back to indexHow to Change Your Brain & Improve Learning | Dr. Andrew Huberman

Chapters
0:0 The Biggest Lie in the Universe
3:0 Changing the Nervous System in Adulthood
5:0 Using Attention to Change Your Brain
9:0 The Adult Brain is Very Plastic
00:00:07.200 |
is that every experience you have changes your brain. 00:00:13.140 |
"Your brain is going to be different after this lecture. 00:00:15.460 |
Your brain is going to be different after today's class 00:00:28.220 |
The nervous system changes when certain neurochemicals 00:00:31.420 |
are released and allow whatever neurons are active 00:00:35.560 |
in the period in which those chemicals are swimming around 00:00:38.980 |
to strengthen or weaken the connections of those neurons. 00:00:55.940 |
Those two individuals go by the name David Hubel 00:01:15.800 |
meaning they put the electrodes in the visual cortex, 00:01:20.840 |
and how the visual brain organizes all the features 00:01:24.340 |
as these incredible things we call visual perceptions. 00:01:35.000 |
when, for instance, a child comes into the world 00:01:38.600 |
The lens of their eye isn't clear, but it's opaque. 00:01:46.200 |
which is when the eyes either deviate outward or inward. 00:01:54.580 |
And what David and Torsten did is they figured out 00:02:05.040 |
the visual brain would completely rewire itself 00:02:10.760 |
whatever bit of visual information was coming in. 00:02:15.500 |
that kind of simulate a droopy eye or a deviating eye 00:02:20.640 |
And then what they found is that the visual brain 00:02:24.720 |
There was sort of a takeover of the visual brain 00:02:29.120 |
Many experiments in many different sensory systems 00:02:34.500 |
There are beautiful experiments, for instance, 00:02:43.380 |
if two fingers were taped together early in development, 00:02:51.640 |
so that the person couldn't actually distinguish 00:02:55.960 |
of the two fingers separately, pretty remarkable. 00:02:59.160 |
All of this is to say that David and Torsten's work, 00:03:17.200 |
the amount of activity for a given part of our body, 00:03:32.600 |
if we are to change our nervous system in adulthood, 00:03:36.000 |
we need to think about not just what we're trying to get, 00:03:47.060 |
but it actually turns out to be a great advantage. 00:03:49.560 |
One of the key experiments that David and Torsten did 00:03:53.920 |
was an experiment where they closed both eyes, 00:03:56.680 |
where they essentially removed all visual input 00:04:01.020 |
Now, this is slightly different than blindness 00:04:17.540 |
your brain is going to be completely different, 00:04:23.940 |
unless there's a selective shift in your attention 00:04:37.140 |
strengthening and weakening of particular connections. 00:04:42.000 |
which has nothing to do with emotional depression, 00:04:43.680 |
by the way, spike timing dependent plasticity. 00:04:52.280 |
please, you can go Google those and look them up. 00:04:57.480 |
I might even touch on them in some subsequent episodes. 00:05:04.560 |
we really need to bring an immense amount of attention 00:05:10.040 |
This is very much linked to the statement I made earlier 00:05:18.080 |
Well, David and Tornsten won their Nobel prize 00:05:23.040 |
because they also figured out how vision works. 00:05:25.840 |
'cause they're my scientific great-grandparents, 00:05:27.340 |
but I think everybody in the field of neuroscience agrees 00:05:36.600 |
absolutely deserved a Nobel prize for their work 00:05:43.840 |
Hubel and Wiesel did an amazing thing for science 00:05:56.820 |
that if you were to deprive the nervous system of an input, 00:06:03.220 |
and the rest of the visual cortex is taken over 00:06:07.640 |
that you could never change that unless you intervened early. 00:06:11.820 |
And this actually formed the basis for why a kid 00:06:20.960 |
why now we know that you want to get in there early 00:06:29.520 |
However, their idea that you had to do it early 00:06:34.600 |
to rescue the nervous system deficit later on, 00:06:44.580 |
by the name of Greg Reckensohn was in the laboratory 00:06:56.580 |
because the adult brain simply isn't plastic. 00:07:01.620 |
And they did a series of absolutely beautiful experiments. 00:07:21.100 |
Let's say you were a subject in one of their experiments. 00:07:23.820 |
You would come into the lab and you'd sit down at a table 00:07:26.960 |
and they would record from or image your brain 00:07:31.340 |
and look at the representation of your fingers, 00:07:43.360 |
Some of the bumps were spaced close together, 00:07:54.440 |
the bumps got closer together or further apart. 00:08:05.160 |
or anyone skilled in doing these kinds of experiments. 00:08:11.080 |
more and more attention to the distance between these bumps 00:08:14.720 |
and they would signal when there was a change 00:08:18.120 |
As they did that, there was very rapid changes, 00:08:20.800 |
plasticity in the representation of the fingers. 00:08:39.740 |
But what it told us is that these maps of touch 00:08:50.000 |
They don't have any impairments that we're aware of. 00:08:53.960 |
what it proved is that the adult brain is very plastic. 00:08:56.720 |
And they did some beautiful control experiments 00:08:58.880 |
that are important for everyone to understand, 00:09:00.920 |
which is that sometimes they would bring people in 00:09:11.320 |
or there was a shift in the pitch of that tone, 00:09:15.120 |
So the subject thought they were doing something related 00:09:17.740 |
And all that showed was that it wasn't just the mere action 00:09:23.280 |
They had to pay attention to the bumps themselves. 00:09:36.080 |
And this really spits in the face of this thing 00:09:46.080 |
The experiences that you pay super careful attention to 00:09:53.600 |
And it opens up plasticity to that specific experience.