back to indexTools for Better Productivity & Time Management | Dr. Adam Grant & Dr. Andrew Huberman
00:00:00.000 |
I think it was E.B. White who said, "I rise in the morning torn between the desire to 00:00:07.720 |
enjoy the world and the desire to improve the world, and this makes it difficult to 00:00:14.800 |
I think, I mean, I even, I felt it this morning. 00:00:17.960 |
I was like, "Okay, it's time to leave to come to the Huberman podcast." 00:00:22.560 |
I'm like, "Wait, but I didn't hit my minimum sunlight viewing. 00:00:28.600 |
Do I show up on time for you or do I meet your criteria?" 00:00:33.440 |
The explanation, "I was getting my morning sunlight and therefore I'm X number of minutes 00:00:37.080 |
or even hours late," would have been completely fine. 00:00:42.040 |
That's a built-in acceptable excuse with you. 00:00:44.520 |
I think, I mean, I think everybody experiences a version of this, and it's definitely gotten 00:00:49.600 |
worse with social media and with smartphones. 00:00:53.320 |
I think, so one of the most startling data points for me was Gloria Mark first put this 00:01:00.280 |
Before COVID, the average person was checking email 72 times a day. 00:01:05.680 |
How do you ever concentrate for more than a couple of minutes if you're self-interrupting 00:01:15.820 |
And she says, "We're taking these meaningful blocks of time and we're slicing them up into 00:01:22.600 |
And not only can we not accomplish anything, we're also eroding our own sense of joy." 00:01:29.560 |
Because it's really hard to enjoy the 30-second blip of time that you get on a task. 00:01:36.000 |
And I think we know a lot more about the existence of these problems than how to solve them. 00:01:40.600 |
But one thing we do know is blocking out uninterrupted time is meaningful. 00:01:45.800 |
There's a great Leslie Perlow experiment where she takes engineers and she has them – she 00:01:52.620 |
No interruptions Tuesday, Thursday, Friday before noon. 00:02:02.200 |
So quiet time, there are a couple of iterations of it, but I think the most effective one 00:02:04.960 |
was Tuesday, Thursday, Friday, no meetings, no interruptions, no Slack, no emails before 00:02:11.920 |
And during those periods of no interruptions, one could tend to whatever their primary purpose 00:02:20.220 |
Obviously, I don't have my phone in here, never do. 00:02:23.480 |
But it doesn't mean no interaction with anyone else. 00:02:29.800 |
And you come in with a clear sense of priority and purpose. 00:02:32.800 |
And I don't think there's anything magical about Tuesday, Thursday, Friday before noon. 00:02:35.920 |
It's just the idea of setting a boundary and collectively committing to it that seems 00:02:41.880 |
And when I think about this, I'd be really curious about your take on chronotypes here. 00:02:48.680 |
Because I think one thing I've learned in the last couple of years is that if you're 00:02:52.000 |
a morning person, you do your best analytical and creative thinking in the morning. 00:02:56.500 |
And so the quiet time block would work very well for me as a morning person. 00:03:00.320 |
If you're a night owl, you probably want that block in the late afternoon. 00:03:04.480 |
And I was encouraged, there was some evidence during COVID that people have their best meetings 00:03:10.920 |
That they're something like 30% less likely to multitask in an after-lunch meeting. 00:03:15.720 |
And I guess, you know, you could probably unpack the food coma, you know, getting re-energized 00:03:24.160 |
But it's led me to wonder if we should all be protecting the first few hours and the 00:03:30.680 |
And then doing our core meetings and interactions and kind of off-task activities in the middle. 00:03:37.280 |
Yeah, well, I have a lot of questions about this for you. 00:03:44.800 |
I think there's ample evidence to support the fact that provided one is sleeping well 00:03:49.280 |
at night and is on a more or less a standard schedule. 00:03:51.520 |
When I say standard, I mean going to bed somewhere between, let's say, 9.30 and 11.30 p.m., 00:03:56.720 |
waking up sometime between, let's say, 6 a.m. and 8 a.m., maybe 5.30 to 7.30, something 00:04:05.260 |
That's a highly unusual night out or super early bird. 00:04:10.720 |
For people that are following that sort of schedule, the first, let's just say from zero 00:04:15.460 |
to eight hours after waking, there tends to be a fairly robust increase in all the catecholamines, 00:04:23.200 |
so dopamine, norepinephrine, epinephrine, which generally, okay, generally speaking, 00:04:27.420 |
lead to increases in alertness, attention, and focus that are great for analytic work, 00:04:33.960 |
for implementation of strategies that you already understand, and you need to churn 00:04:41.340 |
And of course, there's a big increase in the morning, especially if you view morning sunlight, 00:04:45.320 |
a healthy increase, I should say, in cortisol. 00:04:48.960 |
You want cortisol, but you want that peak early in the day. 00:04:51.920 |
Okay, so for most people, it seems, at least my understanding is that that period of time, 00:04:59.120 |
zero to eight hours after waking or so, is best devoted to the "most critical tasks," 00:05:06.780 |
but one of the common problems is that people take that ability to implement a known strategy 00:05:12.320 |
and they start battering back all the emails. 00:05:16.160 |
Or talking to all, by the way, talking to coworkers is great, and it's often required, 00:05:20.640 |
but the question is whether or not it's productive conversation or whether or not it's just conversation. 00:05:25.700 |
And we tend to have a lot of energy early in the day, and I'm obsessed with the idea 00:05:28.680 |
of neural energy as opposed to just caloric energy. 00:05:33.380 |
And then post-lunch, so really, as we get to sort of nine to 17 hours after waking, 00:05:40.680 |
there is a dip in autonomic arousal that during the middle of the day, that postprandial dip, 00:05:44.680 |
those are post-lunch sleepiness, that can be partially offset by delaying your morning 00:05:48.960 |
caffeine a bit if you have the afternoon crash. 00:05:51.120 |
But it's interesting that you know that more productive meetings and less task switching 00:05:55.800 |
and distraction occurred in meeting set after lunch, because that makes me think that perhaps 00:06:02.040 |
being a little bit less alert is going to lend itself to more focus. 00:06:08.080 |
And indeed, that's the sort of optimal state, relaxed but focused, you're not sleepy, but 00:06:14.520 |
you also don't have so much intrinsic energy that you're tending to a bunch of things, 00:06:18.640 |
because I think a lot of people do feel that way. 00:06:20.720 |
I mean, I'm drinking double espresso right now, late morning, and I can sit still, but 00:06:29.720 |
I think certain Zoom meetings, how do I say this? 00:06:35.880 |
They are not content rich enough to grab all my attention. 00:06:39.640 |
And nowadays, of course, there are multiple screens. 00:06:41.280 |
Typically, I've got two phones and a computer, and you have to really spend some work to 00:06:45.240 |
flip over those phones while I'm on a Zoom and things like that. 00:06:49.800 |
So it's maybe the reduction in autonomic arousal that supports what you just described, but 00:06:57.000 |
My thinking, or my understanding rather, was that creative work and kind of brainstorming 00:07:07.200 |
I've noticed when lecturing, I'd be curious what your experience is with in university 00:07:11.680 |
lectures when I held courses in the evening, I used to like to hold my courses 5 to 7 PM 00:07:17.640 |
or even 7 to 9.30 PM when I was teaching undergraduates, that people were much looser and more relaxed. 00:07:24.480 |
And I always thought that that might have something to do with an increase in GABA transmission 00:07:29.460 |
that's known to happen in late evening, that people are just kind of more relaxed and less 00:07:35.640 |
They've been around people for much of the day. 00:07:40.560 |
I don't have any firm neuroscience explanations for what you described, but there are some 00:07:43.960 |
emerging theories about how that might work, and it has this 0 to 9 hours, phase one, 9 00:07:49.400 |
to 17 hours, phase two, and then of course, from 17 to 24 hours, I'll call it phase three, 00:07:58.200 |
Well, I think there's a confound in your teaching experience, which is undergrads often sleep 00:08:03.520 |
in until what, noon, or they might be up until 4 AM. 00:08:06.800 |
Or at least 10 AM seems to be a typical rise time for the undergrad. 00:08:10.880 |
So morning class might be too early for them to be fully awake, but there's some brand 00:08:15.440 |
new evidence that at least on creativity at work, I read a series of, I think it was three 00:08:19.920 |
studies recently showing that early birds actually did do more creative work in the 00:08:25.600 |
And in part, I think, again, I don't think any neuroscientist has touched the mechanisms 00:08:31.820 |
on this yet, but in terms of the psychological processes, early on, there seems to be a benefit 00:08:37.400 |
of the energy level, and some of that energy leads to more divergent thinking. 00:08:43.400 |
And later, if you're a morning person, you might lose the ability to diverge quite as 00:08:48.440 |
And so you end up in a more conventional space of thought. 00:08:51.240 |
Does that track at all with your understanding of how it might play out in the brain? 00:08:54.660 |
My understanding is it'd be a little bit, it would be individual, but there is something 00:08:58.960 |
to these liminal states between sleep and waking. 00:09:01.880 |
So maybe we can wrap a convenient bow around what I said and what you just said, which 00:09:07.080 |
is that we know that in the transition states into and out of sleep, and it doesn't necessarily 00:09:14.680 |
have to be within the first half hour in and out of sleep, that there seems to be more 00:09:20.000 |
divergent thinking, or at least activation of neural networks that are not as constrained 00:09:25.320 |
as one observes when they're in a sheer task and strategy implementation mode.