back to indexPaola Arlotta: Brain Development from Stem Cell to Organoid | Lex Fridman Podcast #32
Chapters
0:0 Intro
1:3 Is there intelligent life outside of Earth
2:46 Paola Arlotta on the human brain
5:27 Development of the human brain
8:19 Order of development matters
9:24 The development process
11:30 Biological systems are complex
13:53 Building process is not a dictatorship
15:26 Building blocks of the brain
16:51 Myelin in the brain
20:6 How much is nature and nurture
22:25 What are organoids
25:15 Building Organoids
27:5 How many can you build
28:19 What is an Organoid
32:0 Organoids
35:45 History of Brain Development
39:40 How difficult is it to build a human brain
40:22 The beauty of the human brain
42:45 The ethical framework
45:19 The downside
46:32 Politics of stem cell research
48:47 Language matters
50:12 Genetics
52:11 How has studying the brain changed you
54:7 The next brain
00:00:00.000 |
The following is a conversation with Paola Arlotta. 00:00:03.380 |
She's a professor of stem cell and regenerative biology 00:00:06.400 |
at Harvard University and is interested in understanding 00:00:09.780 |
the molecular laws that govern the birth, differentiation, 00:00:13.180 |
and assembly of the human brain's cerebral cortex. 00:00:25.660 |
It's part of the Artificial Intelligence Podcast. 00:00:30.620 |
give it five stars on iTunes, support it on Patreon, 00:00:39.980 |
And I'd like to give a special thank you to Amy Jeffress 00:00:45.580 |
She's an artist, and you should definitely check out 00:00:47.660 |
her Instagram at lovetruthgood, three beautiful words. 00:00:57.740 |
And now, here's my conversation with Paola Arlotta. 00:01:01.780 |
You studied the development of the human brain 00:01:10.540 |
How likely is it that there's intelligent life out there 00:01:30.420 |
- Well, it has happened here, right, on this planet? 00:01:36.340 |
So that simply tells you that it could, of course, 00:01:44.140 |
what the probability that you would get a brain 00:01:46.620 |
like the ones that we have, like the human brain. 00:01:51.220 |
So how difficult is it to make the human brain? 00:02:09.140 |
Most of what we know about how the mammalian brains, 00:02:13.380 |
or the brain of mammals, develop comes from studying, 00:02:22.580 |
But if I showed you a picture of a mouse brain, 00:02:25.420 |
and then you put it next to a picture of a human brain, 00:02:35.300 |
to what you can learn about how the human brain is made 00:02:39.780 |
There is a huge value in studying the mouse brain. 00:02:49.220 |
or through the mouse and through other methodologies 00:02:54.700 |
I mean, you're one of the experts in the world, 00:02:57.500 |
how much do you feel you know about the brain? 00:03:07.780 |
- Yeah, you pretty much find yourself in awe all the time. 00:03:30.420 |
and it can happen also from stem cells in a dish. 00:03:36.860 |
these stem cells that are present within the neural tube 00:03:39.860 |
give rise to all of the thousands and thousands 00:03:42.220 |
of different cell types that are present in the brain 00:03:49.980 |
interesting observation is that the time that it takes 00:03:54.900 |
for the human brain to be made, it's human time. 00:04:02.180 |
it took almost nine months of gestation to build a brain, 00:04:05.300 |
and then another 20 years of learning postnatally 00:04:16.780 |
- So it's mouse time. - For an embryo to be born. 00:04:19.220 |
And so the brain is built in a much shorter period of time. 00:04:23.820 |
And the beauty of it is that if you take mouse stem cells 00:04:33.340 |
is formed faster than if you took human stem cells 00:04:49.020 |
- Which means it's on purpose, it's not accidental. 00:05:07.340 |
It takes us a long time as human beings after we're born 00:05:13.060 |
to learn all the things that we have to learn 00:05:23.500 |
goes through puberty to adults, it's a long time. 00:05:30.340 |
the first few months and then on through the first 20 years 00:05:37.660 |
what is the development of the human brain look like? 00:05:42.620 |
- Yeah, at the beginning you have to build a brain, right? 00:05:56.060 |
in addition to making all of the other tissues 00:05:58.580 |
of the embryo, the muscle, the heart, the blood, 00:06:13.980 |
sort of the length of the embryo from the head 00:06:16.980 |
all the way to the tail, let's say, of the embryo. 00:06:19.940 |
And then in human beings, over many months of gestation, 00:06:25.540 |
from that neural tube, which contains stem cell-like cells 00:06:39.620 |
'cause there are many, many different types of cells 00:06:41.900 |
in the brain, that will form specific structures 00:06:49.260 |
of the brain as just the time in which you are making 00:06:56.860 |
like uniform, or are they all different type? 00:07:09.980 |
- Multipotent means that it has the potential 00:07:13.540 |
to make many, many different types of other cells. 00:07:19.820 |
become more heterogeneous, which means more diverse. 00:07:22.860 |
There are gonna be many different types of the stem cells. 00:07:33.340 |
that are very different from the mother stem cell. 00:07:36.100 |
And now you think about this process of making cells 00:07:43.860 |
And what you're doing, you're building the cells 00:07:48.260 |
and then you arrange them in specific structures 00:07:55.460 |
So you can think about the embryonic development 00:07:58.740 |
of the brain as the time where you're building the bricks. 00:08:02.180 |
You're putting the bricks together to form buildings, 00:08:15.300 |
that transmit action potentials and electricity. 00:08:19.300 |
- I've heard you also say somewhere, I think, 00:08:22.300 |
that the order of the way this builds matters. 00:08:26.100 |
If you are an engineer and you think about development, 00:08:29.900 |
you can think of it as, well, I could also take all the cells 00:08:34.900 |
and bring them all together into a brain in the end. 00:08:40.380 |
So the cells are made in a very specific order 00:08:43.900 |
that subserve the final product that you need to get. 00:08:52.260 |
and all of the supportive cells of the neurons, 00:08:58.420 |
because they have to assemble together in specific ways. 00:09:03.380 |
why don't we just put them all together in the end? 00:09:05.740 |
It's because as they develop next to each other, 00:09:15.420 |
than a glia cell be made in a developing embryo 00:09:27.820 |
From my perspective in artificial intelligence, 00:09:29.780 |
you often think of how incredible the final product is, 00:09:35.220 |
But you're making me realize that the final product 00:09:44.460 |
Do we know the code that drives that development? 00:10:16.380 |
and this is why developmental neurobiologists 00:10:29.740 |
of really fine tuning gene expression programs 00:10:33.180 |
that allow certain cells to be made at a certain time 00:10:41.700 |
but also mechanical forces of pressure, bending. 00:10:46.700 |
This embryo is not just, it will not stay a tube, 00:10:52.020 |
At some point, this tube in the front of the embryo 00:10:54.900 |
will expand to make the primordium of the brain, right? 00:10:58.180 |
Now the forces that control, that the cells feel, 00:11:06.820 |
which is different from a week before or a week ago, 00:11:18.380 |
"or you are in a stretch of cells," or whatever it is. 00:11:30.340 |
It's not only chemical, it's also mechanical. 00:11:34.520 |
biology is this incredibly complex mess, gooey mess. 00:11:47.860 |
or any kind of mechanical machine that we humans build 00:11:54.740 |
- 'Cause you've worked a lot with biological systems. 00:12:13.080 |
we go back to printing a brain versus developing a brain. 00:12:20.500 |
given that you start with the same building blocks, 00:12:24.020 |
you could potentially print it the same way every time. 00:12:28.660 |
But that final brain may not work the same way 00:12:34.480 |
because the very same building blocks that you're using 00:12:38.740 |
developed in a completely different environment, right? 00:12:43.060 |
Therefore, they're gonna be different just by definition. 00:12:52.740 |
which maybe we will be talking about in a few minutes. 00:13:03.380 |
you can see that sometimes things can go wrong 00:13:07.540 |
And by wrong, I mean different one organoid from the next. 00:13:14.820 |
So this development, for as complex as it is, 00:13:26.180 |
But it's not the same if you develop it in a dish. 00:13:31.180 |
And first of all, we don't even develop a brain, 00:13:33.820 |
you develop something much simpler in the dish. 00:13:36.100 |
But there are more options for building things differently, 00:13:47.940 |
for how in the end the brain is built in vivo. 00:14:11.540 |
It seems like there's a really strong distributed mechanism. 00:14:20.940 |
And if you think about, for example, different species, 00:14:31.060 |
from that of a chicken, from that of one of us, 00:14:35.540 |
and so on and so forth, and still is a brain, 00:14:46.020 |
But in the end, evolution builds different brains 00:14:50.900 |
because that serves in a way the purpose of that species 00:15:04.860 |
Nobody knows what the entire code of development is. 00:15:08.660 |
We know bits and pieces of very specific aspects 00:15:14.500 |
what genes are involved to make a certain cell types, 00:15:22.820 |
how it's so well controlled, it's really mind blowing. 00:15:29.140 |
or whatever, the first few weeks, months, months. 00:15:32.740 |
So yeah, the building blocks are constructed, 00:15:37.140 |
the actual, the different regions of the brain, I guess, 00:15:52.060 |
but then there is continuous building of new cell types 00:16:06.660 |
that is built around the cables of the neurons 00:16:09.780 |
so that the electricity can go really fast from- 00:16:15.980 |
And so as human beings, we myelinate our cells 00:16:28.700 |
the process of making the mature oligodendrocytes, 00:16:42.420 |
So there is a continuous process of maturation 00:16:50.620 |
- I remember taking AP Biology in high school, 00:16:58.580 |
that scientists disagree on the purpose of myelin 00:17:24.860 |
and you can think about it as a cable of some type 00:17:29.380 |
And what myelin does, by insulating the outside, 00:17:36.340 |
and pieces of axons that are naked without myelin. 00:17:41.740 |
the electricity, instead of going straight through the cable, 00:17:47.220 |
to the next naked little piece and jump again. 00:17:49.940 |
And therefore, that's the idea that you go faster. 00:17:52.700 |
And it was always thought that in order to build 00:18:07.820 |
because you wanna go fast with this information 00:18:17.100 |
or so, we discovered that some of the most evolved, 00:18:23.140 |
that we have as non-human primates, as human beings 00:18:33.180 |
well, those have axons that have very little myelin. 00:18:42.020 |
in which they put this myelin on their axons, 00:18:45.500 |
then a long track with no myelin, another chunk there, 00:19:10.780 |
but if we give evolution another few million years, 00:19:14.140 |
we'll see a lot of myelin on these neurons too. 00:19:21.060 |
- Less myelin might allow for more flexibility 00:19:38.500 |
so in the timing, you can encode a lot of information. 00:19:44.700 |
- The timing, the chemistry of that little piece of axon, 00:19:48.620 |
perhaps it's a dynamic process where the myelin can move. 00:19:52.180 |
Now you see how many layers of variability you can add, 00:19:58.980 |
if you're trying to come up with a new function 00:20:02.340 |
or a new capability or something unpredictable in a way. 00:20:19.060 |
we seem to be kind of somewhat smart, intelligent, 00:20:25.300 |
cognition, consciousness, all of these things 00:20:28.340 |
are just incredible ability to reason and so on, emerge. 00:20:32.100 |
In your sense, how much is in the hardware, in the nature, 00:20:41.100 |
through interacting with the environment and so on? 00:20:48.060 |
that has most of his cells and most of his structures, 00:21:04.140 |
of interacting with the environment around us. 00:21:07.060 |
And so what that brain that was so perfectly built 00:21:15.820 |
will then be used to incorporate the environment 00:21:27.900 |
like if you and I may have had a different childhood 00:21:31.940 |
or a different, we have been going to different schools, 00:21:36.460 |
and our brain is a little bit different because of that, 00:21:48.780 |
What that means is a brain that is able to change 00:21:56.340 |
So perhaps some of the most illuminating studies 00:22:02.500 |
in which the sensory organs were not working, right? 00:22:06.740 |
Like if you are born with eyes that don't work, 00:22:14.620 |
the visual cortex, develops postnatally differently, 00:22:19.620 |
and it might be used to do something different, right? 00:22:40.860 |
- And how can you use them to help us understand the brain 00:22:52.660 |
The first thing I'd like to say is that an organoid, 00:22:56.020 |
a brain organoid, is not the same as a brain, okay? 00:23:17.140 |
of the development of the brain, but not all of it. 00:23:23.100 |
Maximum, they become about four to five millimeters 00:23:27.860 |
They are much simpler than our brain, of course, 00:23:45.060 |
Remember when I told you that we can't understand 00:23:47.980 |
everything about development in our own brain 00:23:51.460 |
Well, we can study the actual process of development 00:23:54.100 |
of the human brain because it all happens in utero, 00:23:56.220 |
so we will never have access to that process, ever. 00:24:04.220 |
like a bunch of stem cells that can be coaxed 00:24:08.300 |
into starting a process of neural tube formation. 00:24:11.620 |
Remember that tube that is made by the embryo, Leon? 00:24:40.100 |
that are also many different types of autism. 00:24:50.860 |
make a stem cell, and then with that stem cell, 00:24:54.300 |
watch a process of formation of a brain organoid 00:25:02.180 |
and you can ask, what is this genetic code doing 00:25:08.740 |
And for the first time, you may come to solutions 00:25:11.980 |
like what cells are involved in autism, right? 00:25:20.460 |
for that particular person with that genetic code, 00:25:33.260 |
- Yes, or how much variability is the flip side of that. 00:25:40.260 |
in building organoids than there is in building brain. 00:25:54.860 |
where it builds a brain in the context of a body, 00:26:05.540 |
we don't have the full code for how this is done. 00:26:09.460 |
And so in part, the organoid somewhat builds itself, 00:26:13.380 |
because there are some structures of the brain 00:26:17.180 |
And another part comes from the investigator, 00:26:39.620 |
is much more simple than what you get in vivo. 00:26:42.580 |
It mimics early events of development as of today, 00:26:46.100 |
and it doesn't build very complex type of anatomy 00:27:07.340 |
how hard and maybe another flip side of that, 00:27:10.380 |
expensive is it to go from one stem cell to an organoid? 00:27:18.420 |
- It's work, definitely, and it's money, definitely. 00:27:34.580 |
- Yeah, which is how many cells, sorry to ask. 00:27:35.420 |
- So this is about the size of a tiny, tiny, you know, 00:27:55.500 |
So the problem is not to grow more or less of them. 00:28:00.500 |
It's really to figure out how to grow them in a way 00:28:09.980 |
so they can be used to study a biological process. 00:28:22.060 |
Are there different neurons already emerging? 00:28:27.580 |
can you tell me what kind of neurons are there? 00:28:49.420 |
I told you that the brain has different parts. 00:28:56.020 |
but there is another region called the striatum 00:28:57.980 |
that is below the cortex and so on and so forth. 00:28:59.980 |
All of these regions have different types of cells 00:29:05.660 |
And so scientists have been able to grow organoids 00:29:13.940 |
And so we are very interested in the cerebral cortex. 00:29:25.300 |
the part of the brain that really truly makes us human, 00:29:30.300 |
And so in the attempt to make the cerebral cortex, 00:29:33.700 |
and by figuring out a way to have these organoids 00:29:37.300 |
continue to grow and develop for extended periods of times, 00:29:52.220 |
so the glia cells of the cerebral cortex also appear. 00:30:09.060 |
but of course they have much more active type of roles 00:30:14.580 |
which are the point of contact and communication 00:30:18.860 |
- So all that chemistry fun happens in the synapses 00:30:36.380 |
including the ability to make neurotransmitters, 00:30:40.420 |
which are the chemicals that are secreted to the synapses, 00:30:43.380 |
including the ability of making these axons grow 00:30:46.540 |
with their growth cones and so on and so forth. 00:31:06.540 |
So the mechanical and the chemical stuff happens. 00:31:13.380 |
because scientists have been culturing neurons forever. 00:31:18.220 |
And when you take a neuron, even a very young one, 00:31:21.740 |
eventually finds another cell or another neuron to talk to, 00:31:30.660 |
- So you can culture a neuron, like a single neuron, 00:31:33.300 |
and give it a little friend, and it starts interacting? 00:31:40.300 |
it sounds, it's more simple than what it may sound to you. 00:31:43.580 |
Neurons have molecular properties and structural properties 00:31:48.380 |
that allow them to really communicate with other cells. 00:31:55.220 |
chances are that they will form synapses with each other. 00:32:10.500 |
mimic some properties of the cerebral cortex, for example. 00:32:18.020 |
by studying an organoid of a cerebral cortex? 00:32:23.100 |
how all this incredible diversity of cell type, 00:32:26.460 |
all these many, many different classes of cells, 00:32:34.940 |
And what goes wrong if now the genetics of that stem cell 00:32:41.140 |
came from a patient with a neurodevelopmental disease? 00:32:47.580 |
what may have gone wrong years before in this kid 00:32:54.700 |
In a way, it's a little tiny rudimentary window 00:32:59.580 |
into the past, into the time when that brain in a kid 00:33:05.060 |
that had this neurodevelopmental disease was being made. 00:33:12.820 |
because today we have no idea of what cell types, 00:33:36.540 |
Which ones are the ones that really messed up here 00:33:44.460 |
Is it a less strong kind of circuit and brain? 00:33:59.300 |
rather than just based on current diagnostics. 00:34:14.020 |
How hard is it to detect that, wait a minute, 00:34:26.580 |
- 'Cause things can go wrong at multiple levels, right? 00:34:39.660 |
with other cells differently and so on and so forth. 00:34:52.180 |
of a single cell in a sea of cells with high precision. 00:35:06.220 |
and an individual with a neurodevelopmental disease, 00:35:10.180 |
that may tell you what is different molecularly. 00:35:13.820 |
Or you could see that some cells are not even made, 00:35:18.620 |
for example, or that the process of maturation 00:35:26.660 |
and we can study the cells at the molecular level, 00:35:29.620 |
but also we can use the organoids to ask questions 00:35:38.980 |
how they respond to a stimulus and so on and so forth. 00:35:41.420 |
And we may get at abnormalities there, right? 00:35:59.820 |
if you and I time travel a thousand years into the future, 00:36:12.820 |
but something that has properties of a brain. 00:36:15.740 |
So it feels like you might be getting close to, 00:36:19.060 |
in the building process, to build this, to understand. 00:36:30.300 |
- A thousand years from now, it's a long time from now. 00:36:38.220 |
- So I mean, if, you know, like they write a book, 00:36:44.020 |
- Let's write the science fiction book today. 00:36:53.100 |
I was a big fan in high school of reading Freud and so on. 00:37:19.740 |
even 10 years ago or 20, certainly not 20 years ago, 00:37:23.180 |
to even think about experimentally investigating 00:37:35.500 |
Now we have some technologies which I'll spell out 00:37:39.620 |
that allow us to actually look at the real thing 00:37:46.900 |
There has been huge progress in stem cell biology. 00:37:50.500 |
The moment someone figured out how to turn a skin cell 00:38:06.100 |
And in fact, there was a Nobel Prize for that. 00:38:08.220 |
That started the field really of using stem cells 00:38:14.260 |
Now we can build on all the knowledge of development 00:38:17.060 |
that we build over the many, many, many years 00:38:20.740 |
now make more and more complex aspects of development 00:38:24.420 |
So this field is young, the field of brain organoids, 00:38:32.580 |
that is rooted in labs with the right ethical framework 00:38:51.460 |
to basically study the properties of single cells 00:39:04.780 |
that has millions of cells can be profiled in a way, 00:39:11.260 |
the single cell level to really understand what is going on. 00:39:14.900 |
And you could do it in multiple stages of development 00:39:17.460 |
and you can build your hypothesis and so on and so forth. 00:39:25.180 |
And I see these as sort of an exponential growth 00:39:34.980 |
And so we're gonna see something transformative 00:39:36.940 |
that we didn't see at all in the prior thousand years. 00:39:41.860 |
- So I apologize for the crazy sci-fi questions, 00:40:09.380 |
because you don't have to build the whole human brain 00:40:25.140 |
It shows to me the difference between you and I 00:40:35.500 |
thousands or millions of people with disease and so on. 00:40:41.460 |
we're trying to build systems that we can put in robots 00:40:49.100 |
of the intelligence about reasoning about the world, 00:40:58.700 |
But we operate in science fiction a little bit. 00:41:03.460 |
even though that is not the focus or interest 00:41:05.820 |
perhaps of the community, how difficult is it? 00:41:11.220 |
- I think the field will progress, like I said, 00:41:13.980 |
and that the system will be more and more complex in a way. 00:41:18.740 |
But there are properties that emerge from the human brain 00:41:26.820 |
may have to do with intelligence or whatever, 00:41:31.980 |
even how they can emerge from an actual real brain. 00:41:35.580 |
And therefore we can now measure or study in an organoid. 00:41:39.140 |
So I think that this field, many, many years from now, 00:41:43.020 |
may lead to the building of better neural circuits 00:41:52.260 |
And it's hard to predict how complex this really will be. 00:42:00.220 |
it makes me laugh really, it's really that far 00:42:23.740 |
that there is no other way to do this science, 00:42:28.260 |
because where you're going with this is also, 00:42:30.660 |
we can talk about science fiction and write that book 00:42:36.620 |
but this work happens in a specific ethical framework 00:42:44.940 |
- So the ethical framework here is a fascinating one, 00:42:53.340 |
of how we think about ethically of building organoids 00:42:58.340 |
from human stem cells to understand the brain? 00:43:11.100 |
or at least start the cure by understanding it, 00:43:34.500 |
and from the ones that we will have tomorrow. 00:43:42.060 |
very simple models that clearly can help you in many ways 00:43:49.940 |
but tomorrow we need to have another conversation 00:43:55.980 |
really bring together constantly a group of people 00:43:58.940 |
that are not only scientists, but also bioethicists, 00:44:17.580 |
Now, I also think though, that as a scientist, 00:44:23.740 |
So if you think about how transformative it could be 00:44:28.740 |
for understanding and curing a neuropsychiatric disease, 00:44:43.220 |
How transformative at this moment in time, this could be. 00:44:47.220 |
We couldn't do it five years ago, we could do it now. 00:44:50.820 |
- Taking a stem cell of a particular patient. 00:45:04.700 |
And we could understand perhaps what is going wrong. 00:45:08.260 |
Perhaps we could use as a platform, as a cellular platform 00:45:15.220 |
So we could do it now, we couldn't do it five years ago. 00:45:24.740 |
- I don't see a downside at this very moment. 00:45:30.020 |
I'm sure there would be somebody who would argue against it. 00:45:37.580 |
- So it's exactly perhaps what you alluded at 00:45:47.140 |
enabling some process of formation of the brain 00:45:59.060 |
that ethically we don't wanna see in a tissue. 00:46:03.980 |
So today, I repeat, today, this is not an issue. 00:46:07.740 |
And so you just gain dramatically from the science 00:46:14.340 |
and so different in a way from the actual brain. 00:46:19.980 |
we have an obligation to really consider all of this, right? 00:46:27.180 |
where we should put disease and betterment of humanity 00:46:35.420 |
there was some politicization of embryonic stem cells 00:46:49.140 |
Is that still a force that we have to think about, 00:47:37.980 |
or not allowed in the future to do with the system. 00:47:43.340 |
because they bring reality of data to the conversation. 00:47:53.340 |
- Data needs to have a voice because not only data, 00:48:30.300 |
your reaction is gonna be very different to this. 00:48:37.740 |
I mean, we and journalists alike need to be a bit careful 00:48:42.500 |
that this debate is a real debate and informed by real data. 00:49:01.480 |
You are, as I've seen from a presentation, you're a parent. 00:49:06.220 |
I saw you show a couple of pictures of your son. 00:49:14.140 |
So what have you learned from the human brain 00:49:31.540 |
That we have a responsibility to foster their growth 00:49:36.540 |
in good, healthy ways that keep them curious, 00:49:48.940 |
which is in part coming from the genetics we talked about. 00:49:52.340 |
My children are very different from each other 00:49:57.800 |
I also learned that what you do for them comes back to you. 00:50:07.620 |
most of the time, have perhaps a decent kids at the end. 00:50:12.180 |
- So what do you think, just a quick comment, 00:50:13.740 |
what do you think is the source of that difference? 00:50:17.760 |
That's often the surprising thing for parents. 00:50:25.580 |
they're so different, yet they came from the same parents. 00:50:35.700 |
creates every time a genetically different individual 00:50:43.740 |
that is a different mix every time from the two parents. 00:50:46.540 |
And so they're not twins, they are genetically different. 00:50:55.340 |
'Cause you said really from a biological perspective, 00:51:02.420 |
So the genetics you have, the genes that you have 00:51:05.420 |
that play that beautiful orchestrated symphony 00:51:33.480 |
And so you are born superficially with the same brain. 00:51:45.220 |
and how the cells will then react to the environment 00:51:49.580 |
will be also shaped by who genetically you are. 00:51:55.160 |
this is not something that comes from my work. 00:52:01.180 |
that they have a different personality in a way, right? 00:52:23.420 |
How has studying the brain changed the way you see yourself 00:52:32.420 |
when you see your own life, your own mortality? 00:52:37.380 |
It's almost impossible to dissociate sometime for me. 00:52:45.340 |
or some of the things that other people do from, 00:53:01.140 |
and being at times funny in the way they think. 00:53:06.340 |
it's because they're going through this period of time 00:53:13.300 |
where their synapses are being eliminated here and there 00:53:17.780 |
And so from that comes perhaps a different take 00:53:22.300 |
on that behavior or maybe I can justify it scientifically 00:53:37.060 |
and the kind of ideas that we can come up with. 00:54:20.300 |
When you imagine over a period of a thousand years, 00:54:31.420 |
Artificial intelligence as it is hoped by many, 00:54:40.620 |
- Yeah, I think in a way that will happen, right? 00:54:45.420 |
It's almost like a part of the way we evolve. 00:54:53.220 |
that shape us as we grow up and so on and so forth. 00:54:56.660 |
Sometime I think about something that may sound silly, 00:55:04.660 |
Part of me thinks that somehow in their brain 00:55:13.720 |
And this comes from a lot of studies in modern organisms 00:55:24.260 |
So if we need to move our fingers in a very specific way, 00:55:54.640 |
- In artificial intelligence, it may merge with it, 00:56:08.780 |
The very space around us, the fact, for example, 00:56:11.820 |
think about we put on some goggles of virtual reality 00:56:15.240 |
and we physically are surfing the ocean, right? 00:56:18.820 |
Like I've done it and you have all these emotions 00:56:31.160 |
It didn't take thousands of years of adapting to this. 00:56:36.040 |
The brain is plastic, so adapts to new technology. 00:56:41.800 |
by simply hijacking some sensory capacities that we have. 00:56:51.600 |
the cerebral cortex has been a part of the brain 00:56:56.000 |
So we have put a lot of chips on evolving this specific brain 00:57:06.000 |
It's this ability to change in response to things. 00:57:10.320 |
So yes, they will integrate, that we want it or not.