | I haven't really talked a lot
01:17:34.420 | about the underlying neural mechanisms,
01:17:36.260 | so I'm just going to take a minute or two
01:17:37.320 | and describe those for you because they are informative.
01:17:40.800 | We all have a brain structure called the amygdala.
01:17:43.540 | A lot of people think it's associated with fear,
01:17:45.560 | but it's actually associated with threat detection,
01:17:48.820 | and more generally, and I should say more specifically,
01:17:52.140 | with detecting what sorts of events in the environment
01:17:55.580 | are novel and are linked to particular emotional states,
01:17:59.480 | both positive emotional states
01:18:01.060 | and negative emotional states.
01:18:03.040 | So the neurons in the amygdala are exquisitely good
01:18:06.120 | at figuring out, right, they don't have their own mind,
01:18:09.280 | but at detecting correlations between sensory events
01:18:13.160 | in the environment that trigger the release of adrenaline
01:18:15.620 | and what's going on in the brain,
01:18:17.220 | and because the amygdala is so extensively interconnected
01:18:19.920 | with other areas of the brain,
01:18:21.280 | it basically connects to everything,
01:18:22.420 | and everything connects back to it,
01:18:24.180 | the amygdala is in a position
01:18:26.960 | to strengthen particular connections in the brain
01:18:30.380 | very easily, provided certain conditions are met,
01:18:34.400 | and those conditions are the ones we've been talking about
01:18:36.080 | up until now, emotional saliency that results in increases
01:18:39.600 | in epinephrine and cortisol,
01:18:41.400 | or circulating epinephrine and cortisol being much higher
01:18:44.440 | than it was 10 minutes or 15 minutes before,
01:18:46.800 | and the net effect of the amygdala in this context
01:18:49.440 | is to take whatever patterns of neural activity
01:18:52.160 | preceded that increase in adrenaline and corticosterone
01:18:55.200 | and strengthen those synapses
01:18:57.320 | that were involved in that neural activity.
01:18:59.200 | So the amygdala doesn't have knowledge,
01:19:01.120 | it's not a thinking area, it's a correlation detector,
01:19:05.400 | and it's correlating neurochemical states
01:19:07.160 | of the brain and body with different patterns
01:19:08.960 | of electrical activity in the brain.
01:19:11.440 | This is important because it really emphasizes the fact
01:19:15.280 | that both negative and positive emotional states
01:19:19.320 | and the different but somewhat overlapping chemical states
01:19:24.320 | that they create are the conditions, as we say,
01:19:27.260 | the AND gates through which memory is laid down.
01:19:30.760 | AND gates will be familiar to those of you
01:19:32.520 | who have done a bit of computer programming.
01:19:35.120 | An AND gate is simply a condition in which you need one thing
01:19:40.120 | and another to happen in order for a third thing to happen.
01:19:44.400 | So you need epinephrine elevated
01:19:46.080 | and you need robust activity in a particular brain circuit
01:19:49.260 | if in fact that brain circuit is going to be strengthened.
01:19:52.520 | It's not sufficient to have one or the other,
01:19:54.820 | you need both, hence the name AND gate.
01:19:57.240 | And the amygdala is very good
01:19:58.640 | at establishing these AND gate contingencies.
01:20:01.480 | It's also a very generic brain structure
01:20:03.820 | in the sense that it doesn't really care
01:20:06.360 | what sorts of sensory events are involved
01:20:09.240 | provided they correlated in time
01:20:11.100 | with that increase in adrenaline and corticosterone.
01:20:14.140 | This has a wonderful side and a kind of dark side.
01:20:17.800 | The dark side is that PTSD and traumas of various kinds
01:20:22.800 | often involve a increase in adrenaline
01:20:27.060 | because whatever it was that caused the PTSD
01:20:29.900 | was indeed very stressful,
01:20:31.060 | caused these big increases in these chemicals,
01:20:33.140 | and because the amygdala is rather general
01:20:37.000 | in its functions, right?
01:20:37.840 | It's not tuned or designed in any kind of way
01:20:40.600 | to be specifically active in response
01:20:42.840 | to particular types of sensory events or perceptions.
01:20:47.060 | Well, then what it means is that we can start
01:20:49.260 | to become afraid of entire city blocks
01:20:53.060 | where one bad thing happened in a particular room
01:20:56.040 | of a particular building in a city block.
01:20:58.220 | We can become fearful of any place
01:21:01.140 | that contains a lot of people
01:21:02.520 | if something bad happened to us
01:21:04.260 | in a place that contained a lot of people.
01:21:06.440 | The amygdala is not so much of a splitter,
01:21:09.380 | as we say in science, we talk about lumpers and splitters.
01:21:11.820 | Lumpers are kind of generalizers, if that's even a word,
01:21:16.380 | and I think it is,
01:21:17.220 | someone will tell me one way or the other,
01:21:18.720 | and splitters are people that are ultra precise
01:21:23.200 | and specific and nuanced about every little detail.
01:21:26.480 | The amygdala is more of a lumper than a splitter
01:21:29.560 | when it comes to sensory events.
01:21:31.560 | Other areas of the brain only become active
01:21:33.660 | under very, very specific conditions
01:21:35.580 | and only those conditions.
01:21:36.780 | And similarly, epinephrine is just a molecule.
01:21:40.740 | It's just a chemical that's circulating
01:21:43.500 | in our brain and body.
01:21:44.560 | There's no epinephrine specifically for a cold shower
01:21:48.120 | that is distinct from the epinephrine
01:21:50.000 | associated with a bad event,
01:21:51.200 | which is distinct from the epinephrine
01:21:52.760 | associated with a really exciting event
01:21:54.340 | that makes you really alert.
01:21:55.340 | Epinephrine is just a molecule, it's generic.
01:21:57.680 | And so these systems have a lot of overlap,
01:21:59.780 | and that can explain in large part
01:22:02.340 | why when good things happen in particular locations
01:22:05.560 | and in the company of particular people,
01:22:08.280 | we often generalize to large categories
01:22:11.620 | of people, places, and things.
01:22:13.440 | And when negative things happen in particular circumstances,
01:22:16.140 | we often generalize about people, places, and things
01:22:19.080 | associated with that negative event.
01:22:20.880 | So now I'd like to talk about other tools
01:22:22.660 | that you can leverage that have been shown
01:22:24.480 | in quality peer-reviewed studies
01:22:26.000 | to enhance learning and memory.
01:22:28.040 | And perhaps one of the most potent of those tools
01:22:30.720 | is exercise.
01:22:32.360 | There are numerous studies on this in both animal models
01:22:36.600 | and fortunately now also in humans,
01:22:39.400 | thanks to the beautiful work of people like Wendy Suzuki
01:22:42.220 | from New York University.
01:22:43.740 | Wendy's lab has identified how exercise works
01:22:47.040 | to enhance learning and memory
01:22:48.600 | and other forms of cognition, I should mention,
01:22:51.240 | as well as things that can augment,
01:22:54.480 | can enhance the effects of exercise on learning and memory
01:22:57.800 | and other forms of cognition.
01:22:59.840 | Wendy is going to be a guest on this podcast.
01:23:01.800 | It's actually the episode that follows this episode
01:23:05.000 | and includes a lot of material
01:23:06.320 | that we have not covered today.
01:23:08.120 | And she's an incredible scientist
01:23:10.720 | and has some incredible findings
01:23:12.600 | that I know everyone is going to find immensely useful.
01:23:15.540 | In the meantime,
01:23:16.920 | I want to talk about some of the general effects of exercise
01:23:19.900 | on learning and memory that she's discovered
01:23:22.000 | and the other laboratories have discovered.
01:23:24.260 | If you recall earlier,
01:23:25.140 | I mentioned that learning and memory
01:23:27.880 | almost always involves the strengthening
01:23:30.400 | of particular synapses and neural circuits in the brain
01:23:33.960 | and not so much the increase
01:23:36.580 | in the number of neurons in the brain.
01:23:39.160 | There is one exception, however,
01:23:40.960 | and we now have both animal data and some human data
01:23:43.640 | to support the fact that cardiovascular exercise
01:23:46.500 | seems to increase what we call dentate gyrus neurogenesis.
01:23:51.200 | Neurogenesis is the creation of new neurons.
01:23:53.440 | The dentate gyrus is a sub-region of the hippocampus
01:23:56.200 | that's involved in learning and memory of particular kinds,
01:23:59.800 | certain types of events, in particular contextual learning,
01:24:02.920 | but some other things as well,
01:24:04.860 | sometimes involved in spatial learning.
01:24:06.680 | There's a lot of debate about exactly
01:24:08.120 | what the dentate gyrus does,
01:24:09.480 | but for sake of this discussion,
01:24:11.340 | and I think everyone in the neuroscience community
01:24:14.580 | would agree that the dentate gyrus is important
01:24:16.620 | for memory formation and consolidation.
01:24:19.000 | The dentate gyrus does seem to be one region of the brain,
01:24:24.260 | certainly in the rodent brain,
01:24:25.540 | but more and more it's seeming also in the human brain
01:24:29.100 | where at least some new neurons
01:24:31.280 | are added throughout the lifespan.
01:24:33.180 | And as it turns out that cardiovascular exercise
01:24:36.720 | can increase the proliferation of new neurons
01:24:40.180 | in the structure and that those new neurons, excuse me,
01:24:44.040 | are important for the formation
01:24:46.560 | of certain types of new memories.
01:24:48.600 | There are wonderful data showing
01:24:49.960 | that if you use X irradiation,
01:24:52.100 | which is a way to eliminate the formation of those new cells
01:24:55.200 | or other tools and tricks
01:24:57.240 | to eliminate the formation of those cells,
01:24:59.160 | that you block the formation
01:25:00.960 | of certain kinds of learning and memory.
01:25:02.480 | What does this mean?
01:25:03.320 | Well, there are a lot of reasons
01:25:05.120 | for the statement I'm about to make
01:25:06.400 | that extend far beyond neurogenesis
01:25:09.280 | and the hippocampus learning and memory,
01:25:11.000 | but it's very clear that getting anywhere from 180,
01:25:14.700 | I should say a minimum of 180 to 200 minutes
01:25:17.760 | of so-called zone two cardiovascular exercise.
01:25:20.040 | So this is cardiovascular exercise that can be performed
01:25:23.320 | at a pretty steady state,
01:25:25.480 | which would allow you to just barely hold a conversation.
01:25:28.080 | So breathing hard, but not super hard.
01:25:31.000 | This isn't sprints or high intensity interval training,
01:25:33.320 | but doing that for 180 to 200 minutes per week total is,
01:25:37.940 | it appears the minimum threshold
01:25:39.640 | for enhancing some of the longevity effects
01:25:43.160 | associated with improvements in cardiovascular fitness.
01:25:46.280 | And we believe that it is indirectly,
01:25:48.960 | I should say indirectly through enhancements
01:25:51.280 | in cardiovascular fitness,
01:25:53.000 | that there are improvements
01:25:54.160 | in hippocampal dentate gyrus neurogenesis.
01:25:56.920 | What does that mean?
01:25:57.960 | The improvements in cardiovascular function
01:26:00.560 | are indirectly impacting the ability
01:26:02.700 | of the dentate gyrus to create these new neurons.
01:26:05.160 | To my knowledge, there's no direct relationship
01:26:07.920 | between exercise and stimulating the production
01:26:11.600 | of new neurons in the brain.
01:26:13.000 | It seems that it's the improvements in blood flow
01:26:17.260 | that also relate to improvements
01:26:18.920 | in things like lymphatic flow,
01:26:20.460 | the circulation of lymph fluid within the brain
01:26:22.840 | that are enhancing neurogenesis
01:26:24.900 | and that neurogenesis, it appears is important.
01:26:27.840 | Now, in fairness to the landscape of neuroscience
01:26:31.060 | and my colleagues at Stanford and elsewhere,
01:26:34.040 | there is a lot of debate as to whether or not
01:26:36.320 | there is much, if any, neurogenesis
01:26:39.640 | in the adult human brain.
01:26:42.100 | But regardless, I think the data are quite clear
01:26:44.760 | that the 180 to 200 minutes minimum
01:26:48.200 | of cardiovascular exercise is going to be important
01:26:50.960 | for other health metrics.
01:26:52.560 | Now, it is clear that exercise can impact learning
01:26:55.520 | and memory through other non-neurogenesis,
01:26:58.160 | non new neuron type mechanisms.
01:27:00.300 | And one of the more exciting ones
01:27:02.000 | that has been studied over the years
01:27:05.040 | is this notion of hormones from bone
01:27:08.680 | traveling in the bloodstream to the brain
01:27:11.000 | and enhancing the function of the hippocampus.
01:27:13.400 | Now, the words hormones from bones is surprising to you.
01:27:17.520 | I'm here to tell you that yes, indeed,
01:27:19.560 | your bones make hormones.
01:27:22.160 | We call these endocrine effects.
01:27:23.980 | So in biology, we hear about autocrine,
01:27:27.640 | paracrine and endocrine.
01:27:28.920 | And those different terms refer to over what distance
01:27:32.740 | a given chemical has an effect on a cell.
01:27:35.620 | For instance, a cell can have an effect on itself.
01:27:38.120 | It can have an effect on immediately neighboring cells,
01:27:40.600 | or it can have an effect on both itself,
01:27:43.420 | immediately neighboring cells,
01:27:44.600 | and cells far, far away in the body.
01:27:46.440 | And that last example of a given chemical or substance
01:27:50.840 | having an effect on the cell that produced it
01:27:54.220 | plus neighboring cells plus cells far away
01:27:56.380 | is an endocrine effect.
01:27:57.520 | And a lot of hormones not all work in this fashion.
01:28:01.360 | Hence, why we sometimes hear about endocrine and hormone
01:28:04.620 | is kind of synonymous terms.
01:28:06.000 | Your bones make chemicals that travel in the bloodstream
01:28:10.900 | and have these endocrine effects.
01:28:12.320 | So they're effectively acting as hormones.
01:28:14.000 | And one such chemical is something called osteocalcin.
01:28:17.260 | Now, these findings arrive to us through various labs,
01:28:19.560 | but one of the more important labs
01:28:20.860 | for sake of this discussion today
01:28:22.680 | is the laboratory of Eric Kandel
01:28:24.440 | at Columbia Medical School.
01:28:26.440 | Eric is now, I believe, in his mid to late 90s,
01:28:29.160 | still very sharp, and has studied learning and memory.
01:28:32.160 | It also turns out that he is an avid swimmer.
01:28:34.840 | Now, I happen to know that Eric swims
01:28:37.240 | anywhere from a half a mile to a mile a day.
01:28:39.960 | And again, this is anecdotal.
01:28:42.860 | I'm not referring to the published data just yet,
01:28:44.720 | but he credits that exercise as one of the ways
01:28:48.280 | in which he keeps his brain sharp
01:28:50.100 | and has indeed kept his brain sharp for many, many decades.
01:28:54.180 | And as I mentioned before, he's well into his 90s,
01:28:55.960 | so pretty impressive.
01:28:57.360 | His laboratory has studied the effects of exercise
01:29:00.640 | on hippocampal function and memory,
01:29:02.760 | and other laboratories have done that as well.
01:29:05.240 | And what they found is that cardiovascular exercise,
01:29:08.440 | and perhaps other forms of exercise too,
01:29:10.200 | but mainly cardiovascular exercise,
01:29:12.740 | creates the release of osteocalcin
01:29:15.040 | from the bones that travels to the brain
01:29:17.560 | and to subregions of the hippocampus
01:29:19.880 | and encourages the electrical activity
01:29:22.920 | and the formation and maintenance of connections
01:29:25.740 | within the hippocampus and keeps the hippocampus
01:29:28.540 | functioning well in order to lay down new memories.
01:29:31.440 | Now, osteocalcin has a lot of effects
01:29:33.480 | besides just improving the function of the hippocampus.
01:29:36.320 | Osteocalcin is involved in bone growth itself.
01:29:39.360 | It's involved in hormone regulation.
01:29:41.240 | In fact, there's really nice evidence
01:29:42.920 | that it can regulate testosterone and estrogen production
01:29:45.980 | by the testes and ovaries,
01:29:47.480 | and a bunch of other effects in other organs of the body,
01:29:49.880 | because again, it's acting in this endocrine manner.
01:29:52.120 | It's arriving from bone to a lot of different organs
01:29:55.500 | to have effects.
01:29:56.400 | Load-bearing exercise in particular
01:29:59.840 | turns out to be important
01:30:01.640 | for inducing the release of osteocalcin.
01:30:03.620 | And when you think about this, it makes sense.
01:30:06.800 | A nervous system exists for a lot of reasons,
01:30:08.940 | to sense, perceive, et cetera.
01:30:10.240 | You've got taste, you've got smell, you've got hearing.
01:30:12.160 | But the vast majority of brain real estate,
01:30:15.080 | especially in humans, is dedicated to two things.
01:30:18.080 | One, vision.
01:30:19.840 | We have an enormous amount of brain real estate
01:30:21.720 | devoted to vision, certainly compared to other senses.
01:30:25.560 | And to movement,
01:30:27.020 | the ability to generate coarse movements of the body,
01:30:29.540 | the ability, excuse me, to generate fine movements
01:30:32.500 | of the body, like the digits, or to wink one eye,
01:30:34.580 | or to tilt your head in a particular way,
01:30:36.440 | or move your lips, or move your face,
01:30:37.860 | and do all sorts of different things
01:30:39.060 | in a very nuanced and detailed way.
01:30:42.080 | So much of our brain real estate is devoted to movement,
01:30:45.100 | that it's been hypothesized for more than a half century,
01:30:49.420 | but especially in recent years,
01:30:51.120 | as we've learned more about the function of the brain
01:30:52.920 | at a really detailed circuit level,
01:30:54.840 | that the relationship between the brain and body
01:30:58.040 | and the maintenance,
01:31:00.200 | and perhaps even the improvement of neural circuitry
01:31:02.360 | in the brain, depends on our body movements
01:31:04.920 | and the signal from the body that our brain is still moving.
01:31:08.460 | So think about that.
01:31:09.300 | How would your brain know if your body was moving regularly?
01:31:11.900 | And how would it know how much it was moving?
01:31:13.580 | How would it know which limbs it was moving?
01:31:15.940 | Well, you could say, if the heart rate is increased,
01:31:18.660 | then the blood flow will be increased,
01:31:20.200 | and then the brain will know.
01:31:21.940 | Ah, but how does your brain know
01:31:24.600 | that it's increased blood flow due to movement
01:31:27.260 | and not to, for instance, just stress, right?
01:31:29.980 | Maybe you actually can't move
01:31:31.080 | and you're very stressed about that,
01:31:32.300 | and so the increased blood flow
01:31:33.800 | is simply a consequence of increased stress.
01:31:37.280 | The fact that osteocalcin is released from bone,
01:31:42.880 | and in particular can be released
01:31:44.720 | in response to load-bearing exercise,
01:31:46.900 | so this would be running.
01:31:48.400 | Again, weightlifting hasn't been tested directly,
01:31:50.480 | but one would imagine anything that involves
01:31:52.600 | jumping and landing or weightlifting or body weight movements
01:31:57.380 | and things of that sort,
01:31:59.320 | that's a signal to release osteocalcin,
01:32:01.840 | and we know that signal occurs,
01:32:03.960 | that is directly reflective of the fact
01:32:08.480 | that the body was moving and moving in particular ways.
01:32:11.200 | In fact, you could imagine that big bones, like your femur,
01:32:14.600 | are going to release more osteocalcin
01:32:16.240 | or be in a position to release more osteocalcin
01:32:18.280 | than fine movements like the movements of the digits.
01:32:21.240 | And this idea that the body is constantly signaling
01:32:24.480 | to the brain about the status of the body
01:32:26.720 | and the varying needs of the brain
01:32:29.100 | to update its brain circuitry
01:32:31.360 | is a really attractive idea that fits entirely
01:32:34.840 | with the biology of exercise,
01:32:36.840 | osteocalcin, and hippocampal function.
01:32:39.520 | I do want to mention that I'm not the first
01:32:42.440 | to raise this hypothesis.
01:32:43.760 | This hypothesis actually was discussed
01:32:45.660 | in a fair amount of detail by John Rady,
01:32:48.060 | who's a professor in Harvard Medical School.
01:32:50.300 | He wrote a book called "Spark,"
01:32:51.700 | which was one of the early books,
01:32:54.240 | at least from an academic, about brain plasticity
01:32:56.160 | and the relationship between exercise
01:32:57.640 | and movement in plasticity.
01:32:58.960 | And John, who I have the good fortune to know,
01:33:01.940 | has described to me experiments,
01:33:03.860 | or I should say observations,
01:33:05.480 | of species of ocean-dwelling animals
01:33:08.620 | that have, at least for the early part of their life,
01:33:11.040 | a very robust and complicated nervous system.
01:33:14.160 | But then these particular animals
01:33:16.640 | are in the habit of plopping down onto a rock.
01:33:19.380 | They find a kind of a safe, comfy space,
01:33:21.720 | and they actually stick to that rock,
01:33:24.000 | and they don't move anymore for a certain portion,
01:33:26.960 | I should say the late portion of their life.
01:33:29.100 | And it is at the transition between moving a lot
01:33:31.620 | and being stationary that those animals
01:33:33.540 | actually digest their own brain.
01:33:36.460 | They literally metabolize a good portion
01:33:38.920 | of their nervous system because they decide,
01:33:40.760 | oh, don't need this anymore, and gobble it up,
01:33:43.580 | use it for its nutritional value,
01:33:46.300 | and then sit there like a moron version of themselves
01:33:49.780 | with a limited amount of brain tissue
01:33:53.860 | because they don't need to move anymore.
01:33:55.420 | Now, I certainly don't want to give the message
01:33:57.120 | that just moving, just exercise is sufficient
01:34:00.060 | to keep the neural architecture of your brain
01:34:02.000 | healthy, young, and able to learn.
01:34:03.660 | While that might be true,
01:34:06.380 | it's also important to actually engage
01:34:08.180 | in attempts to learn new material,
01:34:10.220 | either physical material,
01:34:11.600 | so new types of movements and skills,
01:34:14.600 | and/or new types of cognitive information,
01:34:16.980 | languages, mathematics, history, current events,
01:34:20.840 | all sorts of things that involve your brain.
01:34:23.720 | Nonetheless, it's clear that physical movement
01:34:26.780 | and cognitive ability
01:34:28.560 | and the potential to enhance cognitive ability
01:34:31.360 | and the ability to learn new physical skills
01:34:33.280 | are intimately connected.
01:34:34.960 | And osteocalcin appears to be at least one way
01:34:38.400 | in which that brain-body relationship
01:34:40.040 | is established and maintained.
01:34:41.680 | So given the information about osteocalcin and movement,
01:34:45.120 | and given the information about spiking adrenaline late
01:34:48.960 | or after a period of attempt to learn,
01:34:52.300 | you might be asking, when is the best time to exercise?
01:34:55.120 | Now, unfortunately, that has not been addressed
01:34:58.020 | in a lot of varying detail
01:34:59.800 | where every sort of variation on the theme
01:35:02.720 | has been carried out.
01:35:03.680 | And yet, Wendy Suzuki's lab
01:35:05.400 | has done really beautiful experiments
01:35:07.720 | where they have people exercise,
01:35:10.200 | generally it was in the morning,
01:35:11.860 | but at other periods of the day as well.
01:35:14.640 | And what they find is that at least as late as two hours
01:35:18.600 | after that exercise,
01:35:20.680 | there's an enhancement in learning and memory.
01:35:23.240 | Now, I want to be clear,
01:35:24.440 | we don't know whether or not that exercise
01:35:26.760 | led to big increases in adrenaline.
01:35:29.600 | It may be that those forms of exercise were modest enough
01:35:33.600 | or didn't challenge people enough,
01:35:35.500 | that they merely got a lot of blood flow going,
01:35:37.600 | and that the improvements in learning and memory
01:35:39.220 | were related to blood flow,
01:35:40.380 | and we presume increases in osteocalcin.
01:35:43.920 | However, you could imagine
01:35:45.280 | a couple of different logical protocols
01:35:48.100 | based on what we've talked about.
01:35:50.060 | Let's say you were going to do a form of exercise
01:35:51.880 | that was going to spike adrenaline a lot.
01:35:53.560 | So this would be exercise that really challenges
01:35:56.420 | your system and forces you to kind of push through a burn.
01:35:59.160 | So here I'm mainly thinking about cardiovascular exercise,
01:36:01.960 | but it could even be yoga, it could be resistance training.
01:36:07.200 | If it's going to give you a big spike in adrenaline,
01:36:09.840 | it's going to take some serious effort,
01:36:12.380 | then logically speaking,
01:36:15.080 | you would want to place that after a learning bout
01:36:17.460 | in order to increase learning and memory.
01:36:19.180 | However, if you're using the exercise
01:36:21.740 | in order to enhance blood flow
01:36:23.480 | and to enhance osteocalcin release
01:36:26.120 | in efforts to augment the function of your hippocampus,
01:36:28.780 | I think it stands to reason that doing that exercise
01:36:31.760 | sometime within the hour to three hours
01:36:35.400 | preceding an attempt to learn
01:36:36.880 | makes a lot of sense.
01:36:37.880 | And there I'm basing it on the human data
01:36:39.480 | from Wendy Suzuki's lab,
01:36:41.080 | I'm basing it on the studies from Eric Kandel
01:36:43.340 | and from others labs.
01:36:45.320 | Again, right now there hasn't been an evaluation
01:36:48.040 | of a lot of different protocols to arrive
01:36:49.840 | at the peer reviewed laboratory super protocol.
01:36:53.500 | However, since what we're talking about
01:36:55.640 | is using activities like exercise
01:36:57.520 | that most of us probably, perhaps all of us
01:37:00.520 | should be doing regularly anyway.
01:37:02.520 | And I do believe most, if not all of us
01:37:04.640 | should probably regularly be trying to learn
01:37:06.440 | and keep our brain functioning well
01:37:08.080 | and acquire new knowledge
01:37:09.040 | because it's just a wonderful part of life.
01:37:10.740 | And there is evidence that that actually
01:37:12.160 | can keep your brain young, so to speak.
01:37:14.600 | Well then exercising either before or after a learning bout
01:37:19.600 | makes a lot of sense with the emphasis
01:37:22.660 | on after a learning bout,
01:37:24.180 | if the form of exercise spikes a lot of adrenaline
01:37:26.480 | for all the reasons we talked about before.
01:37:28.440 | Okay, so we've talked about two major categories
01:37:30.620 | of protocols to improve memory
01:37:32.200 | that are grounded in quality peer reviewed science.
01:37:35.240 | And there is yet another third protocol
01:37:37.760 | that we'll talk about in a few minutes.
01:37:39.440 | But before we do that,
01:37:40.760 | I want to briefly touch on an aspect of memory.
01:37:43.320 | In fact, two aspects of memory
01:37:44.760 | that I get a lot of questions about.
01:37:46.960 | The first one is photographic memory.
01:37:50.800 | To be clear, there are people out there
01:37:52.920 | who have a true photographic memory.
01:37:54.760 | They can look at a page of text,
01:37:56.600 | they can scan it with their eyes
01:37:57.840 | and they can essentially commit that to memory
01:38:00.760 | with very little, if any effort.
01:38:03.200 | While it might seem that having a photographic memory
01:38:04.920 | is a very attractive skill to have,
01:38:06.840 | I should caution you against believing that
01:38:09.040 | because it turns out that people
01:38:10.700 | with true photographic memory are often very challenged
01:38:13.260 | at remembering things that they hear
01:38:15.560 | and oftentimes are not so good at learning physical skills.
01:38:18.800 | It's not always the case, but often that's the case.
01:38:21.000 | So be careful what you wish for.
01:38:22.700 | If you do have a photographic memory,
01:38:24.400 | there are certain professions
01:38:25.580 | that lend themselves particularly well to you.
01:38:29.640 | And indeed, a lot of people with photographic memory
01:38:31.760 | have to find a profession and have to move through life
01:38:34.940 | in a way that is in concert with that photographic memory.
01:38:38.840 | So again, it's a super ability, it's a hyper ability,
01:38:42.280 | and yet it's not necessarily one
01:38:43.920 | that is desirable for most people.
01:38:46.360 | There's also this category
01:38:47.360 | of what are called super recognizers.
01:38:49.440 | These people are, I should mention,
01:38:51.020 | highly employable by government agencies.
01:38:53.320 | These are people that have an absolutely astonishing ability
01:38:57.820 | to recognize faces and to match faces,
01:39:01.400 | to templates.
01:39:02.240 | They can look at a photograph of, say,
01:39:04.640 | somebody on a most wanted list,
01:39:07.360 | and then they can look at video footage of,
01:39:11.160 | let's say, an airport or a mall or a city street
01:39:13.360 | at fairly low resolution,
01:39:15.260 | and they can spot the person whose face matches
01:39:18.420 | that photograph that they looked at.
01:39:20.360 | Even if that video or other footage is of people's profiles
01:39:25.360 | or even the tops of their heads
01:39:28.200 | and just a portion of their forehead,
01:39:29.380 | these people have just an incredible ability
01:39:32.140 | to recognize faces and to template match.
01:39:34.440 | And again, these people often will take jobs with agencies
01:39:37.560 | where this sort of thing is important.
01:39:39.760 | Some of you out there probably are super recognizers
01:39:42.560 | and may or may not notice it.
01:39:44.140 | If you've ever had the experience of watching a movie
01:39:46.180 | and thought to yourself,
01:39:47.620 | wow, her mouth looks so much like my cousin's mouth,
01:39:52.380 | or you look at a character in a movie or television show
01:39:56.340 | and you think, wow, they look almost like
01:39:59.240 | the younger sister of so-and-so,
01:40:01.680 | well, then it's very likely that you have this,
01:40:05.920 | or at least a mild form of this super recognizer ability.
01:40:09.080 | That is not memory per se.
01:40:11.640 | That is the hyperfunctioning of an area of the brain
01:40:14.320 | that we call the fusiform gyrus.
01:40:16.960 | The fusiform gyrus is literally a face recognition area
01:40:20.840 | and a face template matching area,
01:40:22.880 | and it harbors neurons that respond to faces generally.
01:40:25.840 | So as humans and other non-human primates
01:40:29.040 | care a lot about faces and their emotional content,
01:40:31.700 | and the identity of faces is super important to us
01:40:35.020 | for all the kinds of reasons that are probably obvious,
01:40:38.200 | knowing who's friend, who's foe, who do you know well,
01:40:40.900 | who's famous, who's not famous, et cetera.
01:40:43.840 | That is not memory per se.
01:40:45.700 | And yet if you're a super recognizer,
01:40:48.100 | or I guess we could call it a moderate face recognizer,
01:40:51.900 | or not very good at recognizing faces,
01:40:54.080 | 'cause indeed there are some people
01:40:55.200 | that are kind of face blind.
01:40:56.900 | They don't actually recognize people.
01:40:59.040 | When they walk in the room,
01:40:59.880 | I used to work with somebody like this,
01:41:01.040 | I'd walk into his office and he'd say,
01:41:02.660 | "Are you rich or are you Andrew?"
01:41:04.960 | I'd say, "Well, am I rich rich?
01:41:07.040 | Like, you know, wealth rich?"
01:41:09.060 | And he'd say, "No, are you Richard or are you Andrew?"
01:41:11.560 | And I'd say, "I'm Andrew.
01:41:12.720 | We know each other really well."
01:41:13.680 | He said, "Oh, I'm sorry, I'm kind of face blind."
01:41:16.080 | And it actually tend to be better or worse
01:41:18.920 | depending on how much he was working.
01:41:21.200 | Ironically, the more rested he was,
01:41:24.080 | the more face blind he would become.
01:41:25.520 | So it wasn't a sleep deprivation thing.
01:41:27.160 | That exists, that's out there.
01:41:28.280 | There's the full constellation
01:41:30.200 | of people's ability to recognize faces.
01:41:32.240 | That's not really memory.
01:41:33.380 | And yet visual function is a profoundly powerful way
01:41:37.680 | in which we can enhance our memory.
01:41:39.140 | So whether or not you're a super recognizer of faces,
01:41:41.800 | whether or not you are face blind or anything in between,
01:41:45.160 | next I'm going to tell you about a study
01:41:47.400 | which points out the immense value of visual images
01:41:52.120 | for laying down memories.
01:41:54.120 | And you can leverage this information
01:41:56.120 | and this involves both the taking a photograph,
01:41:58.280 | something that's actually quite easy,
01:41:59.760 | easily done these days with your phone,
01:42:01.560 | as well as your ability to take mental photographs
01:42:04.240 | by literally snapping your eyelid shut.
01:42:06.700 | So I just briefly want to describe this paper
01:42:08.440 | because it provides a tool that you can leverage
01:42:10.760 | in your attempt to learn and remember things better.
01:42:14.120 | The title of this paper is "Photographic Memory,
01:42:16.980 | the effects of our volitional photo taking
01:42:19.120 | on memory for visual and auditory aspects of an experience."
01:42:23.680 | I really like this paper
01:42:24.780 | because it refers to photographic memory,
01:42:27.680 | not in the context of photographic memory
01:42:30.160 | that we normally hear about
01:42:31.120 | where people are truly photographic,
01:42:32.760 | look at a page and somehow absorb all that information
01:42:35.600 | and commit it to memory,
01:42:36.680 | but rather the use of camera photographs
01:42:39.600 | or the use of mental camera photographs,
01:42:42.840 | literally looking at something and deciding blink
01:42:45.400 | and snapping a, so to speak, snapping a snapshot
01:42:49.380 | of whatever it is that you were looking at
01:42:50.880 | and remembering the content.
01:42:52.440 | The reason I like this paper
01:42:53.640 | and the reason I'm attracted to this issue
01:42:55.520 | of mental snapshots is this is something
01:42:57.200 | that I've been doing since I was a kid.
01:42:58.360 | I don't know why I started doing it,
01:42:59.640 | but every once in a while, I would say maybe twice a year,
01:43:03.420 | I would look at something
01:43:04.480 | and decide to just snap a mental snapshot of it.
01:43:07.440 | And I've maintained very clear memories
01:43:09.120 | of those visual scenes.
01:43:10.560 | Two years ago, I was in an Uber and I looked out the window
01:43:14.800 | and it was a street scene.
01:43:15.640 | I was actually in New York at the time
01:43:17.360 | and I decided for reasons that are still unclear to me
01:43:21.120 | to take a mental snapshot of this city street image,
01:43:23.400 | even though nothing interesting in particular was happening.
01:43:26.320 | And I do recall that there was a guy wearing a yellow shirt,
01:43:29.800 | walking, there was some construction, et cetera.
01:43:31.440 | I can still see that image in my mind's eye
01:43:33.720 | because I took this mental snapshot.
01:43:35.560 | This paper addresses
01:43:36.440 | whether or not this mental snapshotting thing is real.
01:43:38.780 | And this is something that I think a lot of people
01:43:42.040 | will resonate with,
01:43:43.000 | whether or not the constant taking of pictures on our phones
01:43:45.840 | or with other devices is either improving
01:43:48.560 | or degrading our memory.
01:43:50.140 | You can imagine an argument for both.
01:43:52.560 | A lot of people are taking pictures
01:43:53.820 | that they never look at again.
01:43:55.680 | And so in a sense,
01:43:56.520 | they're outsourcing their visual memory of events
01:44:00.480 | into their phone or to some other device.
01:44:03.440 | And they're not ever accessing the actual image again,
01:44:06.160 | they're not looking at it, right?
01:44:07.280 | You're not printing out those photos,
01:44:08.640 | you're not scanning through your phone again.
01:44:10.060 | Sometimes you might do that,
01:44:10.980 | but most of the time people don't.
01:44:12.060 | Most of the photographs that people take in,
01:44:13.520 | they're not revisiting again.
01:44:15.000 | So the motivation for this study was that
01:44:16.780 | previous experiments had shown that
01:44:19.720 | if people take photos of a scene or a person or an object,
01:44:24.440 | that they are actually less good at remembering
01:44:27.700 | the details of that scene or object, et cetera.
01:44:31.220 | This study challenged that idea and raised the hypothesis
01:44:36.300 | that if people are allowed to choose
01:44:39.000 | what they take photos of,
01:44:40.720 | that taking photos, again,
01:44:42.400 | this is with a camera, not mental snapshotting,
01:44:44.220 | that taking those photos would actually enhance their memory
01:44:47.020 | for those objects, those places, those people,
01:44:49.680 | and in fact, details of those object places and people.
01:44:53.080 | And indeed that's what they found.
01:44:54.920 | So in contrast to previous studies,
01:44:56.940 | where people had been more or less told,
01:44:59.520 | take photos of these following objects
01:45:01.380 | or these following people or these following places,
01:45:03.680 | and then they were given a memory test at some point later.
01:45:07.480 | In this study, people were given volitional control, right?
01:45:11.280 | They were given agency in making the decision
01:45:13.700 | of what to take photos of.
01:45:15.000 | And I'll just summarize the results.
01:45:16.240 | We'll provide a link to this study.
01:45:17.480 | I should say that some of the stuff that they tested
01:45:19.900 | was actually pretty challenging.
01:45:21.520 | Some of them were pottery and other forms of ceramics
01:45:24.960 | that are of the sort that you see
01:45:26.600 | if you go to a big museum in a big city,
01:45:28.560 | and if you've ever done that
01:45:29.480 | and you see all the different objects,
01:45:31.120 | there are a lot of details in those objects,
01:45:32.600 | and a lot of those objects look a lot alike.
01:45:35.300 | And so, you know, some will have two handles,
01:45:37.700 | some will have one handle, the position of the handles,
01:45:39.660 | how broad or narrow these things are.
01:45:42.500 | You know, a lot of this is pretty detailed stuff.
01:45:44.460 | They also took photos of other things.
01:45:47.300 | So basically what they found
01:45:48.820 | was that if people take pictures of things
01:45:50.860 | and they choose which things they're taking pictures of,
01:45:53.460 | right, it's up to them, it's volitional,
01:45:55.240 | that there's enhanced memory for those objects later on.
01:46:00.240 | However, it degraded their ability
01:46:04.380 | to remember auditory information.
01:46:06.180 | So what this means is that
01:46:07.020 | when we take a picture of something or a person,
01:46:11.060 | we are stamping down a visual memory of that thing,
01:46:14.620 | and that makes sense, it's a photograph after all,
01:46:16.840 | but we are actually inhibiting our ability
01:46:19.060 | to remember the auditory, the sound components
01:46:22.460 | of that visual scene or what the person was saying.
01:46:24.560 | Very interesting, and points to the fact
01:46:26.460 | that the visual system can out-compete the auditory system,
01:46:29.140 | at least in terms of how the hippocampus
01:46:30.980 | is encoding this information.
01:46:32.740 | The other finding I find particularly interesting
01:46:34.720 | within this study is that it didn't matter
01:46:38.420 | whether or not they ever looked at the photos again.
01:46:40.520 | So they actually had people take photos
01:46:42.940 | or not take photos of different objects.
01:46:45.600 | They had some people keep their photos
01:46:47.660 | and they had other people delete their photos.
01:46:49.340 | And turns out that whether or not people kept the photos
01:46:52.260 | or deleted those photos had no bearing
01:46:53.980 | on whether or not they were better or worse
01:46:56.420 | at remembering things, they were always better
01:46:58.020 | at remembering them as compared to not taking photos of them.
01:47:01.020 | What does this mean?
01:47:01.860 | It means if you really want to remember something
01:47:03.580 | or somebody, take a photo of that thing or person,
01:47:07.600 | pay attention while you take the photo,
01:47:10.100 | but it doesn't really matter if you look at the photo again.
01:47:12.020 | Somehow the process of taking that photo,
01:47:14.500 | probably looking at it in a camera,
01:47:18.340 | typically we'd say through the viewfinder now
01:47:20.240 | because of digital cameras on the screen
01:47:22.260 | on the back of that camera or on your phone,
01:47:25.180 | that framing up of the photograph
01:47:27.020 | stamps down a visual image in your mind
01:47:29.020 | that is more robust at serving a memory
01:47:32.020 | than had you just looked at that thing with your own eyes.
01:47:34.980 | Very interesting and it raises all sorts of questions for me
01:47:37.980 | about whether or not it's because you're framing up
01:47:39.980 | a small aperture or a small portion of the visual scene.
01:47:42.820 | That's one logical interpretation,
01:47:44.420 | although they didn't test that.
01:47:45.920 | I should also say that they found
01:47:47.700 | that whether or not you looked at a photo that you took
01:47:50.580 | or whether or not you deleted it
01:47:51.860 | and never looked at it again,
01:47:53.720 | didn't just enhance visual memory
01:47:55.740 | or the memory for the visual components of that image,
01:47:59.000 | but it always reduced your ability to remember
01:48:02.480 | sounds associated with that experience.
01:48:04.760 | So that's interesting.
01:48:06.300 | And then last but not least,
01:48:08.100 | and perhaps most interesting, at least to me,
01:48:10.380 | was the fact that you didn't even need a camera
01:48:12.820 | to see this effect.
01:48:14.540 | If subjects looked at something
01:48:16.500 | and took a mental photograph of that thing,
01:48:19.340 | it enhanced their visual memory of that thing
01:48:23.060 | significantly more than had they not taken a mental picture.
01:48:26.620 | In fact, it increased their memory of that thing
01:48:30.260 | almost as much as taking an actual photograph
01:48:32.760 | with an actual camera.
01:48:34.360 | And the reason I find this so interesting
01:48:36.340 | is that a lot of what we try and learn is visual.
01:48:40.500 | And for a lot of people,
01:48:41.900 | the ability to learn visual information feels challenging
01:48:45.900 | and we'll look at something
01:48:47.020 | and we'll try and create some detailed understanding of it.
01:48:49.860 | We'll try and understand the relationships
01:48:51.260 | between things in that scene.
01:48:53.380 | It does appear based on the study
01:48:54.940 | that the mere decision to take a mental snapshot,
01:48:58.380 | like, okay, I'm going to blink my eyelids
01:48:59.880 | and I'm going to take a snapshot of whatever it is I see,
01:49:01.700 | can actually stamp down a visual memory
01:49:04.220 | much in the same way
01:49:05.580 | that a camera can stamp down a visual memory,
01:49:07.760 | of course, through vastly distinct mechanisms.
01:49:10.680 | No discussion of memory would be complete
01:49:13.220 | without a discussion of the ever intriguing phenomenon
01:49:17.100 | known as deja vu.
01:49:19.260 | This sense that we've experienced something
01:49:21.220 | before but we can't quite put our finger on it.
01:49:23.500 | Where and when did it happen?
01:49:24.980 | Or the sense that we've been someplace before
01:49:27.260 | or that we are in a familiar state
01:49:30.220 | or place or context of some kind.
01:49:33.600 | Now, I've talked about this on the podcast before,
01:49:36.380 | at least I think I have,
01:49:37.860 | and the way this works has been defined
01:49:41.000 | largely by the wonderful work of Susumu Tonogawa
01:49:44.060 | at Massachusetts Institute of Technology, MIT.
01:49:47.340 | Susumu collected a Nobel Prize quite appropriately
01:49:50.660 | for his beautiful work on immunology
01:49:52.920 | and he's also a highly accomplished neuroscientist
01:49:55.620 | who studies memory and learning and deja vu.
01:49:58.860 | And I should also mention the beautiful work of Mark Mayford
01:50:01.860 | at the Scripps Institute in UC San Diego,
01:50:04.620 | beautiful work on this notion of deja vu.
01:50:06.660 | Here's what they discovered.
01:50:08.720 | They evaluated the patterns of neural firing
01:50:11.220 | in the hippocampus as subjects learn new things, okay?
01:50:16.220 | So neuron A fires, then neuron B fires,
01:50:20.020 | then neuron C fires in a particular sequence.
01:50:22.820 | Again, the firing of neurons in a particular sequence,
01:50:25.800 | like the playing of keys on a piano
01:50:27.500 | in a particular sequence
01:50:28.360 | leads to a particular song on the piano
01:50:30.020 | and leads to a particular memory
01:50:32.180 | of an experience within the brain.
01:50:34.020 | They then used some molecular tools and tricks
01:50:39.220 | to label and capture those neurons
01:50:41.940 | such that they could go back later
01:50:43.980 | and activate those neurons in either the same sequence
01:50:47.980 | or in a different sequence to the one that occurred
01:50:51.140 | during the formation of the memory.
01:50:53.660 | And to make a long story short
01:50:55.220 | and to summarize multiple papers
01:50:57.300 | published in incredibly high tier journals,
01:51:00.540 | journals like Nature and Science,
01:51:02.060 | which are extremely stringent,
01:51:03.800 | found that whether or not those particular neurons
01:51:08.800 | were played in the precise sequence
01:51:10.680 | that happened when they encoded the memory
01:51:13.380 | or whether or not those neurons were played
01:51:15.140 | in a different sequence
01:51:16.660 | or even if those neurons were played,
01:51:19.580 | activated that is, all at once with no temporal sequence,
01:51:24.580 | all firing in concert all at once,
01:51:27.540 | evoked the same behavior and in some sense, the same memory.
01:51:33.580 | So at a neural circuit level, this is deja vu.
01:51:38.940 | This is a different pattern of firing of neurons
01:51:41.820 | in the brain leading to the same sense of what happened
01:51:46.220 | leading to a particular emotional state or behavior.
01:51:51.120 | Now, whether or not this same sort of phenomenon occurs
01:51:54.180 | when you're walking down the street
01:51:55.420 | and suddenly you feel as if,
01:51:57.160 | wow, I feel like I've been here before.
01:51:58.580 | You meet someone and you feel like,
01:51:59.740 | gosh, I feel like I know you.
01:52:01.180 | I feel like there's some familiarity here
01:52:03.340 | that I can't quite put my finger on.
01:52:04.760 | We don't know for sure that that's what's happening,
01:52:07.660 | but this is the most mechanistic and logical explanation
01:52:11.820 | for what has for many decades, if not hundreds of years,
01:52:15.740 | has been described as deja vu.
01:52:17.660 | So for those of you that experienced deja vu often,
01:52:20.500 | just know that this reflects a normal pattern
01:52:23.660 | of encoding experiences and events within your hippocampus.
01:52:27.940 | I'm not aware of any pathological situations
01:52:30.820 | where the presence of deja vu inhibits daily life.
01:52:34.820 | Some people like the sensation of deja vu,
01:52:37.420 | other people don't.
01:52:38.620 | Almost everybody, however, describes it as somewhat eerie.
01:52:41.820 | This idea that even though you're in a very different place,
01:52:44.300 | even though you're interacting with a very different person,
01:52:46.900 | that you could somehow feel as if this has happened before.
01:52:50.980 | And just realize this, that your hippocampus,
01:52:53.760 | while it is exquisitely good at encoding
01:52:57.700 | new types of perceptions, new experiences, new emotions,
01:53:02.700 | new contingencies and relationships of life events,
01:53:07.180 | it is not infinitely large,
01:53:08.900 | nor does it have an infinite bucket
01:53:11.780 | full of different options of different sequences
01:53:14.220 | for those neurons to play.
01:53:15.540 | So in a lot of ways, it makes perfect sense
01:53:17.820 | that sometimes we would feel as if a given experience
01:53:20.620 | had happened previously.
01:53:22.100 | I'd like to cover one additional tool that you can use
01:53:24.820 | to improve learning and memory.
01:53:26.380 | And I should mention, this is a particularly powerful one,
01:53:29.500 | and it's one that I'm definitely going to employ myself.
01:53:33.420 | This is based on a paper from none other
01:53:36.860 | than Wendy Suzuki at New York University.
01:53:39.360 | We talked about her a little bit earlier.
01:53:40.980 | And again, she's going to be on the podcast
01:53:42.900 | in our next episode and is just an incredible researcher.
01:53:46.380 | I've known Wendy for a number of years,
01:53:47.820 | and it's only in the last, I would say five or six years,
01:53:50.200 | that she's really shifted her laboratory
01:53:52.340 | toward generating protocols that human beings can use.
01:53:56.100 | And she's putting that to great effect,
01:53:58.580 | great positive effect, I should say,
01:54:00.420 | publishing papers of the sort that I'm about to describe,
01:54:02.520 | but also incorporating some of these tools and protocols
01:54:05.480 | into the learning curriculum
01:54:07.260 | and the lifestyle curriculum of students at NYU,
01:54:10.380 | which I think is a terrific initiative.
01:54:12.540 | So you don't need to be an NYU student
01:54:14.020 | in order to benefit from her work.
01:54:15.620 | I'm going to tell you about some of that work now,
01:54:17.100 | and she'll tell you about this and much more
01:54:19.740 | in the episode that follows this one.
01:54:21.640 | The title of this paper will tell you a lot
01:54:23.980 | about where we're going.
01:54:24.920 | The title is "Brief Daily Meditation Enhances Attention,
01:54:28.740 | Memory, Mood, and Emotional Regulation
01:54:31.220 | in Non-Experienced Meditators."
01:54:33.900 | If ever there was an incentive to meditate,
01:54:36.040 | it is the data contained within this paper.
01:54:40.300 | I want to briefly describe the study,
01:54:41.860 | and then I also want to emphasize
01:54:44.420 | that when you meditate is absolutely critical.
01:54:47.500 | I'll talk about that just at the end.
01:54:49.540 | This is a study that involves subjects aged 18 to 45,
01:54:54.180 | none of whom were experienced meditators
01:54:56.380 | prior to this study.
01:54:57.500 | There were two general groups in this study.
01:55:01.460 | One group did a 13-minute long meditation,
01:55:06.800 | and this meditation was a fairly conventional meditation.
01:55:09.940 | They would sit or lie down.
01:55:11.840 | They would do somewhat of a body scan,
01:55:14.220 | evaluating, for instance,
01:55:15.460 | how tense or relaxed they felt throughout their body,
01:55:17.700 | and they would focus on their breathing,
01:55:19.560 | trying to bring their attention back to their breathing
01:55:21.480 | and to the state of their body as the meditation progressed.
01:55:25.200 | The other group, which we can call the control group,
01:55:28.740 | listened to, of all things, a podcast.
01:55:31.660 | They did not listen to this podcast.
01:55:33.480 | They listened to "Radio Lab," which is a popular podcast
01:55:36.620 | for an equivalent amount of time,
01:55:38.520 | but they were not instructed to do any kind of body scan
01:55:40.720 | or pay attention to their breathing.
01:55:43.300 | Every subject in the study either meditated daily
01:55:46.240 | or listened to a equivalent duration podcast daily
01:55:49.400 | for a period of eight weeks.
01:55:51.840 | And the experimenters measured a large number of things,
01:55:56.060 | of variables, as we say.
01:55:58.020 | They looked at measures of emotion regulation.
01:56:00.500 | They actually measured cortisol, a stress hormone.
01:56:02.980 | They measured, as the title suggests,
01:56:05.200 | attention and memory and so forth.
01:56:07.740 | And the basic takeaway of the study is that eight weeks,
01:56:12.260 | but not four weeks, of this daily 13-minute-a-day meditation
01:56:16.240 | had a significant effect in improving attention, memory,
01:56:21.240 | mood, and emotion regulation.
01:56:23.080 | I find this study to be very interesting
01:56:26.820 | and, in fact, important because most of us have heard
01:56:30.080 | about the positive effects of meditation
01:56:31.860 | on things like stress reduction
01:56:34.080 | or on things such as improving sleep.
01:56:36.700 | And I want to come back to sleep in a few moments
01:56:39.280 | 'cause it turns out to be a very important feature
01:56:41.100 | of this study.
01:56:41.940 | This particular study I like so much
01:56:45.660 | because they used a really broad array of measurements
01:56:49.880 | for cognitive function,
01:56:51.260 | things like the Wisconsin card sorting task.
01:56:53.260 | I'm not going to go into this.
01:56:54.220 | Things like the Stroop task.
01:56:55.600 | And they also, as I mentioned, measured cortisol
01:56:58.700 | and many other things, including, not surprisingly, memory.
01:57:03.260 | And people's ability to remember
01:57:05.620 | certain types of information,
01:57:06.740 | in fact, varied types of information.
01:57:08.800 | And the basic takeaway was, again,
01:57:11.740 | that you could get really robust improvements
01:57:14.180 | in learning and memory, mood, and attention
01:57:16.720 | from just 13 minutes a day of meditation.
01:57:19.620 | Now, there's an important twist in this study
01:57:21.380 | that I want to emphasize.
01:57:22.780 | If you read into the discussion of this study,
01:57:25.260 | it's mentioned that somehow meditation did not improve
01:57:30.100 | but actually impaired sleep quality
01:57:32.340 | compared to the control subjects.
01:57:34.240 | You might think, wow, why would that be?
01:57:36.080 | I mean, meditation is supposed to reduce our stress.
01:57:38.860 | Stress is supposed to inhibit sleep.
01:57:40.680 | And therefore, why would sleep get worse?
01:57:44.460 | Well, what's interesting is the time of day
01:57:47.040 | when most of these subjects tended to do their meditation.
01:57:51.200 | Most of the subjects in this study
01:57:52.680 | did their meditation late in the day.
01:57:54.400 | This is often the case in experiments.
01:57:56.060 | I know this because we run experiments
01:57:58.260 | with human subjects in my laboratory.
01:57:59.840 | And people are paid some amount of money
01:58:02.040 | in order to participate,
01:58:03.100 | or they're given something as compensation
01:58:05.400 | for being in the study.
01:58:06.240 | But oftentimes, the meditation, or in the case of my lab,
01:58:09.580 | the respiration work or other kinds of things
01:58:12.560 | that they're assigned to do are not their top, top priority.
01:58:15.500 | And we understand this.
01:58:16.580 | But in this study, the majority of subjects here I'm reading
01:58:19.020 | completed their meditation sessions
01:58:21.340 | from somewhere between 8 and 11 p.m.
01:58:24.460 | and sometimes even between 12 and 3 a.m.
01:58:27.660 | I think there probably were a lot of college students
01:58:29.280 | enrolled in this study.
01:58:30.460 | And their hours often are late shifted.
01:58:33.640 | That impaired sleep.
01:58:35.320 | And this raises a bigger theme that I think is important
01:58:37.740 | many times before on this podcast
01:58:39.700 | and certainly in the episode on mastering sleep
01:58:41.640 | and conquering or mastering stress.
01:58:44.580 | Those episodes, we talked about the value, again,
01:58:46.820 | of these non-sleep deep rest protocols, NSDR,
01:58:49.840 | for reducing the activity of your sympathetic nervous system,
01:58:53.980 | the alertness, so-called stress arm
01:58:56.420 | of your autonomic nervous system,
01:58:58.960 | that makes you feel really alert.
01:59:00.420 | NSDR is superb for reducing your level of alertness,
01:59:04.420 | increasing your level of calmness,
01:59:05.780 | and putting you into a so-called
01:59:06.720 | more parasympathetic relaxed state.
01:59:09.860 | Meditation does that too, but it also increases attention.
01:59:14.200 | If you think about meditation,
01:59:16.240 | meditation involves focusing on your breath
01:59:18.580 | and constantly focusing back on your breath
01:59:20.600 | and trying to avoid the distraction of things you're thinking
01:59:23.020 | or things that you're hearing
01:59:24.260 | and coming so-called back to your body, back to your breath.
01:59:28.000 | So meditation actually has a high attentional load.
01:59:33.000 | It requires a lot of prefrontal cortical activity
01:59:35.720 | that's involved in attention,
01:59:37.100 | which then logically relates
01:59:40.680 | to one of the outcomes of this study,
01:59:42.840 | which is that attention abilities
01:59:44.640 | improved in daily meditators.
01:59:46.840 | It also points out that increasing the level of attention
01:59:50.640 | and the activity of your prefrontal cortex may,
01:59:53.060 | and I want to emphasize may
01:59:53.980 | because here I'm speculating about the underlying mechanism,
01:59:56.540 | inhibit your ability to fall asleep.
01:59:59.060 | So while we have meditation on the one hand
02:00:01.140 | that does tend to put us into a calm state,
02:00:03.060 | but it is a calm, very focused state.
02:00:05.560 | In fact, attention and focus are inherent
02:00:08.140 | to most forms of meditation.
02:00:10.040 | Non-sleep deep rest, such as yoga nidra,
02:00:12.740 | as some of you know it to be, or NSDR.
02:00:16.060 | There's a terrific NSDR script that's available free online
02:00:19.220 | that's put out by Made For.
02:00:20.420 | So you can go to YouTube, NSDR Made For.
02:00:22.380 | You can also just do a search for NSDR.
02:00:25.100 | There are a number of these available out there,
02:00:26.900 | again, at no cost.
02:00:28.860 | Those NSDR protocols tend to put people
02:00:32.340 | into a state of deep relaxation,
02:00:34.620 | but also very low attention.
02:00:37.960 | And we have to assume very low activation
02:00:41.140 | of the prefrontal cortex.
02:00:42.660 | So the takeaways from the study are several fold.
02:00:44.600 | First of all, that daily meditation of 13 minutes
02:00:48.260 | can enhance your ability to pay attention and to learn.
02:00:51.300 | It can truly enhance memory.
02:00:54.400 | However, you need to do that for at least eight weeks
02:00:57.940 | in order to start to see the effects to occur.
02:01:00.660 | And we have to presume that you have to continue
02:01:02.900 | those meditation training sessions.
02:01:05.280 | In fact, they found that if people
02:01:08.340 | only did four weeks of meditation,
02:01:09.740 | these effects didn't show up.
02:01:11.020 | Now, eight weeks might seem like a long time,
02:01:13.020 | but I think that 13 minutes a day
02:01:14.620 | is not actually that big of a time commitment.
02:01:18.500 | And the results of this study certainly incentivize me
02:01:21.440 | to start adopting a, I'm going for 15 minutes a day now.
02:01:24.860 | I've been a on and off meditator for a number of years.
02:01:27.740 | I've been pretty good about it lately,
02:01:29.180 | but I confess I've been doing far shorter meditations
02:01:31.940 | of anywhere from three to five or maybe 10 minutes.
02:01:34.660 | I'm going to ramp that up to 15 minutes a day.
02:01:37.540 | And I'm doing that specifically to try
02:01:39.540 | and access these improvements in cognitive ability
02:01:42.260 | and our abilities to learn.
02:01:43.700 | Also based on the data in this paper,
02:01:45.740 | I'm going to do those meditation sessions
02:01:47.500 | either early in the day,
02:01:48.980 | such as immediately after waking or close to it.
02:01:52.960 | I might get my sunshine first.
02:01:54.820 | As you all know,
02:01:55.660 | very big on getting sunlight in the eyes early in the day
02:01:57.640 | as much as one can and as early as one can
02:02:00.200 | once the sun is out,
02:02:02.720 | but certainly doing it early in the day
02:02:04.920 | and not past 5 PM or so
02:02:07.640 | in order to make sure that I don't inhibit sleep.
02:02:09.920 | Because I think this result that they describe
02:02:11.940 | of meditation inhibiting quality sleep
02:02:14.880 | compared to controls is an important one
02:02:17.880 | to pay attention to, no pun intended.
02:02:20.180 | Today, we covered a lot of aspects of memory
02:02:22.560 | and how to improve your memory.
02:02:24.480 | We talked about the different forms of memory
02:02:26.260 | and we talked about some of the underlying neural circuitry
02:02:28.840 | of memory formation.
02:02:30.340 | And we talked about how the emotional saliency
02:02:34.020 | and intensity of what you're trying to learn
02:02:36.440 | has a profound impact on whether or not you learn
02:02:40.100 | in response to some sort of experience,
02:02:41.980 | whether or not that experience is reading
02:02:44.540 | or mathematics or music or language or a physical skill.
02:02:49.000 | It doesn't matter.
02:02:50.240 | The more intense of an emotional state that you're in
02:02:54.080 | in the period immediately following that learning,
02:02:57.040 | the more likely you are to remember
02:02:59.420 | whatever it is that you're trying to learn.
02:03:01.440 | And we talked about the neurochemicals
02:03:02.800 | that explain that effect,
02:03:04.560 | about epinephrine and corticosterones like cortisol
02:03:08.320 | and how adjusting the timing of those
02:03:10.580 | is so key to enhancing your memory.
02:03:13.280 | And we talked about the different ways
02:03:14.600 | to enhance those chemicals,
02:03:15.660 | everything ranging from cold water to pharmacology
02:03:19.200 | and even just adjusting the emotional state within your mind
02:03:22.420 | in order to stamp down and remember experiences better.
02:03:26.080 | We also talked about how to leverage exercise
02:03:28.140 | in particular load-bearing exercise
02:03:31.700 | in order to evoke the release of hormones like osteocalcin,
02:03:34.760 | which can travel from your bones to your brain
02:03:36.520 | and enhance your ability to learn.
02:03:38.520 | And we talked about a new form of photographic memory,
02:03:41.000 | not the traditional type of photographic memory
02:03:43.800 | in which people can remember
02:03:44.920 | everything they look at very easily,
02:03:47.160 | but rather taking mental snapshots of things that you see.
02:03:50.460 | Again, emphasizing that that will create a better memory
02:03:53.320 | of what you see when you take that mental snapshot,
02:03:55.560 | but will actually reduce your memory
02:03:57.600 | for the things that you hear at that moment.
02:03:59.660 | And we discussed the really exciting data
02:04:01.240 | looking at how particular meditation protocols
02:04:03.960 | can enhance memory, but also attention and mood.
02:04:07.660 | However, if done too late in the day,
02:04:10.740 | can actually disrupt sleep precisely
02:04:12.780 | because those meditation protocols can enhance attention.
02:04:17.340 | Now, I know that many of you are interested
02:04:18.800 | in neurochemicals that can enhance learning and memory,
02:04:22.380 | and I intend to cover those in deep detail
02:04:26.600 | in a future episode.
02:04:28.000 | However, for sake of what was discussed today,
02:04:30.240 | please understand that any number of different neurochemicals
02:04:34.320 | can evoke or can increase the amount of adrenaline
02:04:37.280 | that's circulating in your brain and body.
02:04:39.600 | And it's less important how one accesses
02:04:43.120 | that increase in adrenaline, right?
02:04:44.980 | Again, this can be done through behavioral protocols
02:04:46.900 | or through pharmacology.
02:04:48.240 | Assuming that those behavioral protocols
02:04:51.840 | and pharmacology are safe for you,
02:04:53.860 | it really doesn't matter how you evoke the adrenaline release
02:04:56.660 | because remember, adrenaline is the final common pathway
02:05:00.160 | by which particular experiences,
02:05:02.280 | particular perceptions are stamped into memory,
02:05:05.640 | which answers our very first question
02:05:07.960 | raised at the beginning of the episode,
02:05:09.300 | which is why do we remember anything at all, right?
02:05:12.380 | That was the question that we raised.
02:05:14.260 | Why is it that from morning till night
02:05:16.080 | and throughout your entire life,
02:05:17.380 | you have tons of sensory experience, tons of perceptions,
02:05:19.720 | why is it that some are remembered and others are not?
02:05:23.160 | While I would never want to distill an important question
02:05:26.740 | such as that down to a one-molecule type of answer,
02:05:29.840 | I think we can confidently say,
02:05:31.760 | based on the vast amount of animal and human research data,
02:05:36.140 | that epinephrine, adrenaline, and some of the other chemicals
02:05:39.900 | that it acts with in concert is in fact the way
02:05:44.380 | that we remember particular events and not all events.
02:05:49.020 | If you're learning from and/or enjoying this podcast,
02:05:51.460 | please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
02:05:53.140 | That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us.
02:05:55.740 | In addition, please subscribe to our podcast
02:05:58.100 | on Spotify and on Apple.
02:06:00.480 | And now on both Spotify and Apple,
02:06:02.640 | you can leave us up to a five-star review.
02:06:05.780 | Please also leave us comments and feedback
02:06:08.180 | in the comment section on our YouTube channel.
02:06:10.380 | You can also suggest future guests
02:06:12.200 | that you'd like us to cover.
02:06:13.140 | We do read all those comments.
02:06:15.460 | Please also check out the sponsors mentioned
02:06:17.180 | at the beginning of today's podcast.
02:06:18.840 | That's terrific, perhaps the best way
02:06:20.880 | to support this podcast.
02:06:22.300 | We also have a Patreon.
02:06:23.520 | It's patreon.com/andrewhuberman,
02:06:26.280 | and there you can support this podcast
02:06:28.060 | at any level that you like.
02:06:29.580 | During today's episode and on many previous episodes
02:06:31.940 | of the Huberman Lab Podcast, we discuss supplements.
02:06:34.880 | While supplements aren't necessary for everybody,
02:06:37.200 | many people derive tremendous benefit from them
02:06:39.320 | for things like enhancing sleep and focus
02:06:41.400 | and indeed for learning and memory.
02:06:43.480 | For that reason, the Huberman Lab Podcast
02:06:45.340 | is now partnered with Momentous Supplements.
02:06:47.760 | The reason we partnered with Momentous is several fold.
02:06:50.420 | First of all, we wanted to have one location
02:06:52.600 | where people could go to access single ingredient,
02:06:55.560 | high quality versions of the supplements
02:06:57.400 | that we were discussing on this podcast.
02:06:59.760 | This is a critical issue.
02:07:00.980 | A lot of supplement companies out there
02:07:02.580 | sell excellent supplements,
02:07:03.780 | but they combine different ingredients
02:07:05.720 | into different formulations,
02:07:07.080 | which make it very hard to figure out
02:07:08.640 | exactly what works for you
02:07:10.120 | and to arrive at the minimal effective dose
02:07:12.800 | of the various compounds that are best for you,
02:07:14.940 | which we think is extremely important.
02:07:16.800 | And that's certainly the most scientific way
02:07:18.760 | or rigorous way to approach
02:07:20.320 | any kind of supplementation regimen.
02:07:22.240 | So Momentous has made these single ingredient formulations
02:07:24.880 | on the basis of what we suggested to them.
02:07:27.260 | And I'm happy to say they also ship internationally.
02:07:30.040 | So whether or not you're in the US or abroad,
02:07:32.060 | they'll ship to you.
02:07:32.920 | If you'd like to see the supplements recommended
02:07:34.700 | on the Huberman Lab Podcast,
02:07:36.460 | you can go to livemomentous.com/huberman.
02:07:40.040 | They've started to assemble the supplements
02:07:42.000 | that we've talked about on the podcast.
02:07:43.500 | And in the upcoming weeks,
02:07:45.180 | they will be adding many more supplements
02:07:47.180 | such that in a brief period of time,
02:07:50.560 | most, if not all of the compounds
02:07:52.300 | that are discussed on this podcast
02:07:54.020 | will be there again in single ingredient,
02:07:56.620 | extremely high quality formulations
02:07:58.480 | that you can use to arrive
02:07:59.660 | at the best supplement protocols for you.
02:08:01.980 | We also include behavioral protocols
02:08:04.100 | that can be combined with supplementation protocols
02:08:06.520 | in order to deliver the maximum effect.
02:08:08.800 | Once again, that's livemomentous.com/huberman.
02:08:12.140 | And if you're not already following us
02:08:13.620 | on Twitter and Instagram,
02:08:15.200 | it's Huberman Lab on both Twitter and Instagram.
02:08:18.140 | There, I describe science and science-related tools,
02:08:21.220 | some of which overlap with the content
02:08:22.820 | of the Huberman Lab Podcast,
02:08:24.060 | but much of which is distinct
02:08:25.440 | from the content of the Huberman Lab Podcast.
02:08:27.780 | We also have a newsletter
02:08:29.180 | called the Huberman Lab Neural Network.
02:08:30.720 | That newsletter provides summary protocols
02:08:33.880 | and information from our various podcast episodes.
02:08:36.880 | It does not cost anything to sign up.
02:08:38.480 | You can go to HubermanLab.com,
02:08:40.400 | go to the menu and click on newsletter.
02:08:42.700 | You just provide your email.
02:08:43.780 | And I should point out,
02:08:44.620 | we do not share your email with anyone else.
02:08:46.420 | We have a very clear privacy policy
02:08:48.780 | that you can read there.
02:08:50.300 | And that newsletter comes out about once a month.
02:08:52.260 | You can also see some sample newsletters,
02:08:53.960 | things like the toolkit for sleep or for neuroplasticity
02:08:57.300 | and for various other topics
02:08:58.520 | covered on the Huberman Lab Podcast.
02:09:00.540 | Once again, thank you for joining me today
02:09:02.540 | to discuss the neurobiology of learning and memory
02:09:04.780 | and how to improve your memory using science-based tools.
02:09:08.580 | And last, but certainly not least,
02:09:10.620 | thank you for your interest in science.
02:09:12.480 | [upbeat music]
02:09:15.060 | (upbeat music)