back to index

Dr. David Berson: Your Brain's Logic & Function | Huberman Lab Podcast #50


Chapters

0:0 Dr. David Berson
2:55 Sponsors: Athletic Greens, InsideTracker, Magic Spoon
8:2 How We See
10:2 Color Vision
13:47 “Strange” Vision
16:56 How You Orient In Time
25:45 Body Rhythms, Pineal function, Light & Melatonin, Blueblockers
34:45 Spending Times Outdoors Improves Eyesight
36:20 Sensation, Mood, & Self-Image
41:3 Sense of Balance
50:43 Why Pigeons Bob Their Heads, Motion Sickness
60:3 Popping Ears
62:35 Midbrain & Blindsight
70:44 Why Tilted Motion Feels Good
73:24 Reflexes vs. Deliberate Actions
76:35 Basal Ganglia & the “2 Marshmallow Test”
84:40 Suppressing Reflexes: Cortex
93:33 Neuroplasticity
96:27 What is a Connectome?
105:20 How to Learn (More About the Brain)
109:4 Book Suggestion, my Berson Appreciation
110:20 Zero-Cost ways to Support the HLP, Guest Suggestions, Sponsors, Patreon, Thorne

Whisper Transcript | Transcript Only Page

00:00:00.280 | - Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
00:00:02.280 | where we discuss science and science-based tools
00:00:04.880 | for everyday life.
00:00:05.900 | I'm Andrew Huberman,
00:00:10.240 | and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
00:00:13.020 | at Stanford School of Medicine.
00:00:14.920 | Today, my guest is Dr. David Berson,
00:00:17.160 | professor of medical science, neurobiology,
00:00:19.480 | and ophthalmology at Brown University.
00:00:22.240 | Dr. Berson's laboratory is credited
00:00:24.020 | with discovering the cells in the eye
00:00:25.960 | that set your circadian rhythms.
00:00:28.180 | These are the so-called intrinsically
00:00:30.080 | photosensitive melanopsin cells,
00:00:32.020 | and while that's a mouthful,
00:00:33.720 | all you need to know for sake of this introduction
00:00:36.140 | is that those are the cells that inform your brain and body
00:00:38.760 | about the time of day.
00:00:40.560 | Dr. Berson's laboratory has also made
00:00:42.480 | a number of other important discoveries
00:00:44.240 | about how we convert our perceptions of the outside world
00:00:47.880 | into motor action.
00:00:50.080 | More personally, Dr. Berson has been my go-to resource
00:00:53.240 | for all things neuroscience for nearly two decades.
00:00:57.160 | I knew of his reputation as a spectacular researcher
00:01:00.180 | for a long period of time,
00:01:01.500 | and then many years ago, I cold called him out of the blue.
00:01:05.080 | I basically corralled him into a long conversation
00:01:08.220 | over the phone after which he invited me out to Brown,
00:01:10.920 | and we've been discussing neuroscience
00:01:13.020 | and how the brain works and the emerging new technologies
00:01:16.700 | and the emerging new concepts in neuroscience
00:01:19.440 | for a very long time now.
00:01:22.020 | You're going to realize today
00:01:23.100 | why Dr. Berson is my go-to source.
00:01:25.600 | He has an exceptionally clear and organized view
00:01:29.260 | of how the nervous system works.
00:01:31.360 | Now, there are many, many parts of the nervous system,
00:01:33.520 | different nuclei and connections and circuits and chemicals
00:01:36.420 | and so forth, but it takes a special kind of person
00:01:39.180 | to be able to organize that information
00:01:41.240 | into a structured and logical framework
00:01:43.860 | that can allow us to make sense of how we function
00:01:46.980 | in terms of what we feel, what we experience,
00:01:49.140 | how we move through the world.
00:01:51.020 | Dr. Berson is truly one of a kind in his ability
00:01:54.040 | to synthesize and organize and communicate that information,
00:01:57.380 | and I give him credit as one of my mentors
00:02:00.100 | and one of the people that I respect most
00:02:02.260 | in the field of science and medical science generally.
00:02:05.220 | Today, Dr. Berson takes us on a journey
00:02:07.580 | from the periphery of the nervous system,
00:02:09.340 | meaning from the outside, deep into the nervous system,
00:02:12.720 | layer by layer, structure by structure, circuit by circuit,
00:02:16.660 | making clear to us how each of these individual circuits
00:02:19.540 | work and how they work together as a whole.
00:02:22.420 | It's a really magnificent description
00:02:24.480 | that you simply cannot get from any textbook,
00:02:27.420 | from any popular book, and frankly, as far as I know,
00:02:30.280 | from any podcast that currently exists out there.
00:02:33.120 | So it's a real gift to have this opportunity
00:02:34.880 | to learn from Dr. Berson.
00:02:36.540 | Again, I consider him my mentor in the field
00:02:39.340 | of learning and teaching neuroscience,
00:02:41.420 | and I'm excited for you to learn from him.
00:02:43.780 | One thing is for certain, by the end of this podcast,
00:02:46.680 | you will know far more about how your nervous system works
00:02:49.560 | than the vast majority of people out there,
00:02:51.620 | including many expert biologists and neuroscientists.
00:02:55.320 | Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast
00:02:58.020 | is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
00:03:00.940 | It is, however, part of my desire and effort
00:03:02.880 | to bring zero cost to consumer information about science
00:03:05.460 | and science-related tools to the general public.
00:03:08.220 | In keeping with that theme,
00:03:09.220 | I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
00:03:12.000 | Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens.
00:03:14.260 | Athletic Greens is an all-in-one
00:03:15.860 | vitamin mineral probiotic drink.
00:03:18.300 | I've been taking Athletic Greens every day since 2012,
00:03:21.500 | so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.
00:03:23.900 | The reason I started taking Athletic Greens
00:03:25.700 | and the reason I still take Athletic Greens
00:03:27.660 | is that it covers all of my vitamin mineral
00:03:29.740 | and probiotic needs.
00:03:31.480 | Nowadays, there's a lot of data out there
00:03:33.560 | pointing to the fact that a healthy gut microbiome,
00:03:36.240 | literally little microbes that live in our gut
00:03:38.280 | that are good for us, is important to support
00:03:40.700 | our immune system, our nervous system, our endocrine system,
00:03:43.620 | and various aspects of our immediate and long-term health.
00:03:46.940 | With Athletic Greens,
00:03:47.780 | I get all the vitamins and minerals that I need,
00:03:49.820 | plus the probiotics ensure a healthy gut microbiome.
00:03:53.540 | It also tastes really good.
00:03:54.620 | I mix mine up with some water, a little bit of lemon juice.
00:03:56.900 | I'll have that early in the day
00:03:58.240 | and sometimes a second time later in the day as well.
00:04:00.740 | It's compatible with intermittent fasting.
00:04:02.580 | It's compatible with vegan diets, with keto diets,
00:04:05.960 | and essentially every diet
00:04:07.140 | that you could possibly imagine out there.
00:04:09.860 | It's also filled with adaptogens for recovery.
00:04:12.180 | It has digestive enzymes for gut health
00:04:14.040 | and has a number of other things
00:04:15.100 | that support the immune system.
00:04:16.660 | If you'd like to try Athletic Greens,
00:04:18.900 | you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman
00:04:21.460 | to claim their special offer.
00:04:22.940 | They'll give you five free travel packs
00:04:24.460 | that make it really easy to mix up Athletic Greens
00:04:26.340 | while you're on the road,
00:04:27.460 | and they'll give you a year's supply of vitamin D3 K2.
00:04:30.940 | There's now a lot of evidence that vitamin D3
00:04:33.780 | supports a huge number of metabolic factors,
00:04:37.520 | immune system factors, endocrine factors.
00:04:39.640 | Basically, we need vitamin D3.
00:04:41.380 | We can get it from the sun,
00:04:42.700 | but many people are deficient in vitamin D3
00:04:45.180 | even if they are getting
00:04:46.380 | what they think is sufficient sunlight.
00:04:48.300 | And K2 is important for cardiovascular health.
00:04:50.720 | So again, if you go to athleticgreens.com/huberman,
00:04:53.620 | you can claim their special offer,
00:04:55.140 | the five free travel packs,
00:04:56.300 | plus the year supply of vitamin D3 K2.
00:04:59.180 | Today's podcast is also brought to us by InsideTracker.
00:05:02.380 | InsideTracker is a personalized nutrition platform
00:05:04.860 | that analyzes data from your blood and DNA
00:05:07.300 | to help you better understand your body
00:05:08.820 | and help you reach your health goals.
00:05:10.740 | I've long been a believer in getting regular blood work done
00:05:13.660 | for the simple reason that many of the factors
00:05:16.220 | that impact your immediate and long-term health
00:05:18.540 | can only be analyzed from a quality blood test.
00:05:21.100 | And now with the advent of modern DNA tests,
00:05:23.580 | you can also get information about how your specific genes
00:05:26.180 | are impacting your immediate and long-term health.
00:05:28.740 | Now, a problem with a lot of blood tests
00:05:30.220 | and DNA tests out there is you get the numbers back,
00:05:32.700 | but you don't know what to do with those numbers.
00:05:34.700 | With InsideTracker, they make it very simple
00:05:37.380 | to figure out what to do to bring those numbers
00:05:39.620 | into the ranges that are right for you.
00:05:41.580 | They have a dashboard that's very easy to use.
00:05:44.300 | You can see the numbers from your blood and or DNA tests,
00:05:47.780 | and it will point to specific lifestyle factors,
00:05:50.420 | nutritional factors, as well as supplementation,
00:05:53.260 | maybe even prescription factors that would be right for you
00:05:55.920 | to bring the numbers into range that are ideal
00:05:57.940 | for your immediate and long-term health goals.
00:06:00.360 | Another feature that InsideTracker has
00:06:02.040 | is their inner age test.
00:06:03.920 | This test shows you what your biological age is
00:06:06.180 | and compares that to your chronological age
00:06:08.500 | and what you can do to improve your biological age,
00:06:11.220 | which of course is the important number.
00:06:13.100 | If you'd like to try InsideTracker,
00:06:14.460 | you can visit insidetracker.com/huberman
00:06:17.100 | to get 25% off any of InsideTracker's plans.
00:06:20.060 | Also, an interview I did with longevity research doctor
00:06:23.380 | and InsideTracker's founder, Dr. Gil Blander,
00:06:25.860 | is out now on their podcast,
00:06:27.540 | the Longevity by Design podcast,
00:06:29.580 | and a link to that interview can be found
00:06:31.360 | in today's show notes.
00:06:32.820 | Today's episode is also brought to us by Magic Spoon.
00:06:35.940 | Magic Spoon is a zero-sugar,
00:06:37.700 | grain-free, keto-friendly cereal.
00:06:40.140 | I don't follow a ketogenic diet.
00:06:41.880 | The way that I eat is basically geared toward
00:06:44.120 | feeling alert when I want to be alert
00:06:46.100 | and feeling sleepy when I want to go to sleep,
00:06:48.000 | which for me means fasting until about 11 a.m. or noon,
00:06:50.620 | most days.
00:06:51.700 | Then I eat low carb during the day.
00:06:53.940 | So I'll have some meat or fish or chicken and some salad.
00:06:56.580 | That's what works for me.
00:06:57.460 | And in the afternoon,
00:06:58.300 | I remain on a more or less low carb-ish diet.
00:07:00.940 | And then in the evening, I eat pastas and things primarily,
00:07:03.960 | and I throttle back on the protein,
00:07:05.380 | and that's what allows me to fall asleep at night.
00:07:07.140 | That's just what works for me.
00:07:08.700 | So if I want a snack in the afternoon,
00:07:10.740 | I want that to be a ketogenic or low carb snack.
00:07:14.080 | And that snack these days is Magic Spoon.
00:07:16.820 | Magic Spoon is really terrific.
00:07:18.140 | It has zero grams of sugar, 13 to 14 grams of protein,
00:07:21.120 | and only four net grams of carbohydrates in each serving.
00:07:24.200 | It's really delicious.
00:07:25.120 | They have a number of different flavors like cocoa,
00:07:27.200 | fruity, peanut butter, and frosted.
00:07:29.300 | I particularly like frosted, tastes like donuts,
00:07:32.140 | and I really like donuts,
00:07:33.240 | although I try not to eat donuts too often, if ever.
00:07:36.540 | What I do lately is I take Magic Spoon,
00:07:38.580 | I put it in some Bulgarian yogurt, which is really good,
00:07:40.960 | and I mix that up, I put those in there,
00:07:42.660 | and sometimes I put some cinnamon on them,
00:07:43.980 | and as I'm describing this,
00:07:44.860 | I'm getting hungry for Magic Spoon.
00:07:46.160 | So if you want to try Magic Spoon,
00:07:47.960 | you can go to magicspoon.com/huberman
00:07:50.920 | to get their variety pack.
00:07:52.140 | Just use the promo code Huberman
00:07:54.020 | at checkout to get $5 off your order.
00:07:56.300 | Again, that's magicspoon.com/huberman,
00:07:58.940 | and use the code Huberman to get $5 off.
00:08:01.740 | And now for my discussion with Dr. David Berson.
00:08:05.140 | Welcome.
00:08:06.020 | - Thank you. - Yeah.
00:08:06.940 | - So nice to be here.
00:08:08.120 | - Great to have you.
00:08:09.200 | For more than 20 years,
00:08:11.900 | you've been my go-to source for all things,
00:08:14.260 | nervous system, how it works, how it's structured.
00:08:17.120 | So today I want to ask you some questions about that.
00:08:20.120 | I think people would gain a lot of insight
00:08:22.900 | into this machine that makes them think,
00:08:24.940 | and feel, and see, et cetera.
00:08:27.180 | If you would, could you tell us how we see?
00:08:31.760 | You know, a photon of light enters the eye.
00:08:36.300 | What happens?
00:08:37.480 | - Right.
00:08:38.320 | - I mean, how is it that I look outside,
00:08:40.020 | I see a truck drive by, or I look on the wall,
00:08:42.200 | I see a photo of my dog.
00:08:44.400 | How does that work?
00:08:46.320 | - Right, so this is an old question, obviously.
00:08:49.100 | And clearly in the end,
00:08:50.900 | the reason you have a visual experience
00:08:52.780 | is that your brain has got some pattern of activity
00:08:56.120 | that it associates with the input from the periphery.
00:08:59.100 | But you can have a visual experience
00:09:00.760 | with no input from the periphery as well.
00:09:02.560 | When you're dreaming, you're seeing things
00:09:06.000 | that aren't coming through your eyes.
00:09:07.480 | - Are those memories?
00:09:08.520 | - I would say in a sense,
00:09:10.700 | they may reflect your visual experience.
00:09:13.140 | They're not necessarily specific visual memories,
00:09:15.120 | but of course they can be.
00:09:16.720 | But the point is that the experience of seeing
00:09:18.880 | is actually a brain phenomenon.
00:09:21.400 | But of course, under normal circumstances,
00:09:24.520 | we see the world because we're looking at it,
00:09:26.480 | and we're using our eyes to look at it.
00:09:28.500 | And fundamentally, when we're looking at the exterior world,
00:09:32.240 | it's what the retina is telling the brain that matters.
00:09:35.020 | So there are cells called ganglion cells.
00:09:37.860 | These are neurons that are the key cells
00:09:40.480 | for communicating between eye and brain.
00:09:43.100 | The eye is like the camera.
00:09:44.420 | It's detecting the initial image,
00:09:46.420 | doing some initial processing,
00:09:47.660 | and then that signal gets sent back to the brain proper.
00:09:51.480 | And of course, it's there at the level of the cortex
00:09:54.320 | that we have this conscious visual experience.
00:09:56.200 | There are many other places in the brain
00:09:57.720 | that get visual input as well,
00:09:59.740 | doing other things with that kind of information.
00:10:02.740 | - So I get a lot of questions about color vision.
00:10:05.980 | If you would, could you explain how is it
00:10:09.060 | that we can perceive reds and greens and blues
00:10:12.320 | and things of that sort?
00:10:13.460 | - Right.
00:10:14.300 | So the first thing to understand about light
00:10:16.380 | is that it's just a form of electromagnetic radiation.
00:10:19.900 | It's vibrating, it's oscillating.
00:10:24.240 | - When you say it's vibrating, it's oscillating,
00:10:25.520 | you mean that photons are actually moving?
00:10:28.480 | - Well, in a sense, photons are,
00:10:29.800 | they're certainly moving through space.
00:10:31.840 | We think about photons as particles,
00:10:34.440 | and that's one way of thinking about light,
00:10:36.020 | but we can also think of it as a wave, like a radio wave.
00:10:39.200 | Either way is acceptable.
00:10:40.860 | And the radio waves have frequencies,
00:10:42.540 | like the frequencies on your radio dial.
00:10:45.260 | And certain frequencies in the electromagnetic spectrum
00:10:48.380 | can be detected by neurons in the retina.
00:10:50.960 | Those are the things we see.
00:10:52.940 | But there are still different wavelengths
00:10:54.620 | within the light that can be seen by the eye.
00:10:57.780 | And those different wavelengths are unpacked in a sense,
00:11:00.500 | or decoded by the nervous system
00:11:03.540 | to lead to our experience of color.
00:11:06.400 | Essentially, different wavelengths give us the sensation
00:11:10.480 | of different colors through the auspices
00:11:13.660 | of different neurons that are tuned
00:11:15.880 | to different wavelengths of light.
00:11:17.660 | - So when a photon,
00:11:20.060 | so when a little bit of light hits my eye, goes in,
00:11:23.760 | the photoreceptors convert that into electrical signal.
00:11:26.440 | - Right.
00:11:27.280 | - How is it that a given photon of light
00:11:30.700 | gives me the perception,
00:11:31.920 | eventually leads to the perception
00:11:33.160 | of red versus green versus blue?
00:11:35.200 | - Right.
00:11:36.040 | So if you imagine that in the first layer of the retina
00:11:40.640 | where this transformation occurs
00:11:42.320 | from electromagnetic radiation into neural signals,
00:11:45.200 | that you have different kinds of sensitive cells
00:11:49.740 | that are expressing,
00:11:51.120 | they're making different molecules within themselves
00:11:54.920 | for this express purpose of absorbing photons,
00:11:58.320 | which is the first step in the process of seeing.
00:12:01.320 | Now, it turns out that altogether
00:12:03.880 | there are about five proteins like this
00:12:06.660 | that we need to think about in the typical retina.
00:12:09.660 | But for seeing color, really it's three of them.
00:12:11.840 | So there are three different proteins.
00:12:13.300 | Each absorbs light with a different preferred frequency.
00:12:17.540 | And then the nervous system keeps track of those signals,
00:12:20.600 | compares and contrasts them
00:12:23.980 | to extract some understanding
00:12:25.680 | of the wavelength composition of light.
00:12:28.080 | So you can see just by looking at a landscape,
00:12:30.600 | oh, it must be late in the day
00:12:33.360 | because things are looking golden.
00:12:34.920 | That's all a function of our absorbing
00:12:38.080 | the light that's coming from the world
00:12:39.920 | and interpreting that with our brain
00:12:41.680 | because of the different composition
00:12:43.040 | of the light that's reaching our eyes.
00:12:45.980 | - Is it fair to assume that my perception of red
00:12:48.160 | is the same as your perception of red?
00:12:50.200 | - Well, that's a great question.
00:12:51.360 | - And that mine is better.
00:12:52.560 | - No, I'm just kidding.
00:12:54.220 | It's a great question.
00:12:56.100 | It's a deep philosophical question.
00:12:57.460 | It's a question that really probably
00:12:59.000 | can't even ultimately be answered
00:13:01.140 | by the usual empirical scientific processes
00:13:05.180 | 'cause it's really about an individual's experience.
00:13:08.420 | What we can say is that the biological mechanisms
00:13:14.340 | that we think are important for seeing color, for example,
00:13:17.440 | seem to be very highly similar
00:13:19.520 | from one individual to the next,
00:13:20.980 | whether it be human beings or other animals.
00:13:23.180 | And so we think that the physiological process
00:13:27.500 | looks very similar on the front end,
00:13:29.780 | but once you're at the level of perception
00:13:32.220 | or understanding or experience,
00:13:34.920 | that's something that's a little bit tougher to nail down
00:13:38.480 | with the sorts of scientific approaches
00:13:42.560 | that we approach biological vision with, let's say.
00:13:46.100 | - You mentioned that there are five
00:13:47.880 | different cone types, essentially,
00:13:49.940 | cones being the cells that absorb light
00:13:51.620 | of different wavelengths.
00:13:52.820 | I often wondered when I had my dog what he saw
00:13:58.820 | and how his vision differs from our vision.
00:14:01.940 | And certainly there are animals that can see things
00:14:04.220 | that we can't see.
00:14:05.620 | What are some of the more outrageous examples of that?
00:14:09.140 | - Of seeing things that we can't.
00:14:10.940 | - And in the extreme.
00:14:12.260 | Dogs, I'm guessing, see reds more as oranges.
00:14:16.620 | Is that right?
00:14:17.580 | 'Cause they don't have the same array of neurons
00:14:21.340 | that we have for seeing color.
00:14:22.900 | - Right, so the first thing is it's not really
00:14:25.160 | five types of cones.
00:14:26.980 | There are really three types of cones.
00:14:29.020 | And if you look at the way that color vision
00:14:31.140 | is thought to work, you can sort of see
00:14:32.720 | that it has to be three different signals.
00:14:35.120 | There are a couple of other types of pigments.
00:14:37.300 | One is really mostly for dim light vision.
00:14:40.660 | When you're walking around in a moonless night
00:14:42.780 | and you're seeing things with very low light,
00:14:45.720 | that's the rod cell that uses its own pigment.
00:14:49.220 | And then there's another class of pigments
00:14:51.380 | we'll probably talk about a little bit later,
00:14:52.960 | this melanopsin pigment.
00:14:54.380 | - I thought you were referring to like ultraviolet
00:14:56.240 | and infrared and things of that sort.
00:14:58.500 | - Right, so in the case of a typical,
00:15:01.660 | well, let's put it this way, in human beings,
00:15:04.300 | most of us have three cone types
00:15:06.540 | and we can see colors that stem from that.
00:15:10.860 | In most mammals, including your dog or your cat,
00:15:15.860 | there really are only two cone types.
00:15:18.540 | And that limits the kind of vision that they can have
00:15:21.860 | in the domain of wavelength or color, as you would say.
00:15:25.080 | So really a dog sees the world kind of like
00:15:27.960 | a particular kind of colorblind human might see the world
00:15:31.720 | because instead of having three channels
00:15:33.300 | to compare and contrast, they only have two channels.
00:15:35.760 | And that makes it much more difficult to figure out
00:15:37.480 | exactly which wavelength you're looking at.
00:15:40.140 | - Do colorblind people suffer much
00:15:42.320 | as a consequence of being colorblind?
00:15:44.460 | - Well, it's like so many other disabilities.
00:15:47.420 | We are, the world is built for people
00:15:52.420 | of the most common type.
00:15:54.860 | So in some cases, the expectation can be there
00:15:59.660 | that somebody can see something that they won't be able to
00:16:01.980 | if they're missing one of their cone types, let's say.
00:16:04.460 | So in those moments, that can be a real problem.
00:16:08.740 | If there's a lack of contrast to their visual system,
00:16:12.500 | they will be blind to that.
00:16:14.020 | In general, it's a fairly modest visual limitation
00:16:19.020 | as things go.
00:16:20.640 | For example, if not being able to see acutely
00:16:23.020 | can be much more damaging,
00:16:24.180 | not being able to read fine print, for example.
00:16:26.900 | - Yeah, I suppose if I had to give up
00:16:28.700 | the ability to see certain colors
00:16:31.660 | or give up the ability to see clearly,
00:16:33.380 | I'd certainly trade out color for clarity.
00:16:37.500 | - Right, of course, color is very meaningful to us
00:16:40.020 | as human beings, so we would hate to give it up.
00:16:43.920 | But obviously dogs and cats and all kinds of other mammals
00:16:46.580 | do perfectly well in the world.
00:16:48.100 | - Yeah, because we take care of them.
00:16:49.260 | I spent most of my time taking care of that dog.
00:16:51.660 | He took care of me too.
00:16:54.260 | Let's talk about that odd photopigment.
00:16:59.920 | Photopigment, of course, being the thing that absorbs light
00:17:03.540 | of a particular wavelength.
00:17:05.100 | And let's talk about these specialized ganglion cells
00:17:09.060 | that communicate certain types of information
00:17:12.380 | from eye to the brain that are so important
00:17:15.580 | for so many things.
00:17:16.660 | What I'm referring to here, of course,
00:17:17.960 | is your co-discovery of the so-called
00:17:20.120 | intrinsically photosensitive cells,
00:17:22.260 | the neurons in the eye that do so many of the things
00:17:25.020 | that don't actually have to do with perception,
00:17:27.540 | but have to do with important biological functions.
00:17:30.260 | What I would love for you to do is explain to me
00:17:33.220 | why once I heard you say,
00:17:35.100 | we have a bit of fly eye in our eye.
00:17:38.880 | And you showed this slide of like a giant fly
00:17:42.220 | from a horror movie trying to attack this woman.
00:17:46.260 | And maybe it was an eye also.
00:17:48.600 | So what does it mean that we have a bit of a fly eye
00:17:52.080 | in our eye?
00:17:53.140 | - Yeah, so this last pigment is a really peculiar one.
00:18:01.540 | One can think about it as really the initial
00:18:04.460 | sensitive element in a system that's designed
00:18:07.120 | to tell your brain about how bright things are
00:18:09.640 | in your world.
00:18:10.560 | And the thing that's really peculiar about this pigment
00:18:15.260 | is that it's in the wrong place, in a sense.
00:18:18.460 | When you think about the structure of the retina,
00:18:20.620 | you think about a layer cake, essentially.
00:18:22.560 | You've got this thin membrane at the back of your eye,
00:18:25.340 | but it's actually a stack of thin layers.
00:18:27.820 | And the outermost of those layers
00:18:29.460 | is where these photoreceptors you were talking about earlier
00:18:31.940 | are sitting.
00:18:32.780 | That's where the film of your camera is, essentially.
00:18:35.420 | That's where the photons do their magic
00:18:37.020 | with the photopigments and turn it into a neural signal.
00:18:39.140 | - I like that.
00:18:39.980 | I've never really thought of the photoreceptors
00:18:41.020 | as the film of the camera, but that makes sense.
00:18:42.780 | - Yeah, or like the sensitive chip,
00:18:44.820 | CCD chip in your cell phone.
00:18:47.220 | It's the surface on which the light pattern
00:18:49.420 | is imaged by the optics of the eye.
00:18:52.240 | And now you've got an array of sensors
00:18:54.180 | that's capturing that information
00:18:56.260 | and creating a bitmap, essentially.
00:18:59.100 | But now it's in neural signals distributed
00:19:01.060 | across the surface of the retina.
00:19:03.260 | So all of that was known to be going on 150 years ago.
00:19:07.040 | A couple of types of photoreceptors, cones and rods.
00:19:10.740 | If you look a little bit more closely, three types of cones.
00:19:13.820 | That's where the transformation
00:19:15.580 | from electromagnetic radiation to neural signals
00:19:20.480 | was thought to take place.
00:19:22.520 | But it turns out that this last photopigment
00:19:24.680 | is in the other end of the retina,
00:19:27.060 | the innermost part of the retina.
00:19:28.500 | That's where the so-called ganglion cells are.
00:19:30.540 | Those are the cells that talk to the brain,
00:19:32.140 | the ones that actually can communicate directly
00:19:34.860 | what information comes to them from the photoreceptors.
00:19:38.220 | And here you've got a case
00:19:39.900 | where actually some of the output neurons
00:19:43.100 | that we didn't think had any business
00:19:44.780 | being directly sensitive to light
00:19:46.980 | were actually making this photopigment,
00:19:50.060 | absorbing light and converting that to neural signals
00:19:53.420 | and sending it to the brain.
00:19:54.900 | So that made it pretty surprising and unexpected,
00:19:58.640 | but there are many surprising things about these cells.
00:20:01.800 | So, and what is the relationship to the fly eye?
00:20:05.140 | Right, so the link there is that if you ask
00:20:08.180 | how the photopigment now communicates downstream
00:20:13.180 | from the initial absorption event
00:20:15.960 | to get to the electrical signal,
00:20:17.560 | that's a complex cellular process,
00:20:19.500 | involves many chemical steps.
00:20:22.260 | And if you look at how photoreceptors in our eyes work,
00:20:26.640 | you can see what that cascade is, how that chain works.
00:20:30.340 | If you look in the eyes of flies
00:20:33.400 | or other insects or other invertebrates,
00:20:36.660 | there's a very similar kind of chain,
00:20:38.740 | but the specifics of how the signals get
00:20:41.460 | from the absorption event by the pigment
00:20:43.600 | to the electrical response
00:20:44.860 | that the nervous system can understand
00:20:47.040 | are characteristically different
00:20:48.900 | between fuzzy furry creatures like us
00:20:53.740 | and insects, for example, like the fly.
00:20:56.500 | I see.
00:20:57.340 | So these funny extra photoreceptors
00:21:00.640 | that are in the wrong layer
00:21:01.740 | doing something completely different
00:21:03.060 | are actually using a chemical cascade
00:21:06.740 | that looks much more like what you would see
00:21:08.300 | in a fly photoreceptor
00:21:10.100 | than what you would see in a human photoreceptor,
00:21:12.900 | a rod or a cone, for example.
00:21:14.580 | So it sounds like it's a very primitive aspect
00:21:18.700 | of biology that we maintain.
00:21:20.380 | - Exactly right.
00:21:22.100 | - And despite the fact
00:21:22.940 | that dogs can't see as many colors as we can
00:21:25.060 | and cats can't see as many colors as we can,
00:21:26.860 | we have all this extravagant stuff for seeing color.
00:21:29.760 | And then you've got this other pigment
00:21:31.560 | sitting in the wrong, not wrong,
00:21:33.700 | but in a different part of the eye,
00:21:36.980 | sending, processing light very differently
00:21:40.300 | and sending that information into the brain.
00:21:42.460 | So what do these cells do?
00:21:45.580 | I mean, presumably they're there for a reason.
00:21:47.820 | - They are.
00:21:48.740 | And the interesting thing is that one cell type like this,
00:21:53.740 | carrying one kind of signal,
00:21:56.300 | which I would call a brightness signal, essentially,
00:22:00.220 | can do many things in the brain.
00:22:02.120 | - When you say brightness signal,
00:22:03.180 | you mean that it, like right now, I have these cells.
00:22:05.940 | Do I have these cells?
00:22:06.780 | Of course not. - You do.
00:22:07.620 | - I'm joking, I hope I have these cells in my eye.
00:22:09.760 | And they're paying attention to how bright it is overall,
00:22:12.760 | but they're not paying attention, for instance,
00:22:14.060 | to the edge of your ear
00:22:15.140 | or what else is going on in the room.
00:22:16.740 | - Right, so it's the difference between
00:22:19.140 | knowing what the objects are on the table
00:22:21.740 | and knowing whether it's bright enough
00:22:23.180 | to be daylight right now.
00:22:25.820 | So why does your nervous system need to know
00:22:28.960 | whether it's daylight right now?
00:22:30.700 | Well, one thing that needs to know,
00:22:31.980 | that is your circadian clock.
00:22:33.980 | You know, if you travel across time zones to Europe,
00:22:37.420 | now your internal clock thinks it's California time,
00:22:41.940 | but the rotation of the earth is, you know,
00:22:44.880 | for a different part of the planet.
00:22:46.420 | You know, the rising and setting of the sun
00:22:47.860 | is not at all what your body is anticipating.
00:22:50.280 | So you've got an internal representation
00:22:52.100 | of the rotation of the earth in your own brain.
00:22:54.640 | That's your circadian system.
00:22:56.180 | It's keeping time.
00:22:58.660 | But now you've played a trick on your nervous system.
00:23:00.900 | You put yourself in a different place
00:23:02.360 | where the sun is rising at the, quote, wrong time.
00:23:05.660 | Well, that's not good for you, right?
00:23:07.620 | So you gotta get back on track.
00:23:08.980 | One of the things this system does is sends a,
00:23:12.020 | oh, it's daylight now signal to the brain,
00:23:14.980 | which compares with its internal clock.
00:23:16.860 | And if that's not right,
00:23:18.420 | it tweaks the clock gradually
00:23:20.460 | until you get over your jet lag
00:23:21.840 | and you feel back on track again.
00:23:24.100 | So the jet lag case makes a lot of sense to me,
00:23:26.980 | but presumably these elements didn't evolve for jet lag.
00:23:31.380 | Right.
00:23:32.220 | So what are they doing on a day-to-day basis?
00:23:35.900 | Right, well, one way to think about this
00:23:37.460 | is that the clock that you have in not just your brain,
00:23:42.980 | in all the cells, or almost all of the cells of your body,
00:23:45.660 | they're all oscillating, they're all, you know.
00:23:49.020 | They got little clocks in them.
00:23:49.860 | They got little clocks in them themselves.
00:23:51.660 | They're all clocks.
00:23:52.780 | You know, they need to be synchronized appropriately.
00:23:58.820 | And the whole thing has to be built in biological machinery.
00:24:04.620 | This is actually a beautiful story
00:24:07.380 | about how gene expression can control gene expression.
00:24:10.940 | And if you set it up right,
00:24:12.060 | you can set up a little thing
00:24:13.420 | that just sort of hums along at a particular frequency.
00:24:16.940 | In our case, it's humming along at 24 hours,
00:24:19.340 | 'cause that's how our earth rotates,
00:24:21.060 | and it's all built into our biology.
00:24:23.460 | So this is great,
00:24:24.840 | but the reality is that the clock can only be so good.
00:24:28.060 | I mean, we're talking about biology here.
00:24:29.620 | It's not precision engineering,
00:24:32.380 | and so it can be a little bit off.
00:24:34.140 | Well, also it doesn't, it's in our brain,
00:24:35.900 | so it doesn't have access to any regular unerring signal.
00:24:39.620 | Well, if in the absence of the rising and setting
00:24:42.460 | of the sun, it doesn't.
00:24:43.460 | If you put someone in a cave,
00:24:45.560 | their biological clock will keep time
00:24:47.820 | to within a handful of minutes of 24 hours.
00:24:51.860 | That's no problem for one day.
00:24:55.020 | But if this went on without any correction,
00:24:57.280 | eventually you'd be out of phase.
00:24:59.220 | And this is actually one of the things
00:25:00.500 | that blind patients often complain about.
00:25:03.540 | If they've got retinal blindness, is insomnia.
00:25:08.540 | Or sleep and history in the middle of the night.
00:25:10.140 | Exactly, they're not synchronized.
00:25:11.740 | Their clock is there, but they're drifting out of phase
00:25:14.740 | because their clock's only good to 24.2 hours
00:25:19.700 | or 23.8 hours.
00:25:21.900 | Little by little, they're drifting.
00:25:24.140 | So you need a synchronization signal.
00:25:26.020 | So even if you never across time,
00:25:27.580 | as I was saying, of course, we didn't back on the savanna.
00:25:29.680 | We stayed within walking distance of where we were.
00:25:32.620 | You still need a synchronizer
00:25:35.620 | 'cause otherwise you have nothing to actually confirm
00:25:39.140 | when the rising and the setting of the sun is.
00:25:41.580 | That's what you're trying to synchronize yourself to.
00:25:44.340 | I'm fascinated by the circadian clock
00:25:47.100 | and the fact that all the cells of our body
00:25:50.100 | have essentially a 24 hour-ish clock in them.
00:25:53.340 | Right.
00:25:54.460 | We hear a lot about these circadian rhythms
00:25:56.180 | and circadian clocks, the fact that we need light input
00:25:59.180 | from these special neurons in order to set the clock.
00:26:02.300 | But I've never really heard it describe
00:26:04.300 | how the clock itself works
00:26:06.460 | and how the clock signals to all the rest of the body
00:26:10.180 | when the liver should be doing one thing
00:26:12.860 | and when the stomach should be doing another.
00:26:15.580 | I know you've done some work on the clock.
00:26:18.180 | So if you would just maybe briefly describe
00:26:21.580 | where the clock is, what it does,
00:26:24.500 | and some of the top contour
00:26:26.860 | of how it tells the cells of the body what to do.
00:26:29.780 | Right.
00:26:30.600 | So the first thing to say is that, as you said,
00:26:32.860 | the clock is all over the place.
00:26:34.660 | Most of the tissues in your body have clocks.
00:26:37.980 | We probably have, what, millions of clocks in our body.
00:26:40.260 | I would say that's probably fair.
00:26:42.020 | If you have millions of cell types,
00:26:43.340 | you probably may have millions of clocks.
00:26:46.100 | The role of the central pacemaker for the circadian system
00:26:50.260 | is to coordinate all of these.
00:26:52.760 | And there's a little nucleus,
00:26:55.900 | a little collection of nerve cells in your brain.
00:26:59.540 | It's called the suprachiasmatic nucleus, the SCN.
00:27:03.620 | And it is sitting in a funny place
00:27:05.760 | for the rest of the structures in the nervous system
00:27:07.660 | that get direct retinal input.
00:27:09.600 | It's sitting in the hypothalamus,
00:27:11.980 | which you can think about as sort of the great coordinator
00:27:15.500 | of drives and-
00:27:18.460 | The source of all our pleasures and all our problems.
00:27:20.740 | Right. Or most our problems.
00:27:22.100 | Yes, it really is.
00:27:23.980 | But it's sort of deep in your brain,
00:27:26.220 | things that drive you to do things.
00:27:28.020 | If you're freezing cold, you put on a coat, you shivery.
00:27:31.460 | All these things are coordinated by the hypothalamus.
00:27:34.140 | So this pathway that we're talking about from the retina
00:27:37.580 | and from these peculiar cells
00:27:39.660 | that are encoding light intensity
00:27:41.820 | are sending signals directly into a center
00:27:45.420 | that's surrounded by all of these centers
00:27:47.940 | that control autonomic nervous system
00:27:51.060 | and your hormonal systems.
00:27:54.160 | So this is a part of your visual system
00:27:57.220 | that doesn't really reach the level of consciousness.
00:27:59.500 | It's not something you think about.
00:28:01.900 | It's happening under the radar kind of all the time.
00:28:05.900 | And the signal is working its way
00:28:07.540 | into this central clock coordinating center.
00:28:12.380 | Now, what happens then is not that well understood,
00:28:17.400 | but it's clear that this is a neural center
00:28:20.020 | that has the same ability to communicate
00:28:21.740 | with other parts of your brain as any other neural center.
00:28:25.580 | And clearly there are circuits
00:28:28.780 | that involve connections between neurons
00:28:31.220 | that are conventional.
00:28:34.060 | But in addition, it's quite clear
00:28:35.340 | that there are also sort of humoral effects
00:28:37.480 | that things are oozing out of the cells in the center
00:28:42.020 | and maybe into the circulation
00:28:43.580 | or just diffusing through the brain to some extent
00:28:47.620 | that can also affect neurons all swear.
00:28:50.660 | But the hypothalamus uses everything
00:28:52.760 | to control the rest of the bodies.
00:28:54.240 | And that's true of the suprachiasmatic nucleus,
00:28:56.640 | this circadian center as well.
00:28:59.020 | It can get its fingers into the autonomic nervous system,
00:29:03.400 | the humoral system,
00:29:04.740 | and of course up to the centers of the brain
00:29:07.160 | that organize coordinated rational behavior.
00:29:12.160 | So if I understand correctly,
00:29:14.300 | we have this group of cells, the suprachiasmatic nucleus,
00:29:16.620 | it's got a 24 hour rhythm.
00:29:18.520 | That rhythm is more or less matched
00:29:21.620 | to what's going on in our external world
00:29:23.840 | by the specialized set of neurons in our eye.
00:29:26.420 | But then the master clock itself, the SCN,
00:29:30.460 | releases things in the blood, humoral signals
00:29:33.020 | that go out various places in the body.
00:29:37.820 | And then you said to the autonomic system,
00:29:39.360 | which is regulating more or less how alert or calm we are,
00:29:42.100 | as well as our thinking and our cognition.
00:29:44.240 | So I'd love to talk to you about the autonomic part.
00:29:48.380 | Presumably that's through melatonin,
00:29:52.220 | it's through adrenaline.
00:29:55.660 | How is it that this clock is impacting
00:29:58.380 | how the autonomic system, how alert or calm we feel?
00:30:02.060 | - Right, so there are pathways
00:30:05.140 | by which the suprachiasmatic nucleus can access
00:30:09.300 | both the parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous system.
00:30:12.260 | - Just so people know, the sympathetic nervous system
00:30:14.400 | is the one that tends to make us more alert,
00:30:16.180 | and the parasympathetic nervous system
00:30:18.100 | is the portion of the autonomic nervous system
00:30:20.880 | makes us feel more calm, in broad context.
00:30:24.120 | - To first approximation, right.
00:30:25.780 | So this is, both of these systems are within the grasp
00:30:30.780 | of the circadian system through hypothalamic circuits.
00:30:35.020 | One of the circuits that will be, I think,
00:30:37.700 | of particular interest to some of your listeners
00:30:40.040 | is a pathway that involves this sympathetic branch
00:30:44.100 | of the autonomic nervous system, the fight or flight system,
00:30:47.740 | that is actually, through a very circuitous route,
00:30:50.900 | innervating the pineal gland,
00:30:52.780 | which is sitting in the middle of your brain.
00:30:55.380 | - The so-called third eye.
00:30:56.980 | - Right, so this is the-
00:30:58.380 | - We'll have to get back to why it's called the third eye,
00:31:00.600 | because, yeah. - That's an interesting history.
00:31:02.260 | - You can't call something the third eye and not,
00:31:04.060 | and just, you know.
00:31:05.240 | - Just leave it there. - Just leave it there.
00:31:06.780 | - Right. - Right.
00:31:08.440 | - Anyway, this is the major source of melatonin in your body.
00:31:11.500 | - So light comes into my eye. - Yes.
00:31:15.200 | - Passed off to the suprachiasmatic nucleus, essentially,
00:31:18.020 | not the light itself,
00:31:18.880 | but the signal representing the light.
00:31:21.220 | - Sure.
00:31:22.060 | - Then the SCN, the suprachiasmatic nucleus,
00:31:24.300 | can impact the melatonin system via the pineal.
00:31:28.880 | - Right, the way this is seen is that
00:31:30.600 | if you were to measure your melatonin level
00:31:33.880 | over the course of the day,
00:31:35.600 | if you could do this, you know, hour by hour,
00:31:38.160 | you'd see that it's really low during the day,
00:31:40.480 | very high at night.
00:31:42.160 | But if you get up in the middle of the night
00:31:44.100 | and go to the bathroom and turn on
00:31:45.300 | the bright fluorescent light,
00:31:47.120 | your melatonin level is slammed to the floor.
00:31:49.480 | Light is directly impacting your hormonal levels
00:31:53.360 | through this mechanism that we just described.
00:31:57.180 | So this is one of the routes by which light can act
00:32:00.560 | on your hormonal status through pathways
00:32:04.440 | that are completely beyond
00:32:06.440 | what you normally would think about, right?
00:32:08.440 | You're thinking about the things in the bathroom.
00:32:10.540 | Oh, there's the toothbrush.
00:32:12.080 | You know, there's the tube of toothpaste.
00:32:13.820 | But meanwhile, this other system is just counting photons
00:32:17.680 | and saying, oh, wow, there's a lot of photons right now.
00:32:20.120 | Let's shut down the melatonin release.
00:32:22.640 | - This is one of the main reasons why
00:32:24.400 | I've encouraged people to avoid bright light exposure
00:32:27.280 | in the middle of the night.
00:32:28.320 | Not just blue light, but bright light of any wavelength.
00:32:32.120 | Because there's this myth out there that blue light,
00:32:34.800 | because it's the optimal signal for activating this pathway
00:32:37.720 | and shutting down melatonin,
00:32:39.520 | is the only wavelength of light that can shut it down.
00:32:43.420 | But am I correct in thinking that
00:32:45.360 | if a light is bright enough,
00:32:47.200 | it doesn't matter if it's blue light, green light,
00:32:49.320 | purple light, even red light,
00:32:51.720 | you're going to slam melatonin down to the ground,
00:32:54.720 | which is not a good thing to happen
00:32:57.000 | in the middle of the night, correct?
00:32:58.400 | - Right, yeah.
00:32:59.320 | I mean, any light will affect the system to some extent.
00:33:03.480 | The blue light is somewhat more effective,
00:33:06.440 | but don't fool yourself into thinking
00:33:09.000 | that if you use red light,
00:33:09.920 | that means you're avoiding the effect.
00:33:12.640 | It's certainly still there.
00:33:14.460 | And certainly if it's very bright,
00:33:15.660 | it'll be more effective in driving the system
00:33:18.440 | than dim blue light would be.
00:33:20.060 | - Interesting.
00:33:20.900 | A lot of people wear blue blockers.
00:33:22.520 | And in a kind of odd twist of misinformation out there,
00:33:28.480 | a lot of people wear blue blockers
00:33:29.720 | during the middle of the day,
00:33:31.200 | which basically makes no sense
00:33:32.920 | because during the middle of the day
00:33:34.060 | is when you want to get a lot of bright light
00:33:36.880 | and including blue light into your eyes, correct?
00:33:39.420 | - Absolutely.
00:33:40.260 | Not just for the reasons we've been talking about
00:33:42.700 | in terms of circadian effects.
00:33:44.540 | There are major effects of light on mood
00:33:47.060 | and seasonal affective disorder
00:33:51.060 | apparently is essentially a reflection
00:33:53.660 | of this same system in reverse.
00:33:55.820 | If you're living in the Northern climes
00:33:58.700 | and you're not getting that much light
00:34:00.620 | during the middle of the winter in Stockholm,
00:34:03.600 | you might be prone to depression
00:34:06.580 | and phototherapy might be just the ticket for you.
00:34:09.420 | And that's because there's a direct effect of light on mood.
00:34:13.620 | There's an example where if you don't have enough light,
00:34:16.460 | it's a problem.
00:34:17.660 | So I think you're exactly right.
00:34:18.700 | It's not about is light good or bad for you?
00:34:20.380 | It's about what kind of light and when
00:34:22.780 | that makes the difference.
00:34:25.440 | - Yeah, the general rule of thumb that I've been living by
00:34:27.260 | is to get as much bright light in my eyes,
00:34:29.100 | ideally from sunlight,
00:34:31.220 | anytime I want to be alert
00:34:32.900 | and doing exactly the opposite when I want to be asleep
00:34:36.260 | or getting drowsy.
00:34:37.100 | - And there are aspects of this that spin out
00:34:40.820 | way beyond the conversation we're having now
00:34:42.620 | to things like this.
00:34:44.320 | It turns out that the incidence of myopia-
00:34:47.540 | - Nearsightedness.
00:34:48.640 | - Nearsightedness, right,
00:34:50.780 | is strongly related to the amount of time
00:34:53.560 | that kids spend outdoors.
00:34:56.140 | - In what direction of effect?
00:34:57.740 | - The more they spend time outdoors,
00:34:59.800 | the less nearsightedness they have.
00:35:02.120 | So this is all about-
00:35:02.960 | - And is that because they're viewing things at a distance
00:35:04.460 | or because they're getting a lot of blue light,
00:35:06.540 | less sunlight?
00:35:07.360 | - It's a great question.
00:35:08.300 | It is not fully resolved what the epidemiological,
00:35:11.620 | what the basis of that epidemiological finding is.
00:35:14.380 | One possibility is the amount of light,
00:35:16.180 | which would make me think about this
00:35:17.660 | melanopsin system again,
00:35:19.960 | but it might very well be a question of accommodation
00:35:22.700 | that is the process by which you focus on
00:35:24.380 | near or far things.
00:35:25.960 | If you're never outdoors, everything is nearby.
00:35:28.360 | If you're outdoors, you're focusing far.
00:35:30.340 | - Unless you're on your phone.
00:35:31.580 | - Right, exactly.
00:35:32.900 | - There's a tremendous amount of interest these days
00:35:36.060 | in watches and things that count steps.
00:35:39.580 | I'm beginning to realize that we should probably
00:35:41.740 | have a device that can count photons during the day
00:35:45.920 | and can also count photons at night
00:35:47.620 | and tell us, hey, you're getting too many photons.
00:35:49.420 | You're going to shut down your melatonin at night,
00:35:51.040 | or you're not getting enough photons.
00:35:52.360 | Today, you didn't get enough bright light,
00:35:53.900 | whether or not it's from artificial light or from sunlight.
00:35:56.740 | I guess the, where would you put it?
00:35:58.020 | I guess you put it on the top of your head,
00:35:59.180 | or you'd probably want it someplace outward facing.
00:36:02.620 | - Right, probably what you need is as many photons
00:36:05.580 | over as much of the retina as possible
00:36:07.260 | to recruit as much of this system as possible.
00:36:11.380 | - In thinking about other effects
00:36:12.620 | of this non-image forming pathway
00:36:15.180 | that involves these special cells in the eye and the SCN,
00:36:19.800 | you had a paper a few years ago
00:36:21.580 | looking at retinal input to an area of the brain,
00:36:26.580 | which has a fancy name, the perihabenula,
00:36:30.780 | but names don't necessarily matter,
00:36:33.020 | that had some important effects on mood
00:36:36.380 | and other aspects of light.
00:36:39.440 | Maybe you could tell us a little bit about
00:36:40.740 | what is the perihabenula?
00:36:42.780 | - Oh, wow, so that's a fancy term,
00:36:45.100 | but I think the way to think about this
00:36:47.040 | is as a chunk of the brain that is sitting
00:36:51.260 | as part of a bigger chunk that's really the linker
00:36:55.420 | between peripheral sensory input of all kinds,
00:36:58.220 | virtually all kinds, whether it's auditory input
00:37:02.780 | or tactile input or visual input
00:37:07.060 | to the region of your brain, the cortex,
00:37:09.220 | that allows you to think about these things
00:37:11.080 | and make plans around them and to integrate them
00:37:13.740 | and that kind of thing.
00:37:15.000 | So we've known about a pathway that gets from the retina
00:37:21.900 | through this sort of linker center,
00:37:27.900 | it's called the thalamus, and then on up to the cortex.
00:37:31.460 | - Exactly, but you want to arrive at the destination, right?
00:37:34.460 | Now you're at grand central and now you can do your thing
00:37:36.740 | 'cause you're up at the cortex.
00:37:38.340 | So this is the standard pattern.
00:37:40.100 | You have the sensory input coming from the periphery,
00:37:41.860 | you've got these peripheral elements
00:37:43.380 | that are doing the initial stages of-
00:37:45.980 | - The eye, the ear, the nose.
00:37:47.520 | - The eye, the skin of your fingertips, right?
00:37:49.820 | No, the taste buds on your tongue,
00:37:51.520 | they're taking this raw information in
00:37:53.480 | and they're doing some pre-processing maybe
00:37:56.860 | or the early circuits are,
00:37:58.900 | but eventually most of these signals
00:38:00.420 | have to pass through the gateway to the cortex,
00:38:02.660 | which is the thalamus.
00:38:04.660 | And we've known for years, for decades, many decades,
00:38:09.160 | what the major throughput pathway
00:38:11.100 | from the retina to the cortex is and where it ends up.
00:38:14.500 | It ends up in the visual cortex.
00:38:15.820 | You pat the back of your head,
00:38:17.300 | that's where the receiving center is
00:38:20.820 | for the main pathway from retina to cortex.
00:38:23.840 | But wait a minute, there's more.
00:38:25.700 | There's this little side pathway
00:38:27.540 | that goes through a different part of that linking
00:38:30.340 | thalamus center, the gateway to the cortex.
00:38:32.660 | - It's like a local train from Grand Central to-
00:38:34.900 | - It's in a weird part of the neighborhood, right?
00:38:37.580 | It's a completely different,
00:38:38.700 | it's like a little trunk line that branches off
00:38:41.580 | and goes out into the hinterlands
00:38:43.620 | and it's going to the part of this linker center
00:38:46.460 | that's talking to a completely different part of cortex,
00:38:48.820 | way up front, frontal lobe,
00:38:51.300 | which is much more involved in things like planning
00:38:54.140 | or self-image or-
00:38:57.660 | - Self-image, literally how one thinks about oneself.
00:39:01.020 | - Do you feel good about yourself?
00:39:02.920 | Or what's your plan for next Thursday?
00:39:06.480 | It's a very high level center
00:39:11.020 | in the highest level of your nervous system.
00:39:13.580 | And this is the region that is getting input
00:39:16.060 | from this pathway, which is mostly worked out
00:39:18.980 | in this function by Samer Hatara's lab.
00:39:21.340 | I know you had him on the podcast.
00:39:23.220 | - We didn't talk about this pathway.
00:39:24.220 | - This pathway at all, right.
00:39:25.660 | So, Diego Fernandez and Samer and the folks
00:39:30.660 | that work with them were able to show
00:39:33.100 | that this pathway doesn't just exist
00:39:35.020 | and get you to a weird place.
00:39:37.140 | But if you activate it at kind of the wrong time of day,
00:39:41.940 | animals can become depressed.
00:39:45.360 | And if you silence it under the right circumstances,
00:39:48.740 | then weird lighting cycles that would normally
00:39:52.000 | make them act sort of depressed,
00:39:55.560 | no longer have that effect.
00:39:57.520 | - So, it sounds to me like there's this pathway
00:39:59.240 | from I to this unusual train route
00:40:03.740 | through the structure we call the thalamus,
00:40:06.740 | then up to the front of the brain that relates
00:40:08.780 | to things of self-perception, kind of higher level functions.
00:40:13.360 | I find that really interesting because most
00:40:14.980 | of what I think about when I think about these fancy,
00:40:17.740 | well, or these primitive, rather, neurons
00:40:20.860 | that don't pay attention to the shapes of things,
00:40:22.840 | but instead to brightness, I think of,
00:40:24.800 | well, it regulates melatonin, circadian clock, mood, hunger,
00:40:28.780 | the really kind of vegetative stuff, if you will.
00:40:32.540 | And this is interesting because I think a lot
00:40:35.480 | of people experience depression, not just people
00:40:38.480 | that live in Scandinavia in the middle of winter.
00:40:42.120 | And we are very much divorced
00:40:45.080 | from our normal interactions with light.
00:40:47.800 | It also makes me realize
00:40:49.780 | that these intrinsically photosensitive cells
00:40:52.220 | that set the clock, et cetera, are involved
00:40:55.400 | in a lot of things.
00:40:56.620 | I mean, they seem to regulate a dozen
00:40:59.760 | or more different basic functions.
00:41:01.500 | I want to ask you about a different aspect
00:41:05.200 | of the visual system now, which is the one
00:41:08.300 | that relates to our sense of balance.
00:41:10.880 | So, I love boats, but I hate being on them.
00:41:13.700 | I love the ocean from shore because I get incredibly seasick.
00:41:18.280 | I'll just, it's awful.
00:41:19.540 | I think I'm gonna get seasick if I think about it too much.
00:41:22.060 | And once I went on a boat trip, I came back
00:41:23.920 | and I actually got, I got motion sick
00:41:27.380 | or wasn't seasick 'cause I went rafting.
00:41:30.260 | So, there's a system that somehow gets messed up.
00:41:33.520 | They always tell us if you're feeling sick
00:41:34.720 | to look at the horizon, et cetera, et cetera.
00:41:37.320 | So, what is the link between our visual system
00:41:39.480 | and our balance system?
00:41:40.700 | And why does it make us nauseous sometimes
00:41:43.320 | when the world is moving in a way
00:41:45.640 | that we're not accustomed to?
00:41:47.360 | I realize this is a big question
00:41:48.880 | because it involves eye movement, et cetera.
00:41:51.600 | But let's maybe just walk in at the simplest layers
00:41:56.320 | of vision, vestibular, so-called balance system,
00:42:01.320 | and then maybe we can piece the system together for people
00:42:04.320 | so that they can understand.
00:42:05.160 | And then also we should give them some tools
00:42:06.880 | for adjusting their nausea
00:42:08.640 | when their vestibular system is out of whack.
00:42:12.000 | - Cool, so, I mean, the first thing to think about
00:42:14.460 | is that the vestibular system is designed to allow you
00:42:20.460 | to see how your, or detect, sense how you're moving
00:42:25.460 | in the world, through the world.
00:42:29.020 | It's a funny one because it's about your movement
00:42:34.360 | in relationship to the world in a sense,
00:42:36.300 | and yet it's sort of interoceptive in the sense
00:42:39.460 | that it is really in the end sensing the movement
00:42:44.460 | of your own body.
00:42:46.180 | - Okay, so, interoception we should probably delineate
00:42:48.120 | for people is when you're focusing on your internal state
00:42:50.660 | as opposed to something outside you.
00:42:52.140 | - Right.
00:42:52.980 | - But it's a gravity sensing system.
00:42:55.280 | - Well, it's partly a gravity sensing system
00:42:57.540 | in the sense that gravity is a force that's acting on you
00:43:02.540 | as if you were moving through the world
00:43:05.380 | in the opposite direction.
00:43:06.860 | - All right, now you gotta explain that one to me.
00:43:10.440 | - Okay, so basically the idea is that
00:43:12.920 | if we leave gravity aside,
00:43:16.840 | we're just sitting in a car in the passenger seat
00:43:21.820 | and the driver hits the accelerator
00:43:24.500 | and you start moving forward, you sense that.
00:43:26.740 | If your eyes were closed, you'd sense it.
00:43:28.420 | If your ears were plugged and your eyes were closed,
00:43:30.580 | you'd still know it.
00:43:31.740 | - Yeah, many people take off on the plane like this,
00:43:33.900 | they're dreading the flight
00:43:35.100 | and they know when the plane is taking off.
00:43:36.900 | - Sure, that's your vestibular system talking
00:43:38.920 | because anything that jostles you
00:43:40.900 | out of the current position you're in right now
00:43:43.100 | will be detected by the vestibular system, pretty much.
00:43:46.640 | So this is a complicated system,
00:43:49.600 | but it's basically in your inner ear,
00:43:52.840 | very close to where you're hearing.
00:43:54.240 | - Right, they put it there.
00:43:55.580 | And I don't know who they is.
00:43:57.880 | - I don't really know, they're starting to ride.
00:44:00.280 | - I'm just kidding.
00:44:01.400 | To steal our friend Russ Van Gelder's explanation,
00:44:03.960 | we weren't consulted the design phase and no one-
00:44:06.560 | - That's a great line, that's a great line.
00:44:08.760 | - But it's interesting, it's in the ear.
00:44:11.880 | - Yeah, yeah, it's deep in there
00:44:14.660 | and it's served by the same nerve actually
00:44:18.520 | that serves the hearing system.
00:44:20.620 | One way to think about it is both the hearing system
00:44:23.420 | and the vestibular self-motion sensing system
00:44:26.880 | are really detecting the signal in the same way.
00:44:29.320 | They're hairy cells and they're excited.
00:44:32.400 | - Really hairy?
00:44:33.240 | - Yeah, sort of, they got little cilia sticking up
00:44:35.320 | off the surfaces.
00:44:36.820 | And depending on which way you bend those,
00:44:38.720 | the cells will either be inhibited or excited.
00:44:41.100 | They're not even neurons, but then they talk to neurons
00:44:44.080 | with a neuron-like process and off you go.
00:44:46.140 | Now you've got an auditory signal
00:44:47.560 | if you're sensing things bouncing around in your cochlea,
00:44:51.480 | which is-
00:44:52.320 | - Sound waves.
00:44:53.160 | - Sympathetically the bouncing of your eardrum,
00:44:55.760 | which is sympathetically the sound waves in the world.
00:44:58.800 | But in the case of the vestibular apparatus,
00:45:01.280 | evolution has built a system that detects the motion
00:45:04.500 | of say fluid going by those hairs.
00:45:07.880 | And if you put a sensor like that in a tube
00:45:11.300 | that's fluid-filled, now you've got a sensor
00:45:14.040 | that will be activated when you rotate that tube
00:45:17.940 | around the axis that passes through the middle of it.
00:45:20.020 | Those who were just listening won't be able to visualize.
00:45:22.420 | - No, I think that makes sense.
00:45:23.380 | I was thinking of it as three hula hoops.
00:45:25.420 | - Right, three hula hoops.
00:45:26.260 | - One standing up, one lying down on the ground.
00:45:28.300 | - Right, one in the other direction, three directions.
00:45:31.680 | The people who fly will talk about roll, pitch, and yaw,
00:45:35.080 | that kind of thing.
00:45:35.920 | So the three axes of encoding,
00:45:39.260 | just like in the cones of the retina.
00:45:40.080 | - Sort of the yes, the no, and then I always say it's,
00:45:43.220 | and then the puppy head tilt.
00:45:44.380 | - Yeah, the puppy head tilt, that's the other one.
00:45:46.700 | So the point is that your brain is eventually going to be
00:45:50.320 | able to unpack what these sensors are telling you
00:45:54.620 | about how you just rotated your head
00:45:57.100 | in very much the way that the three types of cones
00:45:59.460 | we were talking about before are reading the incoming photons
00:46:04.860 | in the wavelength domain differently.
00:46:06.840 | And if you can compare and trust, you get red, green, blue.
00:46:09.940 | So it's the same basic idea.
00:46:11.680 | If you have three sensors and you array them properly,
00:46:14.600 | now you can tell if you're rotating your head
00:46:16.680 | left or right, up or down.
00:46:18.460 | That's the sensory signal coming back into your brain,
00:46:22.440 | confirming that you've just made a movement that you will.
00:46:26.560 | - But what about on the plane?
00:46:27.700 | Because when I'm on the plane, I'm completely stationary.
00:46:29.720 | The plane's moving, but my head hasn't moved.
00:46:32.440 | So I'm just moving forward, gravity is constant.
00:46:35.420 | - Exactly.
00:46:36.260 | - How do I know I'm accelerating?
00:46:37.880 | - So what's happening now is your brain
00:46:39.640 | is sensing the motion and the brain is smart enough also
00:46:44.640 | to ask itself, did I will that movement
00:46:48.580 | or did that come from the outside?
00:46:50.560 | So now in terms of sort of understanding
00:46:52.520 | what the distributor signal means,
00:46:54.680 | it's gotta be embedded in the context
00:46:56.300 | of what you tried to do or what your other sensory systems
00:47:01.160 | are telling you about what's happening right now.
00:47:02.680 | - I see, so it's very interesting, but it's not conscious.
00:47:06.600 | Or at least if it's conscious, it's not conscious,
00:47:09.400 | it's definitely very fast, right?
00:47:11.520 | The moment that plane starts moving,
00:47:12.840 | I know that I didn't get up out of my chair
00:47:14.480 | and run forward.
00:47:15.320 | - Right.
00:47:16.160 | - But I'm not really thinking about
00:47:17.280 | getting up out of my chair.
00:47:18.580 | I just know.
00:47:19.420 | - I guess the way I think about it
00:47:20.440 | is that the nervous system is "aware" at many levels.
00:47:25.440 | When it gets all the way up to the cortex
00:47:29.000 | and we're thinking about it,
00:47:30.420 | you're talking about it, that's cortical.
00:47:34.140 | But the lower levels of the brain
00:47:37.380 | that don't require you to actually actively think about it,
00:47:40.400 | they're just doing their thing,
00:47:42.320 | are also made aware, right?
00:47:44.040 | A lot of this is happening under the surface
00:47:45.880 | of what you're thinking.
00:47:47.080 | These are reflexes.
00:47:48.760 | - Okay, so we've got this gravity sensing system.
00:47:52.440 | - Right.
00:47:53.280 | - I'm nodding for those that are listening
00:47:57.100 | for a yes movement of the head,
00:47:59.180 | a no movement of the head
00:48:00.360 | or the tilting of the head from side to side.
00:48:02.720 | - Right.
00:48:03.560 | - And then you said that knowledge about
00:48:04.840 | whether or not activation of that system
00:48:07.400 | comes from my own movements
00:48:08.960 | or something acting upon me, like the plane moving,
00:48:12.520 | has to be combined with other signals.
00:48:15.640 | And so how is the visual information
00:48:19.600 | or information about the visual world
00:48:21.040 | combined with balance information?
00:48:22.880 | - Right.
00:48:23.720 | So, yeah, I mean, I guess maybe the best way
00:48:26.560 | to think about how these two systems work together
00:48:29.940 | is to think about what happens
00:48:31.060 | when you suddenly rotate your head to the left.
00:48:34.020 | When you suddenly rotate your head to the left,
00:48:36.620 | your eyes are actually rotating to the right automatically.
00:48:40.820 | You do this in complete darkness.
00:48:43.260 | If you had an infrared camera and watched yourself
00:48:48.100 | in complete darkness, you can't see anything.
00:48:49.780 | Rotating your head to the left,
00:48:50.780 | your eyes would rotate to the right.
00:48:52.460 | That's your vestibular system saying,
00:48:56.340 | I'm gonna try to compensate for the head rotation
00:49:00.960 | so my eyes are still looking in the same place.
00:49:04.160 | Why is that useful?
00:49:05.240 | Well, if it's always doing that,
00:49:08.400 | then the image of the world on your retina
00:49:10.240 | will be pretty stable most of the time.
00:49:12.800 | And that actually helps vision.
00:49:14.800 | - Have they built this into cameras for image stabilization?
00:49:18.280 | 'Cause when I move, when I take a picture with my phone,
00:49:20.400 | it's blurry, it's not clear.
00:49:22.460 | - Well, actually, you might wanna get a better phone
00:49:25.600 | because now what they have is software in the better apps
00:49:29.680 | that will do a kind of image stabilization post hoc
00:49:32.640 | by doing a registration of the images
00:49:34.480 | that are bouncing around.
00:49:35.760 | They say the edge of the house was here,
00:49:38.240 | so let's get that aligned at each of your images.
00:49:40.580 | So you may not be aware if you're using a good new phone
00:49:44.760 | that if you walk around a landscape and hold your phone,
00:49:49.760 | that there's all this image stabilization going on.
00:49:52.980 | But it's built into standard cinematic technology now
00:49:57.980 | because if you tried to do a handheld camera,
00:50:00.200 | things would be bouncing around, things would be unwatchable.
00:50:02.800 | You wouldn't be able to really understand
00:50:04.160 | what's going on in the scene.
00:50:05.560 | So the brain works really hard
00:50:08.720 | to mostly stabilize the image of the world on your retina.
00:50:12.200 | Of course, you're moving through the world,
00:50:13.500 | so you can't stabilize everything.
00:50:14.880 | But the more you can stabilize most of the time,
00:50:17.740 | the better you can see.
00:50:19.060 | And that's why when we're scanning a scene,
00:50:22.840 | looking around at things,
00:50:24.600 | we're making very rapid eye movements
00:50:26.840 | for very short periods of time, and then we just rest.
00:50:30.440 | But we're not the only ones that do that.
00:50:31.940 | If you ever watch a hummingbird,
00:50:33.120 | it does exactly the same thing at a feeder, right?
00:50:35.540 | But it's with its body.
00:50:37.040 | It's gonna make a quick movement,
00:50:38.680 | and then it's gonna be stable.
00:50:41.460 | And when you watch a pigeon walking on the sidewalk,
00:50:44.520 | it does this funny head bobbing thing.
00:50:46.160 | But what it's really doing is racking its head back
00:50:48.820 | on its neck while its body goes forward
00:50:51.260 | so that the image of the visual world stays static.
00:50:54.380 | Is that why they're doing it?
00:50:56.820 | And you've seen the funny chicken videos on YouTube, right?
00:50:59.540 | You take a chicken, move it up and down,
00:51:01.060 | the head stays in one place.
00:51:02.140 | It's all the same thing.
00:51:03.700 | All of these animals are trying hard
00:51:06.200 | to keep the image of the world stable on their retina
00:51:08.820 | as much of the time as they possibly can.
00:51:10.880 | And then when they've got to move, make it fast,
00:51:13.340 | make it quick, and then stabilize again.
00:51:15.460 | That's why the pigeons have their head back?
00:51:16.900 | It is, yeah.
00:51:19.340 | Yeah.
00:51:20.180 | I just need to pause there for a second and digest that.
00:51:23.200 | Amazing.
00:51:24.040 | In case people aren't, well,
00:51:28.440 | there's no reason why people would know
00:51:30.060 | what we're doing here,
00:51:30.900 | but essentially what we're doing is we're building up
00:51:32.740 | from sensory, you know, light onto the eye, color,
00:51:36.780 | to what the brain does with that,
00:51:39.060 | the integration of that, you know,
00:51:40.520 | circadian clock, melatonin, et cetera.
00:51:42.100 | And now what we're doing is we're talking about
00:51:43.460 | multi-sensory or multimodal,
00:51:45.800 | combining one sense vision with another sense balance.
00:51:50.560 | And it turns out that pigeons know more about this
00:51:53.600 | than I do because pigeons know to keep their head back
00:51:56.240 | as they walk forward.
00:51:57.960 | All right, so that gets us to this issue of motion sickness.
00:52:02.520 | And you don't have to go out on a boat.
00:52:05.680 | Anytime I go to New York,
00:52:06.900 | I sit in an Uber or in a cab in the back.
00:52:09.440 | And if I'm looking at my phone while the car is driving,
00:52:13.220 | I feel nauseous by time I arrive at my destination.
00:52:17.020 | I always try and look out the front of the windshield
00:52:19.380 | because I'm told that helps,
00:52:20.620 | but it's a little tiny window.
00:52:22.800 | And I end up feeling slightly less sick if I do that.
00:52:26.960 | So what's going on with the vision and the balance system
00:52:31.960 | that causes a kind of a nausea?
00:52:34.300 | And actually, if I keep talking about this,
00:52:35.820 | I probably will get sick.
00:52:37.220 | I don't throw up easily,
00:52:38.300 | but for some reason, motion sickness is a real thing for me.
00:52:42.620 | It's a problem for a lot of people.
00:52:44.340 | I mean, I think the fundamental problem typically
00:52:47.120 | when you get motion sick
00:52:48.240 | is what they call visual vestibular conflict.
00:52:52.760 | That is, you have two sensory systems
00:52:55.360 | that are talking to your brain
00:52:56.480 | about how you're moving through the world.
00:52:58.520 | And as long as they agree, you're fine.
00:53:02.440 | So if you're driving,
00:53:04.320 | your body senses that you're moving forward.
00:53:07.160 | Your vestibular systems
00:53:08.800 | is picking up this acceleration of the car.
00:53:12.480 | And your visual system is seeing the consequences
00:53:14.900 | of forward motion in the sweeping of the scene past you.
00:53:19.360 | Everything is honky-dory, right?
00:53:20.860 | No problem.
00:53:22.140 | But when you are headed forward,
00:53:25.580 | but you're looking at your cell phone,
00:53:26.880 | what is your retina seeing?
00:53:28.000 | Your retina is seeing the stable image of the screen.
00:53:30.280 | There's absolutely no motion in that screen.
00:53:33.560 | Or the motion is, or some other motion,
00:53:35.740 | like a movie or, yeah. Or it's a motion
00:53:36.580 | you're watching if you're playing a game,
00:53:37.940 | or you're watching a video, a football game.
00:53:40.400 | The motion is uncoupled with what's actually happening
00:53:43.360 | to your body.
00:53:44.300 | Your brain doesn't like that.
00:53:45.560 | Your brain likes everything to be aligned.
00:53:48.280 | And if it's not, it's going to complain to you.
00:53:50.580 | By making me feel nauseous.
00:53:51.420 | By making you feel nauseous,
00:53:52.420 | and maybe you'll change your behavior.
00:53:53.880 | So you're getting-
00:53:54.760 | I'm getting punished.
00:53:55.840 | Yeah, for setting it up
00:53:57.980 | so your signal's not going to flick, right.
00:53:59.740 | By the vestibular-
00:54:01.120 | You'll learn.
00:54:01.940 | Visuals.
00:54:02.780 | In time. I love it.
00:54:06.400 | I love the idea of reward signals.
00:54:08.560 | And we've done a lot of discussion
00:54:10.280 | about this on this podcast of things like dopamine reward
00:54:12.840 | and things, but also punishment signals.
00:54:14.560 | And I love this example.
00:54:17.120 | Well, maybe marching a little bit further
00:54:20.440 | along this pathway, visual input is combined
00:54:24.240 | with balance input.
00:54:25.900 | Where does that occur?
00:54:28.320 | And maybe, 'cause I have some hint of where it occurs,
00:54:32.160 | you could tell us a little bit about this
00:54:34.200 | kind of mysterious little mini brain
00:54:37.560 | that they call the cerebellum.
00:54:38.600 | Cerebellum, yeah.
00:54:40.100 | So, you know, the way I tried to describe the cerebellum
00:54:43.760 | to my students is that it serves sort of like
00:54:48.760 | the air traffic control system functions in air travel.
00:54:53.320 | So that it's a system that's very complicated
00:54:57.560 | and it's really dependent on great information.
00:55:01.060 | So it's taking in the information
00:55:02.620 | about everything that's happening everywhere,
00:55:04.620 | not only through your sensory systems,
00:55:07.640 | but it's listening in to all the little centers elsewhere
00:55:10.520 | in your brain that are computing
00:55:11.680 | what you're gonna be doing next and so forth.
00:55:13.580 | So it's just ravenous for that kind of information.
00:55:15.780 | So it really is like a little mini brain.
00:55:17.700 | It is, it's got access to all those signals.
00:55:21.820 | And it really has an important role
00:55:25.460 | in coordinating and shaping movements.
00:55:29.880 | But, you know, if you suddenly eliminated
00:55:33.680 | the air traffic control system,
00:55:36.460 | planes could still take off and land,
00:55:38.940 | but you might have some unhappy accidents in the process.
00:55:43.940 | So the cerebellum is kind of like that.
00:55:45.860 | It's not that you would be paralyzed
00:55:48.160 | if your cerebellum was gone
00:55:49.900 | because you still have motor neurons.
00:55:51.360 | You still have ways to talk to your muscles.
00:55:54.100 | You still have reflex centers.
00:55:56.420 | And it's not like you would have any sensory loss
00:55:59.800 | because you still have your cortex
00:56:01.480 | getting all of those beautiful signals
00:56:03.020 | that you can think about.
00:56:04.980 | But you wouldn't be coordinating things so well anymore.
00:56:09.380 | The timing between input and output might be off.
00:56:12.860 | Or if you were trying to practice a new athletic move,
00:56:16.660 | like an overhead serve in tennis,
00:56:19.260 | you'd be just terrible at learning.
00:56:21.380 | But all of the sequences of muscle movements
00:56:25.220 | and the feedback from your sensory apparatus
00:56:27.320 | that would let you really hit that ball
00:56:29.360 | exactly where you wanted to after the nth rep, right?
00:56:32.700 | You know, the thousandth rep or something,
00:56:34.940 | you get much better at it.
00:56:36.100 | So the cerebellum's all involved in things like
00:56:38.500 | motor learning and refining the precisions of movement
00:56:44.380 | so that they get you where you want to go.
00:56:47.220 | If you reach for a glass of champagne
00:56:49.940 | that you don't knock it over or stop short.
00:56:52.200 | You know, that's what it's good at.
00:56:54.580 | People who have selective damage to the cerebellum.
00:56:57.320 | Absolutely.
00:56:58.160 | And what I come familiar with, well, Korsakoff's
00:57:03.160 | is different, right?
00:57:04.440 | Isn't that a B vitamin deficiency in chronic alcoholics?
00:57:07.560 | Right.
00:57:08.400 | And they have a, they tend to walk kind of bow-legged
00:57:10.120 | and they can't coordinate their movements.
00:57:11.860 | Is that, that has some, that-
00:57:13.480 | Not sure about the cerebellar-
00:57:15.300 | But also cerebellum.
00:57:16.440 | I'm not sure about the cerebellar involvement there,
00:57:19.060 | but you know, the typical thing would be
00:57:22.000 | a patient who has a cerebellar stroke or a tumor,
00:57:26.680 | for example, might be not that steady on their feet.
00:57:31.680 | You know, if the, you know, dynamics of the situation
00:57:37.340 | is standing on a street car with no pole to hold onto,
00:57:40.840 | they might not be as good at adjusting
00:57:43.000 | all of the little movements of the car.
00:57:45.360 | You know, there's a kind of tremor that can occur
00:57:49.340 | as they're reaching for things
00:57:50.940 | because they reach a little too far
00:57:53.560 | and then they over-correct and come back.
00:57:56.260 | Things like that.
00:57:57.500 | So it's very common neurological phenomenon, actually.
00:58:02.500 | Cerebellar ataxia is what the neurologists call it.
00:58:07.260 | And it can happen not just with cerebellar damage,
00:58:09.140 | but damage to the tracts that feed the information
00:58:11.420 | into the cerebellum.
00:58:12.260 | Right, just deprive the structure.
00:58:13.340 | Exactly, or output from the cerebellum.
00:58:15.500 | And so the cerebellum is where a lot of visual
00:58:17.860 | and balance information is combined.
00:58:19.900 | In a very key place in the cerebellum,
00:58:21.960 | which is, it's really one of the oldest parts
00:58:26.320 | in terms of evolution.
00:58:27.160 | Talking about the flocculus.
00:58:27.980 | The flocculus, right.
00:58:28.820 | This is a, it's a critical place in the cerebellum
00:58:32.200 | where visual and vestibular information comes together
00:58:35.160 | for according just the kinds of movements
00:58:37.300 | we were talking about.
00:58:38.320 | This image stabilizing network, it's all happening there.
00:58:41.920 | And there's learning happening there as well.
00:58:43.660 | So that if your vestibular apparatus
00:58:45.960 | is a little bit damaged somehow,
00:58:48.880 | your visual system is actually talking to your cerebellum
00:58:52.820 | saying there's a problem here, there's an error.
00:58:55.860 | And your cerebellum is learning to do better
00:58:58.840 | by increasing the output of the vestibular system
00:59:01.260 | to compensate for whatever that loss was.
00:59:03.680 | So it's a little error correction system.
00:59:05.340 | That's sort of typical of a cerebellar function.
00:59:08.400 | And it can happen in many, many different domains.
00:59:10.180 | This is just one of the domains
00:59:11.900 | of sensory motor integration that takes place there.
00:59:15.300 | So I should stay off my phone in the Ubers.
00:59:18.700 | If I'm on a boat, I should essentially look
00:59:22.300 | and as much as possible act as if I'm driving the machine.
00:59:25.760 | - Right.
00:59:26.600 | - That'd be weird if I was in the passenger seat
00:59:28.180 | pretending I was driving the machine,
00:59:29.480 | but I do always feel better
00:59:30.660 | if I'm sitting in the front seat passenger.
00:59:32.700 | - Right, the more of the visual world that you can see
00:59:35.780 | as if you were actually the one doing the motion,
00:59:38.380 | I would think.
00:59:39.420 | - Let's stay in the inner ear for a minute
00:59:41.020 | as we continue to march around the nervous system.
00:59:44.400 | When you take off in the plane or when you land
00:59:48.500 | or sometimes in the middle of the air,
00:59:50.060 | your ears get clogged, or at least my ears get clogged.
00:59:53.260 | That's because of pressure buildup
00:59:56.300 | in the various tubes of the inner ear, et cetera.
00:59:59.340 | We'll get into this.
01:00:00.220 | But years ago, our good friend, Harvey Carton,
01:00:05.100 | who's another world-class neuroanatomist,
01:00:08.220 | gave a lecture and it talked about
01:00:12.140 | how plugging your nose and blowing out
01:00:14.420 | versus plugging your nose and sucking in
01:00:17.420 | can, should be done at different times
01:00:20.100 | depending on whether or not you're taking off or landing.
01:00:23.600 | And I always see people try to un-pop their ears.
01:00:26.920 | And when you do scuba diving,
01:00:28.340 | you learn how to do this without necessarily,
01:00:30.300 | I can do it by just kind of moving my jaw now
01:00:32.860 | 'cause I've done a little bit of diving.
01:00:35.480 | But what's the story there?
01:00:37.700 | We don't have to get into all the differences
01:00:39.740 | in atmospheric pressure, et cetera.
01:00:41.140 | But if I'm taking off and my ears are plugged,
01:00:44.260 | or I've recently ascended, plane took off,
01:00:46.460 | my ears are plugged, do I plug my nose and blow out
01:00:48.540 | or do I plug my nose and suck in?
01:00:49.940 | - Right, so the basic idea is that if your ears feel bad
01:00:54.940 | because you're going into an area of higher pressure,
01:00:58.620 | so if they pressurize the cabin more than the pressure
01:01:01.960 | that you have on the surface of the planet,
01:01:03.760 | your eardrums will be bending in and they don't like that.
01:01:07.020 | If you push them more, they'll hurt even more.
01:01:08.640 | - That's a good description that the pressure goes up,
01:01:11.620 | then they're gonna bend in.
01:01:12.580 | - Bend in, and then the reverse would be true
01:01:14.660 | if you go into an area of low pressure.
01:01:16.620 | So if you started to drive up the mountainside,
01:01:20.140 | the pressure is getting lower and lower outside.
01:01:22.140 | Now the air behind your eardrum is blooming out, right?
01:01:26.860 | So it's just a question of are you trying to get
01:01:28.680 | more pressure or less pressure behind the eardrum?
01:01:31.460 | And there's a little tube that does that
01:01:32.780 | and comes down into the back of your throat there.
01:01:35.540 | And if you force pressure up that tube,
01:01:37.580 | you're gonna be putting more air pressure
01:01:39.500 | into the compartment.
01:01:41.100 | - To counter it.
01:01:42.260 | - If it's not enough, and if you're sucking,
01:01:45.500 | you're going the other way.
01:01:46.460 | In reality, I think as long as you open the passageway,
01:01:49.080 | I think the pressure differential
01:01:50.780 | is gonna solve your problem.
01:01:52.220 | So I think you could actually blow in
01:01:54.220 | when you're not, quote, supposed to.
01:01:55.960 | - Okay, so you could just hold your nose and blow air out
01:02:00.180 | or hold your nose and suck in the effect.
01:02:04.100 | Either way is fine.
01:02:05.020 | - I think so.
01:02:05.860 | - Excellent, I just won $100 from Harvey Carton.
01:02:09.100 | Thank you very much.
01:02:10.500 | Harvey and I used to teach in our anatomy together,
01:02:12.820 | and I'll say, I don't think it matters,
01:02:14.180 | but thank you, I'll split that with you.
01:02:16.820 | This is important stuff, but it's true.
01:02:21.940 | You hear this, so it doesn't matter either way.
01:02:25.420 | - I'm no expert in this area.
01:02:27.300 | Don't quote me.
01:02:28.140 | - He's not gonna, well, I'm going to quote you.
01:02:30.260 | Okay, so we've talked about the inner ear
01:02:32.140 | and we've talked about the cerebellum.
01:02:33.960 | I want to talk about an area of the brain
01:02:35.740 | that is rarely discussed, which is the midbrain.
01:02:40.160 | And for those that don't know,
01:02:42.820 | the midbrain is an area beneath the cortex.
01:02:45.640 | I guess we never really defined cortex.
01:02:47.180 | It was just kind of the outer layers
01:02:48.500 | or are the outer layers of the,
01:02:51.800 | at least mammalian brain or human brain.
01:02:54.500 | But the midbrain is super interesting
01:02:57.140 | because it controls a lot of unconscious stuff,
01:03:02.140 | reflexes, et cetera.
01:03:03.860 | And then there's this phenomenon even called blindsight.
01:03:07.900 | So could you please tell us about the midbrain,
01:03:11.320 | about what it does and what in the world is blindsight?
01:03:15.660 | - Yeah, so this is a, there's a lot of pieces there.
01:03:19.520 | I think the first thing to say is
01:03:20.920 | if you imagine the nervous system in your mind's eye,
01:03:25.280 | you see this big honking brain
01:03:27.080 | and then there's this little wand that dangles down
01:03:32.080 | into your vertebral column, the spinal cord,
01:03:34.540 | and that's kind of your visual impression.
01:03:37.760 | What you have to imagine is starting in the spinal cord
01:03:40.080 | and working your way up into this big, magnificent brain.
01:03:42.700 | And what you would do as you enter the skull
01:03:46.400 | is get into a little place
01:03:47.920 | where the spinal cord kind of thickens out.
01:03:50.160 | It still has that sort of long, skinny, trunk-like feeling.
01:03:54.800 | - Sort of like a paddle or a spoon shape.
01:03:56.660 | - Right, it starts to spread out a little bit,
01:03:58.520 | and that's 'cause your evolution has packed
01:04:00.480 | more interesting goodies in there
01:04:02.040 | for processing information and generating movement.
01:04:05.300 | So beyond that is this tween brain
01:04:09.740 | we were talking about, this linker brain.
01:04:12.180 | Diencephalon really means the between brain.
01:04:14.820 | - Oh, I thought you said tween.
01:04:16.020 | - Well, it is, yes.
01:04:16.860 | - No, no, between, between, I'm sorry, I thought you said tween.
01:04:19.740 | - Yeah, it's the between,
01:04:20.820 | it's the between brain is what the name means.
01:04:24.280 | It's the linker from the spinal cord in the periphery
01:04:27.740 | up to these grand centers of the cortex.
01:04:30.380 | But this midbrain you're talking about
01:04:33.300 | is the last bit of this enlarged sort of spinal cordy thing
01:04:37.240 | in your skull, which is really the brainstem
01:04:39.500 | is what we call it.
01:04:40.940 | The last bit of that before you get to this relay
01:04:43.860 | up to the cortex is the midbrain.
01:04:46.940 | And there's a really important visual center there.
01:04:49.560 | It's called the superior colliculus.
01:04:51.480 | There's a similar center in the brains
01:04:54.540 | of other vertebrate animals, a frog, for example,
01:04:57.440 | or a lizard would have this.
01:04:59.020 | It's called the optic tectum there.
01:05:01.380 | But it's a center that in these non-mammalian vertebrates
01:05:06.380 | is really the main visual center.
01:05:10.420 | They don't really have what we would call a visual cortex,
01:05:13.900 | although there's something sort of like that.
01:05:16.040 | But this is where most of the action is
01:05:17.540 | in terms of interpreting visual input
01:05:19.560 | and organizing behavior around that.
01:05:22.480 | You can sort of think about this region of the brainstem
01:05:27.420 | as a reflex center that can reorient the animal's gaze
01:05:32.420 | or body, or maybe even attention to particular regions
01:05:37.740 | of space out there around the animal.
01:05:41.580 | And that could be for all kinds of reasons.
01:05:43.540 | I mean, it might be a predator just showed up
01:05:45.780 | in one corner of the forest and you pick that up
01:05:48.520 | and you're trying to avoid it.
01:05:50.000 | Or just any movement.
01:05:51.020 | Many movement, right?
01:05:52.420 | It might be that suddenly something splats on the page
01:05:57.300 | when you're reading a novel
01:05:58.540 | and your eye reflexly looks at it.
01:06:02.020 | You don't have to think about that.
01:06:03.180 | That's a reflex.
01:06:04.140 | - What if you throw me a ball, but I'm not expecting it,
01:06:07.260 | and I just reach up and try and grab it, catch it or not?
01:06:10.700 | Is that handled by the midbrain?
01:06:12.260 | - Well, that's probably not the midbrain,
01:06:14.920 | although, I mean, by itself,
01:06:17.080 | because it's going to involve all these limb movements,
01:06:19.660 | this movement of your arm and body.
01:06:21.480 | - What about ducking if something's suddenly
01:06:24.780 | thrown at my head?
01:06:25.620 | - Yeah, sure, right.
01:06:26.460 | Brains like that will certainly have a brainstem component,
01:06:29.020 | a midbrain component.
01:06:30.460 | You know, something looms and you duck.
01:06:32.420 | It may not be the superior colliculus we're talking about.
01:06:36.840 | Now, it might be another part of the visual midbrain,
01:06:39.140 | but these are centers that emerged early
01:06:41.700 | in the evolution of brains like ours
01:06:43.900 | to handle complicated visual events
01:06:46.740 | that have significance for the animal.
01:06:48.900 | In terms of space, where is it in space?
01:06:51.620 | And in fact, this same center actually gets input
01:06:54.340 | from all kinds of other sensory systems
01:06:56.200 | that take information from the external world,
01:06:58.900 | from particular locations,
01:07:00.580 | and where you might want to either avoid or approach things
01:07:03.540 | according to their significance to you.
01:07:05.660 | So you get input from the touch system.
01:07:08.700 | You get input from the auditory system.
01:07:11.540 | I worked for a while in rattlesnakes.
01:07:13.500 | They get input from a part of their warm sensors
01:07:17.740 | on their face.
01:07:18.580 | They're in these little pits on the face.
01:07:20.580 | - They used to work on baby rattlesnakes, right?
01:07:22.220 | - Well, they were adults, actually.
01:07:23.980 | - Oh, I wasn't trying to diminish the danger.
01:07:25.660 | I thought for some reason they were little ones.
01:07:27.660 | Why in the world would you work on rattlesnakes?
01:07:30.280 | - Well, because they have a version
01:07:33.780 | of an extra receptive sensory system.
01:07:35.940 | That is, they're looking out into the world
01:07:39.020 | using a completely different set of sensors.
01:07:41.100 | They're using the same sensors
01:07:42.300 | that would feel the warmth on your face
01:07:44.000 | if you stood in front of a bonfire,
01:07:45.880 | except evolution has given them
01:07:48.420 | this very nice specialized system
01:07:49.960 | that lets them image where the heat's coming from.
01:07:52.220 | You can sort of do that anyway, right?
01:07:53.900 | If you walk around the fire,
01:07:56.220 | you can feel where the fire is
01:07:57.900 | from the heat hitting your face.
01:08:00.460 | - Is that the primary way in which they detect prey?
01:08:04.300 | - It's one of the major ways.
01:08:06.660 | And in fact, they use vision as well.
01:08:09.100 | And they bring these two systems together
01:08:11.100 | in the same place, in this tectum region,
01:08:13.620 | this brain stem, midbrain region.
01:08:15.460 | - What's all the tongue jutting about when the snakes?
01:08:17.580 | - That I don't know.
01:08:19.080 | That may be olfactory.
01:08:20.380 | There may be-
01:08:21.220 | - They're sniffing the air with their tongue.
01:08:22.220 | - Yeah, there may be a-
01:08:23.900 | - Earlier in our drive,
01:08:24.780 | you told me that flies actually
01:08:26.300 | taste things with their feet.
01:08:27.700 | - They do, yeah.
01:08:28.540 | - That's so weird.
01:08:29.380 | - Yeah, they have taste receptors
01:08:31.060 | in lots of funny places.
01:08:32.540 | - I want to pause here just for one second
01:08:34.920 | before we get back into the midbrain.
01:08:36.500 | I think what's so interesting in all seriousness
01:08:40.120 | about taste receptors on feet, heat sensors,
01:08:45.040 | tongues jutting out of snakes,
01:08:47.660 | and vision and all this integration
01:08:49.360 | is that it really speaks to the fact
01:08:51.980 | that all these sensory neurons
01:08:55.020 | are trying to gather information
01:08:57.820 | and stuff it into a system
01:08:59.720 | that can make meaningful decisions and actions.
01:09:03.260 | And that it really doesn't matter
01:09:04.620 | whether or not it's coming from eyes or ears or nose
01:09:06.680 | or bottoms of feet,
01:09:08.520 | because in the end, it's just electricity flowing in.
01:09:11.460 | And so it sounds like it's placed on each animal.
01:09:14.860 | It always feels weird to call fly an animal,
01:09:17.300 | but they are creatures, they are animals.
01:09:21.380 | It's placed in different locations on different animals,
01:09:23.740 | depending on the particular needs of that animal.
01:09:26.620 | - Right, but how much more powerful
01:09:29.140 | if the nervous systems can also cross-correlate
01:09:32.820 | across sensory systems?
01:09:34.920 | So if you've got a weak signal from one sensory system,
01:09:38.100 | you're not quite sure there's something there,
01:09:40.320 | and a weak signal from another sensory system
01:09:42.980 | that's telling you the same locations
01:09:44.780 | is a little bit interesting.
01:09:46.320 | There might be something there.
01:09:47.740 | If you've got those two together, you've got corroboration.
01:09:50.500 | Your brain now says it's much more likely
01:09:53.100 | that that's gonna be something worth paying attention to.
01:09:56.860 | - Right, so maybe I'm feeling some heat
01:09:59.540 | on one side of my face,
01:10:01.200 | and I also smell something baking in the oven.
01:10:05.820 | So now neither is particularly strong,
01:10:08.180 | but as you said, there's some corroboration,
01:10:10.380 | and that corroboration is occurring in the midbrain.
01:10:12.460 | - Right, and then if you throw things into conflict,
01:10:15.960 | now the brain is confused,
01:10:17.300 | and that may be where your emotion sickness comes from.
01:10:20.000 | So it's great to have, as a brain,
01:10:22.980 | it's great to have as many sources of information
01:10:24.740 | as you can have, just like if you're a spy or a journalist.
01:10:29.400 | You don't want as much information
01:10:30.580 | as you can get about what's out there,
01:10:33.040 | but if things conflict, that's problematic, right?
01:10:35.640 | Your sources are giving you different information
01:10:37.300 | about what's going on.
01:10:38.480 | Now you've got a problem on your hands.
01:10:39.820 | What do you publish?
01:10:40.820 | - The midbrain is so fascinating.
01:10:43.520 | I don't wanna eject us from the midbrain
01:10:46.300 | and go back to the vestibular system,
01:10:47.820 | but I do have a question that I forgot to ask
01:10:49.640 | about the vestibular system,
01:10:50.740 | which is why is it that for many people, including me,
01:10:53.780 | there's, despite my motion sickness in cabs,
01:10:56.420 | that there's a sense of pleasure in moving through space
01:11:00.820 | and getting tilted relative to the gravitational pull
01:11:03.380 | of the earth?
01:11:04.220 | For me growing up, it was skateboarding,
01:11:05.680 | but people like to corner in cars, corner on bikes.
01:11:08.800 | It may be, for some people, it's done running or dance,
01:11:13.340 | but what is it about moving through space
01:11:16.300 | and getting tilted, a lot of surfers around here,
01:11:19.680 | getting tilted that can tap
01:11:23.220 | into some of the pleasure centers?
01:11:25.320 | Do we have any idea why that would feel good?
01:11:26.940 | - I have no clue.
01:11:28.620 | - Is there dopaminergic input to this system?
01:11:31.460 | - Well, the dopaminergic system gets a lot of places.
01:11:36.220 | It's pretty much, to some extent,
01:11:40.300 | everywhere in the cortex,
01:11:41.340 | a lot more in the frontal lobe, of course,
01:11:43.500 | but that's just for starters.
01:11:45.880 | I mean, there's basically dopaminergic innervation
01:11:47.880 | most places in the central nervous system,
01:11:50.280 | so there's the potential for dopaminergic involvement,
01:11:52.700 | but I really have no clue about the tilting phenomenon.
01:11:55.200 | I mean-
01:11:56.040 | - People pay money to go on roller coasters.
01:11:58.000 | - Right.
01:11:58.840 | Well, I think that may be as much about the thrill
01:12:00.640 | as anything else. - Sure.
01:12:01.480 | And falling is, the falling reflex is very robust
01:12:04.360 | in all of us.
01:12:05.200 | When the visual world's going up very fast,
01:12:07.160 | it usually means that we're falling.
01:12:08.420 | - Right, right.
01:12:09.260 | - But in some people like that, some people don't.
01:12:10.740 | - Right, and kids tolerate a lot more
01:12:14.640 | sort of vestibular craziness spinning around
01:12:16.720 | until they've dropped.
01:12:18.460 | - Well, I've friends, it always, you know,
01:12:21.420 | worries me a little bit that they throw their kids.
01:12:23.600 | I'm not recommending anyone do this.
01:12:25.000 | When they're little kids, you know,
01:12:25.860 | like throwing the kids really far back and forth.
01:12:29.000 | Some kids seem to love it.
01:12:30.420 | - Yeah.
01:12:31.260 | Yeah, our son loved being shaken up and down
01:12:33.920 | very, very vigorously.
01:12:37.200 | That's the only thing that would calm him down sometimes.
01:12:39.280 | - Interesting.
01:12:40.280 | Yeah, so I'm guessing.
01:12:41.360 | We can guess that maybe there's some activation
01:12:46.360 | of the reward systems from moving through space.
01:12:50.200 | - Well, I mean, if you think about, you know,
01:12:51.860 | how rewarding it is to be able to move through space
01:12:54.820 | and how unhappy people are who are used to that,
01:12:57.920 | who suddenly aren't able to do that,
01:12:59.680 | there is a sense of agency, right?
01:13:01.740 | If you can choose to move through the world and to tilt,
01:13:05.240 | that's not only you're moving through the world,
01:13:06.800 | but you're doing it with a certain amount of finesse.
01:13:08.440 | Maybe that's what it is.
01:13:09.360 | You can feel like you're the master of your own movement
01:13:13.240 | in a way that you wouldn't if you were going straight.
01:13:15.120 | I'm just blowing smoke here, right?
01:13:17.160 | - Yeah, well, we can speculate.
01:13:18.420 | That's fine.
01:13:19.260 | I couldn't help but ask the question.
01:13:21.400 | Okay, so if we move ourselves, pun intended,
01:13:25.000 | back into the midbrain.
01:13:27.220 | The midbrain's combining all these different signals
01:13:29.080 | for reflexive action.
01:13:31.120 | At what point does this become deliberate action?
01:13:34.880 | Because if I look at something I want
01:13:36.740 | and I want to pursue it,
01:13:37.580 | I'm going to go toward it, and many times,
01:13:41.060 | that's a deliberate decision.
01:13:42.320 | - Right, so this gets very slippery, I think,
01:13:46.660 | because what you have to try to imagine
01:13:49.260 | is all these different parts of the brain
01:13:51.000 | working on the problem of staying alive
01:13:53.940 | and surviving in the world.
01:13:58.980 | They're working on the problem simultaneously,
01:14:01.080 | and there's not one right answer to how to do that.
01:14:06.260 | But one way to think about it is that
01:14:09.880 | you have high levels of your nervous system
01:14:11.760 | that are very well designed to override
01:14:16.100 | an otherwise automatic movement if it's inappropriate.
01:14:19.580 | So if you've imagined you've been invited to tea
01:14:22.320 | with the queen, and she hands you
01:14:25.120 | a very fancy wedge wood tea cup, very thin.
01:14:29.760 | - Wedge wood tea cup?
01:14:30.840 | - Yes, with very hot tea in it,
01:14:33.040 | and you're burning your hand,
01:14:34.100 | you probably will try to find a way to put that back down
01:14:36.960 | on the saucer rather than just dropping it on the floor
01:14:40.140 | because you're with the queen.
01:14:42.380 | You're trying to be appropriate to that.
01:14:45.520 | So you have ways of reining in automatic behaviors
01:14:49.380 | if they're going to be maladaptive,
01:14:51.160 | but you also want the reflex to work quickly
01:14:54.580 | if it's the only thing that's going to save you,
01:14:56.060 | the looming object coming at your head.
01:14:58.340 | You don't have time to think about that.
01:15:00.300 | So this is the interplay in these hierarchically
01:15:04.300 | organized centers of the nervous system.
01:15:06.100 | At the lowest level, you've got the automatic sensors
01:15:09.500 | and centers and reflex arcs that will keep you safe
01:15:14.500 | even if you don't have time to think about it.
01:15:16.740 | And then you've got the higher center saying,
01:15:18.740 | well, maybe we could do this as well,
01:15:20.640 | or maybe we shouldn't do that at all, right?
01:15:23.500 | So you have all of these different levels
01:15:25.700 | operating simultaneously,
01:15:27.540 | and you need bi-directional communication
01:15:30.620 | between high level cognitive centers,
01:15:34.620 | decision-making on the one hand,
01:15:37.740 | and these low level, very helpful reflexive centers,
01:15:40.300 | but they're a little bit rigid, a little hardwired,
01:15:43.460 | so they need some nuance.
01:15:44.540 | So both of these things are operating in tandem
01:15:47.380 | in real time, all the time in our brains.
01:15:49.860 | And sometimes we listen more to one than the other.
01:15:51.780 | You've heard people in sports talking about
01:15:55.060 | messing up at the plate 'cause they overthought it,
01:15:57.540 | thinking too hard about it.
01:15:59.020 | That's partly, you've already trained your cerebellum
01:16:01.900 | how to hit a fastball right down the middle.
01:16:04.980 | Right, and if you start looking for something new
01:16:08.100 | or different, you're gonna mess up your reflexive swing.
01:16:11.140 | Right, if you're trying to think about the physics
01:16:12.960 | of the ball as it's coming at you,
01:16:15.020 | you've already missed, right?
01:16:16.380 | 'Cause you're not using your,
01:16:18.780 | all those reps have built a kind of knowledge
01:16:22.260 | is what you wanna rely on when you don't have enough time
01:16:25.380 | to contemplate.
01:16:27.100 | - This is important and a great segue
01:16:30.300 | for what I'd like to discuss next,
01:16:32.960 | which is the basal ganglia,
01:16:35.900 | this really interesting area of the brain
01:16:38.220 | that's involved in GO-type commands and behaviors,
01:16:43.220 | instructing us to do things, and NO-GO,
01:16:46.460 | preventing us from doing things.
01:16:47.760 | Because so much of motor learning and skill execution
01:16:50.720 | and not saying the wrong thing,
01:16:53.520 | or sitting still in class when,
01:16:55.640 | or as you used with the "Tea with the Queen" example,
01:16:59.480 | feeling discomfort involves suppressing behavior,
01:17:03.900 | and sometimes it's activating behavior.
01:17:05.820 | A tremendous amount of online attention is devoted
01:17:07.880 | to trying to get people motivated.
01:17:11.360 | This isn't the main focus of our podcast.
01:17:13.320 | We touch on some of the underlying neural circuits
01:17:15.080 | of motivation, dopamine, and so forth.
01:17:17.400 | But so much of what people struggle with out there
01:17:20.840 | are elements around failure to pay attention,
01:17:25.840 | or challenges in paying attention,
01:17:28.300 | which is essentially like putting the blinders on,
01:17:30.660 | getting a soda straw view of the world
01:17:32.660 | and maintaining that for a bout of work
01:17:34.700 | or something of that sort,
01:17:35.940 | and trying to get into action.
01:17:38.900 | So of course, this is carried out by many neural circuits,
01:17:42.420 | not just the basal ganglia,
01:17:43.580 | but what are the basal ganglia
01:17:45.840 | and what are their primary roles
01:17:47.900 | in controlling go-type behavior and no-go-type behavior?
01:17:51.920 | - Yeah, so I mean, the basal ganglia are sitting deep
01:17:55.440 | in what you would call the forebrain,
01:17:57.920 | so the highest levels of the brain.
01:18:00.520 | They are sort of cousins to the cerebral cortex,
01:18:05.480 | which we talked about as sort of the highest level
01:18:07.440 | of your brain, the thing you're thinking with.
01:18:09.920 | - Cerebral cortex being the refined cousins,
01:18:11.880 | and then you've got the- - Right.
01:18:13.480 | - The brutes. - Yeah.
01:18:15.320 | - I mean, that's probably totally unfair, but the point-
01:18:18.600 | - I like the basal ganglia.
01:18:19.560 | I can relate to the brutish parts of the brain.
01:18:22.700 | Little bit of hypothalamus, little bit of basal ganglia, sure.
01:18:25.600 | - We need it all, we need it all.
01:18:27.820 | And this area of the brain has gotten a lot bigger
01:18:32.820 | as the cortex has gotten bigger,
01:18:35.280 | and it's deeply intertwined with cortical function.
01:18:39.320 | The cortex can't really do what it needs to do
01:18:41.520 | without the help of the basal ganglia and vice versa,
01:18:43.680 | so they're really intertwined.
01:18:46.560 | And in a way, you can think about this logically
01:18:51.600 | as saying if you have the ability to withhold behavior
01:18:55.440 | or to execute it, how do you decide which to do?
01:18:58.880 | Well, the cortex is gonna have to do that thinking for you.
01:19:01.720 | You have to be looking at all the contingencies
01:19:04.340 | of your situation to decide, is this a crazy move
01:19:07.680 | or is this a really smart investment right now or what?
01:19:11.480 | - I don't wanna go out for a run in the morning,
01:19:13.120 | I'm gonna make myself go out for a run,
01:19:15.280 | or I'm having a great time out on a run
01:19:17.840 | and I know I need to get back,
01:19:19.040 | but I kind of wanna go another mile.
01:19:20.720 | - I mean, another great example is that the marshmallow test
01:19:23.640 | for the little kids, they can get two marshmallows
01:19:25.860 | if they hold off just 30 seconds initially.
01:19:30.860 | They can have one right away,
01:19:32.560 | but if they can wait 30 seconds, they got two.
01:19:34.280 | So that's the no-go because their cortex is saying,
01:19:37.400 | I really like to have two more than having one,
01:19:41.000 | but they're not gonna get the two unless they can
01:19:43.920 | not reach for the one.
01:19:45.680 | So they've got to hold off the action
01:19:48.780 | and that has to result from a cognitive process.
01:19:53.360 | So the cortex is involved in this in a major way.
01:19:56.660 | - Yeah, as I recall in that experiment,
01:19:59.000 | the kids used a variety of tools to,
01:20:01.000 | some would distract themselves.
01:20:03.000 | I particularly related to the kid that would just
01:20:04.880 | put himself right next to the marshmallows
01:20:07.320 | and then some of the kids covered their eyes,
01:20:09.960 | some of them would count or sing.
01:20:11.840 | Yeah, so that's all very cortical, right?
01:20:13.280 | Coming up with a novel strategy,
01:20:15.080 | simple example that we're using here.
01:20:16.560 | But of course, this is at play anytime someone decides
01:20:19.520 | they wanna go watch a motivational speech or something,
01:20:21.880 | just, you know, a Steve Jobs commencement speech
01:20:24.260 | just to get motivated to engage in their day.
01:20:26.560 | - Should I take this new job?
01:20:28.360 | You know, it's got great benefits,
01:20:30.000 | but it's in a lousy part of the country.
01:20:32.080 | - Why do you think that some people have a harder time
01:20:36.080 | running these go no-go circuits
01:20:38.120 | and other people seem to have very low activation energy,
01:20:42.180 | we would say.
01:20:43.020 | They can just, you know, they have a task,
01:20:44.720 | they just lean into the task.
01:20:46.040 | Whereas some people getting into task completion
01:20:49.040 | or things of that sort is very challenging for them.
01:20:52.440 | - Yeah, I mean, I think it's really just another,
01:20:54.920 | it's a special case of a very general phenomenon,
01:20:57.280 | which is brains are complicated
01:21:00.280 | and brains we have are the result of genetics and experience
01:21:07.000 | and my genes are different from your genes
01:21:08.820 | and my experiences are different from your experiences.
01:21:11.000 | So the things that will be easy or hard for us
01:21:13.600 | won't necessarily be aligned.
01:21:15.040 | They might just happen to be just because they are.
01:21:18.640 | But the point is that, you know,
01:21:20.560 | you're dealt a certain set of cards,
01:21:23.940 | you have a certain set of genes, you are handed a brain.
01:21:27.640 | You don't choose your brain, it's handed to you.
01:21:30.280 | But then there's all this stuff you can do with it.
01:21:32.300 | You know, you can learn.
01:21:36.320 | You know, to have new skills or to act differently
01:21:39.880 | or to show more restraint,
01:21:41.240 | which is kind of relevant to what we're talking about here,
01:21:43.820 | or maybe show less restraint if your problem is
01:21:47.200 | you're so buttoned down, you never have any fun in life
01:21:49.200 | and you should loosen up a little bit.
01:21:50.840 | - Thank you, I appreciate the insult.
01:21:52.680 | Yeah.
01:21:53.520 | David's always encouraged me to have a little more fun.
01:21:58.120 | So basal ganglia are, they're kind of the disciplinarian
01:22:04.080 | or they're sort of the instructor conductor of sorts, right?
01:22:07.960 | Go, no go, you know, you be quiet, you start now.
01:22:11.440 | - I wish I knew more about the basal ganglia than I do.
01:22:14.920 | My sense is that it, you know, this system is key
01:22:19.320 | for implementing the plans that get cooked up in the cortex,
01:22:23.500 | but they also influence the plans
01:22:27.640 | that the cortex is dishing out
01:22:30.540 | because this is a major source of information
01:22:33.700 | to the cortex.
01:22:34.540 | So it becomes almost impossible to figure out
01:22:36.900 | where the computation begins and where it ends
01:22:40.640 | and who's doing what,
01:22:41.780 | because these things are all interacting
01:22:44.200 | in a complex network.
01:22:45.780 | And it's all of it, it's the whole network.
01:22:47.600 | It's not, you know, one is the leader
01:22:49.380 | and the other is the follower.
01:22:50.500 | - Right, of course.
01:22:51.340 | Yeah, these are, all the structures that we're discussing
01:22:53.320 | are working in parallel.
01:22:55.400 | - Right.
01:22:56.240 | - And there's a lot of changing crosstalk.
01:22:59.140 | I have this somewhat sick habit, David,
01:23:03.260 | every day I try and do 21 no-goes.
01:23:06.200 | So if I want to reach for my phone,
01:23:08.160 | I try and not do it just to see if I can prevent myself
01:23:12.080 | from engaging in that behavior, if it was reflexive.
01:23:15.520 | If it's something I want to do, deliberate choice,
01:23:18.520 | then I certainly allow myself to do it.
01:23:20.560 | I don't tend to have too much trouble with motivation,
01:23:22.880 | with go type functions, mostly because I'm so busy
01:23:25.880 | that I wish I had more time for more goes,
01:23:29.160 | but so to speak.
01:23:31.100 | But do you think these circuits have genuine plasticity
01:23:34.880 | in them?
01:23:35.720 | - Absolutely.
01:23:36.540 | I mean, everybody knows how they've learned over time
01:23:39.580 | to wait for the two marshmallows, right?
01:23:41.600 | You know, you don't have to have instant gratification
01:23:44.000 | all the time.
01:23:44.840 | You know, you're willing to do a job sometimes
01:23:48.380 | that isn't your favorite job
01:23:49.480 | because it comes with the territory
01:23:50.840 | and you want the salary that comes at the end of the week
01:23:52.800 | or the end of the month, right?
01:23:54.200 | So we can defer gratification.
01:23:57.240 | You know, we can choose not to say the thing
01:23:59.900 | that we know is going to inflame our partner
01:24:01.880 | and create a meltdown for the next week.
01:24:04.960 | You know, we learn this control,
01:24:07.920 | but I think these are skills like any other.
01:24:10.120 | You can get better at them if you practice them.
01:24:13.080 | So I think you're choosing to do that spontaneously
01:24:15.720 | as kind of a, you know, it's a mental practice.
01:24:18.020 | It's a discipline.
01:24:18.860 | It's a way of building a skill that you want to have.
01:24:21.760 | - Yeah, I find it to be something that when I engage
01:24:25.360 | in a no-go type situation,
01:24:29.860 | then the next time and the next time
01:24:32.040 | that I find myself about to move reflexively,
01:24:34.640 | there's a little gap in consciousness
01:24:36.320 | that I can make a decision
01:24:38.240 | whether or not this is really the best use of my time.
01:24:40.820 | Because I sometimes wonder
01:24:42.760 | whether or not all this business around attention,
01:24:45.440 | certainly there's the case of ADHD
01:24:46.960 | and clinical diagnosed ADHD,
01:24:48.680 | but all these, the issue around focus and attention
01:24:51.200 | is really that people just have not really learned
01:24:53.440 | how to short circuit a reflex.
01:24:56.260 | And so much of what makes us different than rattlesnakes
01:24:59.140 | or, well, actually they could be deliberate,
01:25:01.120 | but from the other animals
01:25:03.240 | and is our ability to suppress reflex.
01:25:06.400 | - Yeah, well, that's the cortex.
01:25:08.280 | I mean, or let's say the forebrain.
01:25:10.040 | Cortex and basal ganglia working together,
01:25:12.580 | sitting on top of this lizard brain
01:25:15.600 | that's giving you all these great adaptive reflexes
01:25:17.920 | that help you survive.
01:25:19.440 | You just hope you don't get the surprising case
01:25:22.400 | where the thing that your reflex is telling you
01:25:24.640 | is actually exactly the wrong thing
01:25:26.160 | and you make a mistake, right?
01:25:28.280 | - Right, so that's what the cortex is for.
01:25:29.900 | It's adding nuance and context and experience,
01:25:33.780 | past association, and in human beings,
01:25:36.740 | obviously learning from others through communication.
01:25:41.360 | - Well, I was, you went right to it
01:25:43.660 | and it was where I was gonna go.
01:25:45.700 | So let's talk about the cortex.
01:25:47.100 | We've worked our way up the so-called neuroaxis
01:25:49.420 | as the aficionados will know.
01:25:52.640 | So we're in the cortex.
01:25:54.120 | This is the seat of our higher consciousness,
01:25:55.860 | self-image, planning and action.
01:25:58.220 | But as you mentioned, the cortex isn't just about that.
01:26:00.500 | It's got other regions that are involved in other things.
01:26:02.960 | So maybe we should, staying with vision,
01:26:05.620 | let's talk a little bit about visual cortex.
01:26:07.880 | You told me a story, an amazing story about visual cortex.
01:26:11.800 | And it was a somewhat of a sad story, unfortunately,
01:26:13.840 | about someone who had a stroke to visual cortex.
01:26:17.020 | Maybe if you would share that story,
01:26:20.280 | because I think it illustrates many important principles
01:26:23.240 | about what the cortex does.
01:26:25.080 | - Right, so the visual cortex is,
01:26:29.680 | you could say the projection screen,
01:26:31.600 | the first place where this information streaming
01:26:35.720 | from the retina through this thalamus connecting linker
01:26:40.720 | gets played out for the highest level of your brain to see.
01:26:46.780 | I mean, it's a representation.
01:26:48.800 | It's a map of things going on in the visual world
01:26:52.560 | that's in your brain, and when we describe a scene
01:26:57.560 | to a friend, we're using this chunk of our brain
01:27:03.320 | to be able to put words, which are coming
01:27:04.960 | from a different part of our cortex,
01:27:07.260 | to the objects and movements and colors
01:27:10.220 | that we see in the world.
01:27:11.620 | So that's a key part of your visual experience.
01:27:17.740 | When you can describe the things you're seeing,
01:27:20.280 | you're looking at your visual cortex, and this is-
01:27:23.560 | - Could I just ask a quick question?
01:27:24.640 | So right now, because I'm looking at your face,
01:27:27.780 | as we're talking, there are neurons in my brain,
01:27:31.060 | more or less in the configuration of your face
01:27:34.720 | that are active as you move about.
01:27:37.840 | And what if I were to close my eyes and just imagine,
01:27:42.840 | I do this all the time, by the way, David,
01:27:44.960 | I close my eyes and I imagine David Berson's face.
01:27:48.920 | I don't tend to do that as often, maybe I should,
01:27:51.080 | but you get the point.
01:27:52.360 | I'm now using visualization of what you look like
01:27:55.760 | by way of memory.
01:27:57.200 | If we were to image the neurons in my brain,
01:27:59.960 | would the activity of neurons resemble the activity
01:28:04.440 | of neurons that's present when I open my eyes
01:28:08.600 | and look at your actual face?
01:28:09.640 | - This is a deep question.
01:28:11.040 | We don't really have a full accounting yet.
01:28:14.960 | Yes, except you're talking about looking in detail
01:28:18.760 | at the activity of neurons in a human brain
01:28:22.600 | and that's not as easy to do as it would be
01:28:25.120 | in some kind of animal model.
01:28:27.420 | But the bottom line is that you have a spatial representation
01:28:32.420 | of the visual world, laid as a map of the visual world,
01:28:36.740 | laid out on the surface of your cortex.
01:28:38.880 | The thing that's surprising is that it's not one map.
01:28:42.560 | It's actually dozens of maps.
01:28:44.840 | - What do each of those maps do?
01:28:46.360 | - Well, we don't really have a full accounting there either,
01:28:48.720 | but it looks a little bit like the diversification
01:28:52.200 | of the output neurons of the retina,
01:28:54.960 | the ganglion cells we were talking about before.
01:28:56.920 | There are different types of ganglion cells
01:28:58.760 | that are encoding different kinds of information
01:29:00.920 | about the visual world.
01:29:01.920 | We talk about the ones that were encoding the brightness,
01:29:05.020 | but other ones are encoding motion or color,
01:29:07.720 | these kinds of things.
01:29:08.800 | The same kinds of specializations
01:29:10.560 | in different representations of the visual world
01:29:12.960 | in the cortex seem to be true.
01:29:15.980 | It's a complex story.
01:29:17.200 | We don't have the whole picture yet,
01:29:19.900 | but it does look as if some parts of the brain
01:29:21.760 | are much more important for things like reaching for things
01:29:26.160 | in the space around you.
01:29:28.160 | And other parts of the cortex are really important
01:29:30.040 | for making associations between particular visual things
01:29:32.800 | you're looking at now and their significance.
01:29:35.840 | What is that object?
01:29:37.600 | What can it do for me?
01:29:38.440 | How can I use it?
01:29:39.640 | - What about the really specialized areas of cortex,
01:29:42.360 | like neurons that respond to particular faces,
01:29:46.460 | or neurons that, I don't know,
01:29:49.600 | can help me understand where I am
01:29:52.320 | relative to some other specific object?
01:29:54.680 | - Right, so these are properties of neurons
01:29:58.960 | that are extracted from, detected by,
01:30:03.960 | recording the activity of single neurons
01:30:07.500 | in some experimental system.
01:30:09.500 | What's going on when you actually perceive
01:30:11.880 | your grandmother's face is a much more complicated question.
01:30:15.440 | It clearly involves hundreds and thousands
01:30:17.560 | and probably millions of neurons
01:30:19.680 | acting in a cooperative way.
01:30:21.480 | So you can pick out any one little element
01:30:23.900 | in this very complicated system
01:30:26.060 | and see that it's responding differentially
01:30:28.160 | to certain kinds of visual patterns.
01:30:29.940 | And you think you're seeing a glimpse
01:30:31.620 | of some part of the process
01:30:33.040 | by which you recognize your grandmother's face.
01:30:36.140 | But that's a long way from a complete description.
01:30:38.940 | And it certainly isn't gonna be at the level
01:30:40.940 | of a magic single neuron that has the special stuff
01:30:44.160 | to recognize your grandmother.
01:30:45.320 | It's gonna be in some pattern of activity
01:30:47.640 | across many, many cells resonating
01:30:50.980 | in some kind of special way
01:30:52.900 | that will represent the internal memory of your mother.
01:30:57.580 | - Which is really incredible.
01:30:59.120 | - Yeah.
01:30:59.960 | - I mean, every time we do this deep dive,
01:31:01.620 | which we do from time to time,
01:31:02.840 | you and I, we kind of like march into the nervous system
01:31:05.240 | and explore how different aspects
01:31:08.640 | of our life experiences is handled there
01:31:12.520 | and how it's organized.
01:31:15.040 | It, after so many decades of doing this,
01:31:17.620 | it still boggles my mind that the collection of neurons,
01:31:22.620 | one through seven, active in a particular sequence
01:31:26.400 | gives the memory of a particular face
01:31:28.940 | and run backwards seven through to one.
01:31:32.600 | It gives you a complete, you know,
01:31:33.640 | could be, you know, rattlesnake, pit viper,
01:31:37.260 | heat sensing organs, as we were talking about earlier.
01:31:39.920 | So it sounds, is it true that there's a lot
01:31:43.240 | of multi-purposing of the circuitry?
01:31:45.520 | Like we can't say one area of the brain does A
01:31:49.280 | and another area of the brain does B.
01:31:50.960 | So, you know, areas can multitask
01:31:54.120 | or have multiple jobs, they can moonlight.
01:31:56.440 | - Right, but I think in my career,
01:32:01.120 | the hard problem has been to square that
01:32:05.160 | with the fact that, you know, things are specialized,
01:32:10.880 | that there are specific genes expressed in specific neurons
01:32:14.840 | that make them make synaptic connections
01:32:17.160 | with only certain other neurons.
01:32:19.240 | And that particular synaptic arrangement actually results
01:32:22.880 | in the processing of information that's useful
01:32:26.180 | to the animal to survive, right?
01:32:28.400 | So it's not as if it's either a big undifferentiated network
01:32:33.400 | of cells and looking at any one
01:32:35.840 | is never gonna tell you anything,
01:32:37.060 | that's too extreme on the one hand,
01:32:39.280 | nor is it the case that everything is hardwired
01:32:41.040 | and every neuron has one function
01:32:42.600 | and this all happens in one place in the brain.
01:32:45.760 | It's way more complicated and interactive
01:32:47.960 | and interconnected than that.
01:32:49.560 | - So we're not hardwired or softwired.
01:32:52.160 | We're sort of, I don't know what the analogy should be.
01:32:54.920 | What substance would work best, David?
01:32:57.120 | - No idea there, but you know,
01:32:58.920 | the idea is that it's always network activity.
01:33:03.420 | There's always many, many neurons involved
01:33:06.040 | and yet there's tremendous specificity
01:33:08.680 | in the neurons that might or might not be participating
01:33:10.940 | in any distributed function like that, right?
01:33:13.860 | So you have to get your mind around the fact
01:33:15.400 | that it's both very specific
01:33:17.300 | and very nonspecific at the same time.
01:33:19.360 | It's a little tricky to do,
01:33:20.600 | but I think that's kind of where the truth lies.
01:33:23.080 | - Yeah, and so this example that you mentioned
01:33:27.180 | to me once before about a woman who had a stroke
01:33:29.760 | in visual cortex, I think it speaks to some of this.
01:33:32.120 | - Right.
01:33:33.340 | - Could you share with us that story?
01:33:34.760 | - Sure, so the point is that you all,
01:33:38.480 | those of us who see, have representations
01:33:42.280 | of the visual world and our visual cortex.
01:33:44.680 | What happens to somebody when they become blind
01:33:49.680 | because of problems in the eye, the retina, perhaps?
01:33:53.460 | You have a big chunk of the cortex,
01:33:56.440 | this really valuable real estate for neural processing
01:33:59.520 | that has come to expect input from the visual system
01:34:05.040 | and there isn't any anymore.
01:34:06.480 | So you might think about that as fallow land, right?
01:34:08.960 | It's unused by the nervous system.
01:34:13.080 | And that would be a pity,
01:34:13.920 | but it turns out that it is in fact used.
01:34:16.640 | And the case that you're talking about
01:34:20.840 | is of a woman who was blind from very early in her life
01:34:25.840 | and who had risen through the ranks
01:34:30.400 | to a very high level executive secretarial position
01:34:33.720 | in a major corporation.
01:34:36.200 | And she was extremely good at braille reading
01:34:38.640 | and she had a braille typewriter
01:34:39.760 | and that's how everything was done.
01:34:41.960 | And apparently she had a stroke
01:34:44.980 | and was discovered at work, collapsed,
01:34:46.600 | and they brought her to the hospital.
01:34:48.460 | And apparently the neurologist who saw her
01:34:51.980 | when she finally came to said,
01:34:54.500 | I've got good news and bad news.
01:34:56.200 | Bad news is you've had a stroke.
01:34:57.960 | The good news is that it was in an area of your brain
01:35:00.260 | you're not even using, it's your visual cortex.
01:35:03.040 | And I know you're blind from birth,
01:35:04.580 | so there shouldn't be any issue here.
01:35:06.560 | The problem was she lost her ability to read braille.
01:35:10.000 | So what appears to have been the case,
01:35:13.500 | and this has been confirmed in other ways
01:35:16.380 | by imaging experiments in humans,
01:35:18.700 | is that in people who are blind from very early in birth,
01:35:22.020 | the visual cortex gets repurposed
01:35:25.260 | as a center for processing tactile information.
01:35:28.780 | And especially if you drain to be a good braille reader,
01:35:31.560 | you're actually reallocating somehow
01:35:34.320 | that real estate to your fingertips,
01:35:37.300 | you know, a part of the cortex
01:35:38.380 | that should be listening to the eyes.
01:35:40.260 | So that's an extreme level of plasticity,
01:35:42.440 | but what it shows is the visual cortex
01:35:45.740 | is kind of a general purpose processing machine.
01:35:48.760 | It's good at spatial information
01:35:51.620 | and the skin of your fingers is just another spatial sense
01:35:55.060 | and deprived of any other input.
01:35:57.360 | The brain seems smart enough,
01:35:59.620 | if you want to put it that way,
01:36:01.100 | to rewire itself, to use that real estate for something
01:36:05.160 | useful, in this case, reading braille.
01:36:07.120 | - Incredible.
01:36:08.840 | Somewhat tragic, but incredible.
01:36:10.340 | At least in that case, tragic.
01:36:12.280 | - Very informative.
01:36:13.120 | - Very informative.
01:36:14.080 | And of course it can go the other way too,
01:36:16.080 | where people can gain function in particular modalities
01:36:20.160 | like improved hearing or tactile function
01:36:22.640 | in the absence of vision.
01:36:23.960 | - Right.
01:36:24.800 | - Tell us about connectomes.
01:36:29.360 | We hear about genomes, proteomes, microbiomes,
01:36:33.040 | ohms, ohms, ohms these days.
01:36:35.580 | What's a connectome and why is it valuable?
01:36:39.360 | - Yeah, so connectome actually now has two meanings.
01:36:42.720 | So I only refer to the one that is my passion right now,
01:36:47.680 | and that is really trying to understand the structure
01:36:50.100 | of nervous tissue at a scale that's very, very fine.
01:36:55.100 | - Smaller than a millimeter.
01:36:59.160 | - Way smaller than a millimeter, a nanometer or less.
01:37:02.680 | As that's a thousand times smaller,
01:37:04.580 | or it's actually a million times smaller.
01:37:11.080 | So really, really tiny on the scale of individual synapses
01:37:16.080 | between individual neurons or even smaller,
01:37:18.140 | like the individual synaptic vesicles
01:37:20.440 | containing little packets of neurotransmitter
01:37:22.240 | that are gonna get released
01:37:23.800 | to allow one neuron to communicate to the next.
01:37:26.360 | So very, very fine.
01:37:28.300 | But the notion here is that you're doing this
01:37:33.300 | section after section at very fine scale.
01:37:39.300 | So in theory, what you have is a complete description
01:37:41.900 | of a chunk of nervous tissue that is so complete
01:37:46.300 | that if you took enough time to identify
01:37:48.080 | where the boundaries of all the cells are,
01:37:49.860 | you could come up with a complete description
01:37:52.380 | of the synaptic wiring of that chunk of nervous tissue
01:37:55.780 | because you have a complete description
01:37:56.860 | where all the cells are and where all the synapses
01:37:58.980 | between where all the cells are.
01:38:00.380 | So now you essentially have a wiring diagram
01:38:02.700 | of this complicated piece of tissue.
01:38:04.660 | So the omics part is the exhaustiveness of it.
01:38:08.540 | Rather than looking at a couple of synapses
01:38:10.620 | that are interesting to you from two different cell types,
01:38:13.860 | you're looking at all the synapses of all of the cell types,
01:38:17.980 | which of course is this massive avalanche of data, right?
01:38:22.680 | - So in genetics, you have genetics and then you have genomics
01:38:25.340 | which is the idea of getting the whole genome.
01:38:27.060 | - All of it.
01:38:27.900 | - And we don't really have an analogous word for genetics,
01:38:30.900 | but it would be connectivity and canomics.
01:38:33.180 | - Right, connectivity. - Excuse me, connectomics.
01:38:35.540 | - Connectomics, sure, sure.
01:38:36.980 | - Connectivity and connectomics.
01:38:38.020 | - Right, so it's wanting it all.
01:38:40.340 | And of course it's crazy ambitious,
01:38:42.700 | but that's where it gets fun.
01:38:45.240 | Really it's a use of electron microscopy,
01:38:48.540 | a very high resolution microscopic imaging system
01:38:53.480 | on a new scale with way more payoff
01:38:56.900 | in terms of understanding the connectivity
01:38:58.700 | of the nervous system.
01:38:59.540 | And it's just emerging,
01:39:01.520 | but I really think it's gonna revolutionize the field
01:39:04.300 | because we're gonna be able to query these circuits.
01:39:07.240 | How did they actually do it?
01:39:08.540 | Look at the hardware
01:39:09.900 | in a way that's never been possible before.
01:39:12.420 | - The way that I describe this to people is
01:39:15.100 | if you were to take a chunk of kind of cooked
01:39:18.300 | but cold spaghetti and slice it up very thin,
01:39:21.760 | and you're trying to connect up each image of each slice
01:39:25.940 | of the edge of the spaghetti
01:39:28.100 | as figure out which ropes of spaghetti belong to which.
01:39:30.820 | - And have a complete description
01:39:32.220 | of where this piece of spaghetti touches
01:39:33.820 | that piece of spaghetti,
01:39:34.660 | and is there something special there?
01:39:36.340 | - Where the meat sauce is and all the other cell types
01:39:38.680 | and the pesto, where it all is around the spaghetti,
01:39:43.680 | because those are the other cells,
01:39:44.740 | the blood vessels and the glial cells.
01:39:47.520 | So what's it good for?
01:39:49.820 | I mean, maps are great.
01:39:51.780 | I always think of connectomics and genomics and proteomics,
01:39:57.440 | et cetera, as necessary but not sufficient.
01:40:00.080 | - Right, right.
01:40:01.360 | So I mean, in many cases,
01:40:02.520 | what you do is you go out and probe the function,
01:40:05.860 | and you understand how the brain does the function
01:40:08.000 | by finding neurons that seem to be firing
01:40:10.920 | in association with this function that you're observing.
01:40:14.100 | And little by little, you work your way in,
01:40:15.700 | and now you wanna know what the connectivity is.
01:40:17.300 | Maybe the anatomy could help you.
01:40:19.720 | But this connectomics approach,
01:40:21.540 | or at least the serial electron microscopy
01:40:23.660 | reconstruction of neurons approach,
01:40:27.220 | really is allowing us to frame questions
01:40:29.960 | starting from the anatomy and saying,
01:40:33.460 | I see a synaptic circuit here.
01:40:35.120 | My prediction would be that these cell types
01:40:37.200 | would interact in a particular way.
01:40:38.860 | Is that right?
01:40:40.160 | And then you can go and probe the physiology,
01:40:42.180 | and you might be right or you might be wrong.
01:40:43.760 | But more often than not,
01:40:45.380 | it looks like the structure is pointing us
01:40:47.460 | in the right direction.
01:40:48.780 | So in my case, I'm using this to try to understand a circuit
01:40:53.500 | that is involved in this image stabilization network
01:40:56.160 | we're talking about, keeping things stable on the retina.
01:41:00.260 | And this thing will only respond
01:41:02.540 | at certain speeds of motion.
01:41:04.740 | These cells in the circuit, like slow motion,
01:41:07.300 | they won't respond to fast motion.
01:41:09.180 | How does that come about?
01:41:10.340 | Well, I was able to probe the circuitry.
01:41:14.060 | I knew what my cells looked like.
01:41:15.500 | I could see which other cells were talking to it.
01:41:17.460 | I could categorize all the cells
01:41:19.060 | that might be the players here
01:41:20.580 | that are involved in this mechanism
01:41:22.240 | of tuning the thing for slow speeds.
01:41:25.520 | And then we said, it looks like it's that cell type.
01:41:27.860 | And we went and looked and the data bore that up.
01:41:31.120 | But the anatomy drove the search for the particular cell type
01:41:34.500 | because we could see it connected in the right place
01:41:36.880 | to the right cells.
01:41:38.020 | So that creates the hypothesis
01:41:40.100 | that lets you go query the physiology,
01:41:42.620 | but it can go the other way as well.
01:41:44.020 | So it's always the synergy between these functional
01:41:46.100 | and structural approaches that gives you the most lift.
01:41:51.100 | But in many cases,
01:41:53.660 | the anatomy has been a little bit the weak sister in this,
01:41:56.660 | the structure, trying to work out the diagram
01:41:59.120 | because we haven't had the methods.
01:42:00.900 | Now the methods exist.
01:42:02.660 | And this whole field is expanding very quickly
01:42:05.740 | because people want these circuit diagrams
01:42:08.240 | for the particular part of the nervous system
01:42:10.900 | that they're working on.
01:42:12.400 | If you don't know the cell types and the connections,
01:42:14.300 | how do you really understand how the machine works?
01:42:17.380 | - Yeah, what I love about is
01:42:18.820 | we don't know what we don't know.
01:42:20.580 | And as scientists, we don't ask questions.
01:42:22.700 | We pose hypotheses.
01:42:23.860 | Hypotheses being, of course,
01:42:25.460 | some prediction that you wager your time on, basically.
01:42:29.260 | And it either turns out to be true or not true.
01:42:32.300 | But if you don't know that a particular cell type is there,
01:42:37.580 | you could never in any configuration of life
01:42:42.540 | or a career or exploration of a nervous system
01:42:46.540 | wager a hypothesis because you didn't know it was there.
01:42:49.300 | So this allows you to say,
01:42:50.140 | "Ah, there's this little interesting little connection
01:42:52.900 | between this cell that I know is interesting
01:42:54.980 | and another cell that's a little mysterious,
01:42:56.660 | but interesting.
01:42:57.600 | I'm going to hypothesize that it's doing blank,
01:43:00.020 | blank, and blank and go test that."
01:43:01.280 | And in the absence of these connectomes,
01:43:03.400 | you would never know that that cell
01:43:04.540 | was lurking there in the shadows.
01:43:06.540 | - Right, right.
01:43:07.980 | Yeah, and if you're just trying to understand
01:43:09.500 | how information flows through this biological machine,
01:43:14.240 | you want to know where things are.
01:43:16.380 | Neuro-transmitters are dumped out of the terminals
01:43:18.640 | of one cell and they diffuse across the space
01:43:21.880 | between the two cells, which is kind of a liquidy space,
01:43:24.600 | and they hit some receptors on the postsynaptic cell
01:43:26.700 | and they have some impact.
01:43:28.920 | Sometimes that's not through a regular synapse.
01:43:31.400 | Sometimes it's through a neuromodulator,
01:43:33.080 | like you often talk about on your podcast,
01:43:35.840 | that are sort of oozing-
01:43:36.680 | - Dopamine or something. - Dopamine, exactly.
01:43:38.220 | Oozing into the space between the cells,
01:43:40.540 | and it may be acting at some distance
01:43:42.700 | far from where it was released, right?
01:43:45.020 | But if you don't know where the release is happening
01:43:47.200 | and where other things are that might respond
01:43:49.200 | to that release, you're groping around in the dark.
01:43:52.500 | - Well, I love that you are doing this,
01:43:54.100 | and I have to share with the listeners
01:43:57.800 | that the first time I ever met David,
01:44:00.460 | and every time I've ever met with him in person,
01:44:03.220 | at least at his laboratory at Brown,
01:44:06.220 | he was in his office, door closed,
01:44:08.740 | drawing neurons and their connections.
01:44:11.740 | And this is somewhat unusual for somebody
01:44:13.700 | who's a, you know, endowed full professor,
01:44:16.500 | chairman of the department, et cetera, for many years,
01:44:19.240 | to be doing the hands-on work.
01:44:20.900 | Typically that's the stuff that's done by technicians
01:44:22.780 | or graduate students or postdocs.
01:44:24.180 | But I think it's fair to say that you really love
01:44:28.080 | looking at nervous systems and drawing
01:44:31.460 | the accurate renditions of how those nervous systems
01:44:34.940 | are organized and thinking about how they work.
01:44:37.340 | - Yeah, it's pure joy for me.
01:44:38.820 | I mean, I'm a very visual person.
01:44:40.440 | My wife is an artist.
01:44:41.560 | We look at a lot of art together.
01:44:43.540 | Just the forms of things are gorgeous in their own right.
01:44:47.580 | But to know that the form is, in a sense, the function,
01:44:51.280 | that the architecture of the connectivity
01:44:55.560 | is how the computation happens in the brain at some level,
01:44:59.460 | even though we don't fully understand that in most contexts,
01:45:02.840 | gives me great joy, 'cause I'm working on something
01:45:05.400 | that's both visually beautiful, but also deeply beautiful
01:45:09.880 | and sort of a higher sort of knowledge context.
01:45:14.880 | You know, what is it all about?
01:45:17.600 | - Love it.
01:45:18.440 | Well, as a final question, I get asked very often
01:45:21.560 | about how people should learn about neuroscience
01:45:24.080 | or how they should go about pursuing maybe an education
01:45:28.160 | in neuroscience if they're at that stage of their life
01:45:30.320 | or that's appropriate for their current trajectory.
01:45:34.040 | Do you have any advice to young people, old people,
01:45:37.080 | and anything in between about how to learn
01:45:39.720 | about the nervous system, maybe in a more formal way?
01:45:42.480 | I mean, obviously we have our podcast.
01:45:43.880 | There are other sources of neuroscience information
01:45:46.800 | out there, but for the young person who thinks
01:45:49.340 | they want to understand the brain,
01:45:51.600 | they want to learn about the brain,
01:45:53.800 | what should we tell them?
01:45:55.080 | - Well, that's a great question.
01:45:56.360 | And there's so many sources out there.
01:45:57.840 | It's almost a question of how do you deal with this avalanche
01:46:01.080 | of information out there?
01:46:02.840 | I mean, I think our podcast is a great way for people
01:46:05.320 | to learn more about the nervous system in an accessible way,
01:46:08.240 | but there's so much stuff out there.
01:46:09.760 | And it's not just that.
01:46:10.960 | I mean, the resources are becoming more and more available
01:46:13.920 | for average folks to participate in neuroscience research
01:46:18.580 | on some level.
01:46:19.420 | There's this famous eye wire project of Sebastian Sommer.
01:46:21.360 | - Oh yeah, maybe tell us about eye wire.
01:46:23.240 | - Yeah, so that's connectomics.
01:46:24.560 | And that's a situation where a very clever scientist
01:46:28.200 | realized that the physical work
01:46:31.600 | of doing all this reconstruction of neurons
01:46:34.720 | from these electron micrographs,
01:46:37.240 | there's a lot of time involved.
01:46:39.920 | Many, many person hours have to go into that
01:46:42.400 | to come up with the map that you want
01:46:44.760 | of where the cells are.
01:46:46.340 | And he was very clever about setting up a context
01:46:49.240 | in which he could crowdsource this.
01:46:51.360 | And people who were interested
01:46:52.200 | in getting a little experience, looking at nervous tissue
01:46:54.760 | and participating in a research project
01:46:57.440 | could learn how to do this and do a little bit.
01:46:59.600 | - From their living room.
01:47:00.440 | - From their living room, their laptop.
01:47:01.800 | - We'll put a link to eye wire.
01:47:03.040 | That's a, it also is a great bridge
01:47:04.600 | between what we were just talking about connectomics
01:47:06.480 | and actually participating in research.
01:47:08.520 | - Right.
01:47:09.360 | - And you don't need a graduate mentor
01:47:10.680 | or anything like that.
01:47:12.120 | - Right, so more of this is coming
01:47:14.640 | and I'm actually interested in building more of this
01:47:17.360 | so that people who are interested,
01:47:20.160 | want to participate at some level,
01:47:21.560 | don't necessarily have the time or resources
01:47:24.520 | to get involved in laboratory research
01:47:27.160 | can get exposed to it and participate
01:47:29.920 | and actually contribute.
01:47:31.120 | So I think that's one thing.
01:47:34.640 | I mean, just asking questions of the people around you
01:47:38.240 | who know a little bit more
01:47:39.400 | and have them point you in the right direction.
01:47:41.200 | Here's a book you might like to read.
01:47:42.600 | There's lots of great popular books out there
01:47:45.840 | that are accessible that will give you some more sense
01:47:48.760 | of the full range of what's out there in the neurosciences
01:47:51.720 | and how-
01:47:52.560 | - We can put some links to a few of those
01:47:53.640 | that we like on basic neuroscience.
01:47:56.280 | Our good friend, Dick Maslin, the late Richard,
01:47:59.560 | people will call him Dick, Dick Maslin had a good book.
01:48:02.360 | I forget the title at the moment.
01:48:05.360 | It's sitting behind me somewhere over there on the shelf,
01:48:07.320 | but about vision and how nervous systems work.
01:48:10.440 | A pretty accessible book for the general public.
01:48:12.600 | - Right, right.
01:48:13.480 | So that, and there's so many sources out there.
01:48:16.580 | I mean, Wikipedia is a great way.
01:48:18.120 | If you had a particular question about visual function,
01:48:20.160 | I would say by all means head to Wikipedia
01:48:23.040 | and get the first look and follow the references from there
01:48:27.560 | or go to your library or, you know,
01:48:30.320 | there's so many ways to get into it.
01:48:32.000 | It's such an exciting field now.
01:48:33.960 | There's so many, I mean,
01:48:35.080 | any particular realm that's special to you,
01:48:37.520 | your experience, your, you know, your strengths,
01:48:40.000 | your passions, there's a field of neuroscience
01:48:44.000 | devoted to that.
01:48:44.840 | You know, if you've got,
01:48:45.680 | if you know somebody who's got a neurological problem
01:48:47.920 | or a psychiatric problem,
01:48:50.480 | there's a branch of neuroscience
01:48:51.880 | that is devoted to trying to understand that
01:48:54.640 | and to solve these kinds of problems down the line.
01:48:57.480 | So feel the, feel the buzz.
01:49:00.960 | It's an exciting time to get involved.
01:49:03.380 | - Great, those are great resources
01:49:04.720 | that people can access from anywhere,
01:49:06.520 | zero cost as you need an internet connection.
01:49:08.780 | But aside from that, we'll put the links to some,
01:49:11.320 | and I'm remembering Dick's book is called
01:49:13.820 | "We Know It When We See It."
01:49:15.960 | - One of my heroes.
01:49:16.960 | - Yeah, a wonderful colleague,
01:49:18.480 | who unfortunately we lost a few years ago,
01:49:20.080 | but listen, David, this has been wonderful.
01:49:24.120 | - It's been a blast.
01:49:24.960 | - We really appreciate you taking the time to do this.
01:49:26.820 | As people probably realize by now,
01:49:29.320 | you're an incredible wealth of knowledge
01:49:31.040 | about the entire nervous system.
01:49:32.960 | Today, we just hit this top contour
01:49:35.240 | of a number of different areas
01:49:36.360 | to give a flavor of the different ways
01:49:38.340 | that the nervous system works and is organized
01:49:41.020 | and how that's put together,
01:49:43.360 | how these areas are talking to one another.
01:49:45.220 | What I love about you
01:49:46.060 | is that you're such an incredible educator
01:49:48.320 | and I've taught so many students over the years,
01:49:50.460 | but also for me personally as friends,
01:49:54.200 | but also anytime that I want to touch into the beauty
01:49:57.840 | of the nervous system, I rarely lose touch with it,
01:50:00.740 | but anytime I want to touch into it
01:50:02.160 | and start thinking about new problems
01:50:03.920 | and ways that the nervous system is doing things
01:50:06.480 | that I hadn't thought about, I call you.
01:50:08.360 | So please forgive me for the calls past, present,
01:50:12.040 | and future, unless you change your number.
01:50:13.880 | And even if you do, I'll be calling.
01:50:15.960 | It's been such a blast, Andy.
01:50:18.540 | This has been a great session
01:50:20.980 | and it's always fun talking to you.
01:50:22.700 | It always gets my brain racing.
01:50:24.960 | So thank you.
01:50:27.860 | - Thank you.
01:50:28.780 | Thank you for joining me today
01:50:29.880 | for my discussion with Dr. David Berson.
01:50:32.280 | By now, you should have a much clearer understanding
01:50:35.180 | of how the brain is organized
01:50:37.100 | and how it works to do all the incredible things
01:50:39.580 | that it does.
01:50:40.780 | If you're enjoying and/or learning from this podcast,
01:50:43.200 | please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
01:50:45.020 | That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.
01:50:47.820 | In addition, please subscribe to our podcast
01:50:50.300 | on Apple and Spotify.
01:50:52.020 | And on Apple, you have the opportunity
01:50:53.640 | to leave us up to a five-star review.
01:50:56.140 | As well, if you'd like to make suggestions
01:50:58.040 | for future podcast episode topics
01:51:00.440 | or future podcast episode guests,
01:51:02.900 | please put those in the comment section
01:51:04.960 | on our YouTube channel.
01:51:06.660 | Please also check out our sponsors mentioned
01:51:08.380 | at the beginning of each podcast.
01:51:09.900 | That's the best way to support us.
01:51:11.780 | And we have a Patreon.
01:51:13.020 | It's patreon.com/andrewhuberman.
01:51:15.880 | There, you can support us at any level that you like.
01:51:18.560 | While today's discussion did not focus on supplements,
01:51:21.580 | many previous podcast episodes include discussions
01:51:24.220 | about supplements.
01:51:25.220 | And while supplements aren't necessary for everybody,
01:51:28.220 | many people derive benefit from them
01:51:30.100 | for things like sleep or focus or anxiety relief and so on.
01:51:33.940 | One issue with the supplement industry, however,
01:51:36.140 | is that oftentimes the quality
01:51:38.060 | will really vary across brands.
01:51:40.580 | That's why we partnered with Thorne, T-H-O-R-I-N-E,
01:51:43.060 | because Thorne supplements
01:51:44.540 | are of the absolute highest standards
01:51:46.260 | in terms of the quality of the ingredients they include
01:51:48.520 | and the precision of the amounts
01:51:50.020 | of the ingredients they include.
01:51:51.420 | In other words, what's listed on the bottle
01:51:53.140 | is what's actually found in the bottle,
01:51:54.900 | which is not true of many supplements out there
01:51:57.080 | that have been tested.
01:51:58.280 | If you'd like to see the supplements that I take,
01:52:00.120 | you can go to thorne.com/u/huberman.
01:52:04.980 | And there, you can see the supplements that I take
01:52:06.980 | and you can get 20% off any of those supplements.
01:52:09.600 | And if you navigate deeper into the Thorne site
01:52:11.660 | through that portal, thorne.com/u/huberman,
01:52:15.640 | you can also get 20% off any of the other supplements
01:52:18.560 | that Thorne happens to make.
01:52:20.040 | If you're not already following Huberman Lab
01:52:21.780 | on Instagram and Twitter, feel free to do so.
01:52:24.720 | Both places I regularly post short video posts
01:52:27.860 | or text posts that give tools related to health
01:52:31.200 | and neuroscience and so forth.
01:52:32.780 | And most of the time, that information is non-overlapping
01:52:35.740 | with the information on the podcast.
01:52:37.140 | Again, it's just Huberman Lab on Instagram and Twitter.
01:52:39.680 | And last but not least,
01:52:41.240 | thank you for your interest in science.
01:52:43.120 | [upbeat music]