back to indexDr. Charles Zuker: The Biology of Taste Perception & Sugar Craving | Huberman Lab Podcast #81
Chapters
0:0 Dr. Charles Zuker & Taste Perception
3:5 Momentous Supplements
4:35 Thesis, ROKA, Helix Sleep
8:35 Sensory Detection vs. Sensory Perception
11:48 Individual Variations within Perception, Color
16:20 Perceptions & Behaviors
20:19 The 5 Taste Modalities
26:18 Aversive Taste, Bitter Taste
28:0 Survival-Based & Evolutionary Reasons for Taste Modalities, Taste vs. Flavor
30:14 Additional Taste Modalities: Fat & Metallic Perception
34:2 Tongue “Taste Map,” Taste Buds & Taste Receptors
39:34 Burning Your Tongue & Perception
42:54 The “Meaning” of Taste Stimuli, Sweet vs. Bitter, Valence
51:55 Positive vs. Negative Neuronal Activation & Behavior
56:16 Acquired Tastes, Conditioned Taste Aversion
61:44 Olfaction (Smell) vs. Taste, Changing Tastes over One’s Lifetime
69:14 Integration of Odor & Taste, Influence on Behavior & Emotion
77:26 Sensitization to Taste, Internal State Modulation, Salt
84:5 Taste & Saliva: The Absence of Taste
88:10 Sugar & Reward Pleasure Centers; Gut-Brain Axis, Anticipatory Response
96:23 Vagus Nerve
103:9 Insatiable Sugar Appetite, Liking vs. Wanting, Gut-Brain Axis
112:3 Tool: Sugar vs. Artificial Sweeteners, Curbing Appetite
114:6 Cravings & Gut-Brain Axis
117:30 Nutrition, Gut-Brain Axis & Changes in Behavior
121:53 Fast vs. Slow Signaling & Reinforcement, Highly Processed Foods
130:38 Favorite Foods: Enjoyment, Sensation & Context
135:58 Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Momentous Supplements, Instagram, Twitter, Neural Network Newsletter
00:00:02.280 |
where we discuss science and science-based tools 00:00:10.320 |
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology 00:00:25.160 |
Dr. Zucker is one of the world's leading experts 00:00:28.900 |
That is how the nervous system converts physical stimuli 00:00:31.940 |
in the world into events within the nervous system 00:00:35.220 |
that we come to understand as our sense of smell, 00:00:40.520 |
our sense of touch, and our sense of hearing. 00:00:43.080 |
Dr. Zucker's lab is responsible for a tremendous amount 00:00:50.040 |
For a long time, his laboratory worked on vision, 00:00:56.520 |
that the rest of the eye and the brain can understand. 00:00:59.620 |
In recent years, his laboratory has focused mainly 00:01:08.020 |
leading to our perception of things like sweetness, 00:01:18.660 |
for doing groundbreaking work on the sense of thirst. 00:01:21.620 |
That is how the nervous system determines whether or not 00:01:28.900 |
A key feature of the work from Dr. Zucker's laboratory 00:01:34.140 |
As you'll soon learn from today's discussion, 00:01:41.380 |
not just within the brain, but a separate set of neurons 00:01:43.860 |
that sense sweetness and sugar within the body. 00:01:46.900 |
And that much of the communication between the brain 00:01:53.820 |
Dr. Zucker has received a large number of prestigious awards 00:01:59.880 |
He is a member of the National Academy of Sciences, 00:02:03.600 |
and the American Association for the Advancement of Science. 00:02:11.300 |
with the so-called HHMI, the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, 00:02:14.380 |
Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigators 00:02:16.420 |
are selected on an extremely competitive basis. 00:02:19.420 |
And indeed, they have to come back every five years 00:02:22.080 |
and prove themselves worthy of being reappointed 00:02:27.360 |
Dr. Zucker has been a Howard Hughes investigator since 1989. 00:02:35.580 |
is that you are about to learn about the nervous system 00:02:40.400 |
in particular, the perception of taste and sugar-sensing 00:02:43.280 |
from the world's expert on perception and taste. 00:02:47.000 |
I'm certain that by the end of today's podcast, 00:02:50.600 |
with a deeper understanding of our perceptions 00:02:54.860 |
but indeed, you will come away with an understanding 00:03:03.460 |
how we come to understand our life experience. 00:03:06.200 |
I'm pleased to announce that the Huberman Lab Podcast 00:03:10.380 |
We often talk about supplements on the Huberman Lab Podcast, 00:03:12.860 |
and while supplements aren't necessary for everybody, 00:03:15.080 |
many people derive tremendous benefit from them 00:03:17.220 |
for things like enhancing the quality and speed 00:03:21.560 |
or for enhancing focus, or for hormone support. 00:03:24.580 |
The reason we partnered with Momentous Supplements 00:03:32.200 |
which is important because many of our podcast listeners 00:03:36.320 |
Third, many of the supplements that Momentous makes 00:03:46.160 |
First of all, if you're going to create a supplement protocol 00:03:51.520 |
which supplement ingredients are most essential 00:03:54.640 |
And supplements that combine lots of ingredients 00:03:58.460 |
So in trying to put together a supplement protocol 00:04:00.720 |
for yourself that's the most biologically effective 00:04:03.320 |
and cost-effective, single ingredient formulations 00:04:19.080 |
I should mention that the catalog of supplements 00:04:21.520 |
that are available at livemomentous.com/huberman 00:04:25.940 |
So you can check back there livemomentous.com/huberman 00:04:32.240 |
you'll notice new supplements being added to the inventory. 00:04:34.820 |
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast 00:04:37.340 |
is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. 00:04:42.080 |
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science 00:04:44.620 |
and science related tools to the general public. 00:04:48.260 |
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. 00:04:59.360 |
of the word nootropics because the word nootropics 00:05:06.580 |
is that the brain has many different circuits that it uses 00:05:09.640 |
in order to perform things like focus or task switching 00:05:30.580 |
Thesus will allow you to try different blends 00:05:32.660 |
over the course of a month and determine which blends 00:05:35.300 |
of specific ingredients work best for you to focus 00:05:38.060 |
or for you to gain motivation and energy for workouts 00:05:49.500 |
and indeed the way that I do physical fitness. 00:05:55.220 |
you can go online to takethesus.com/huberman. 00:05:58.540 |
There, you'll just do a brief three minute quiz 00:06:00.660 |
and Thesus will send you four different formulas 00:06:10.100 |
Today's episode is also brought to us by Roca. 00:06:18.980 |
and everything about Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses 00:06:23.540 |
and with the biology of the visual system in mind. 00:06:26.220 |
I spent my lifetime working on the visual system 00:06:27.960 |
and I can tell you that your visual system has to contend 00:06:32.260 |
in order to be able to see the world around you clearly. 00:06:34.620 |
Things like adjusting for background illumination 00:06:36.680 |
so that when you go from a sunny area to a shady area, 00:06:43.460 |
when designing their eyeglasses and sunglasses. 00:06:48.680 |
For instance, because they were initially designed 00:06:50.960 |
for performance with things like running and cycling, 00:06:53.700 |
if you get sweaty, they won't fall off your face. 00:07:00.020 |
I wear Roca eyeglasses at night, so I wear readers 00:07:06.940 |
If you'd like to try Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses, 00:07:11.620 |
and enter the code Huberman to save 20% off your first order. 00:07:15.140 |
Today's episode is also brought to us by Helix Sleep. 00:07:19.340 |
that are uniquely tailored to your sleep needs. 00:07:23.900 |
that sleep is the foundation of mental health, 00:07:27.380 |
So getting adequate deep sleep is absolutely essential. 00:07:30.580 |
Now, one of the key things to getting a great night's sleep 00:07:32.440 |
is to make sure that the surface that you're sleeping on 00:07:36.060 |
Helix understands this and they've created a sleep quiz. 00:07:39.340 |
That is, you go to their website and you fill out 00:07:43.660 |
do you sleep on your side, your back, your stomach? 00:07:45.600 |
Do you tend to run hot or cold during the night? 00:07:50.360 |
By taking that quiz, they will match you to a mattress 00:07:59.180 |
and I've been sleeping on that mattress for some time now, 00:08:02.960 |
So if you're interested in upgrading your mattress 00:08:04.600 |
to one that's uniquely tailored to your sleep needs, 00:08:10.620 |
and they'll match you to a customized mattress, 00:08:12.380 |
and you'll get up to $200 off any mattress order 00:08:20.720 |
They'll even pick it up for you if you don't love it, 00:08:24.040 |
because you'll be sleeping far better than you have before. 00:08:32.980 |
And now for my discussion with Dr. Charles Zucker. 00:08:36.140 |
Charles, thank you so much for joining me today. 00:08:47.340 |
and because you've worked on a number of different topics 00:08:55.900 |
how should the world and people think about perception, 00:09:06.260 |
in terms of vision, hearing, taste, et cetera? 00:09:09.420 |
- So, you know, the brain is an extraordinary organ 00:09:40.400 |
It changes, it transforms, you know, fear into courage, 00:10:07.940 |
and this is a cord, and this is a microphone. 00:10:52.380 |
Detection is what happens when you take a sugar molecule, 00:11:16.180 |
and now detection gets transformed into perception. 00:11:20.180 |
And it's trying to understand how that happens. 00:11:35.600 |
How does the brain ultimately transform detection 00:11:38.660 |
into perception so that it can guide actions and behaviors? 00:11:46.520 |
And is a very clear and beautiful description. 00:11:49.340 |
A sort of high-level question related to that. 00:11:58.980 |
whether or not my perception of the color of your shirt 00:12:02.340 |
is the same as your perception of the color of your shirt. 00:12:14.000 |
The audience will always penalize me for interrupting you, 00:12:17.220 |
and we'll never penalize you for interrupting me. 00:12:26.680 |
that the brain is trying to represent the world 00:12:31.500 |
based in nothing but the transformation of these signals 00:12:43.740 |
It follows that your brain is different than my brain. 00:12:54.180 |
even when receiving the same sensory cues, okay? 00:13:09.500 |
So in the world of vision, as you know, well know, 00:13:15.420 |
we have three classes of photoreceptor neurons 00:13:19.320 |
that sense three basic colors, red, blue, and green. 00:13:31.460 |
And these three are sufficient to accommodate 00:13:40.660 |
and I'm gonna project with one into a white screen, 00:14:03.860 |
right next to that mixed beam, a spectrally pure yellow. 00:14:08.860 |
And I'm gonna ask you to come to the red and green projectors 00:14:16.540 |
and play with intensity knobs so that you can match 00:14:34.680 |
And then I'm gonna ask the next person to do the same. 00:14:39.820 |
And then I'm gonna ask every person around this area 00:15:02.820 |
that yellow is gonna be ever so slightly differently. 00:15:07.120 |
And so I think that simple psychological experiment 00:15:11.440 |
beautifully illustrates how we truly perceive 00:15:18.060 |
And yet in that example, we know the basic elements 00:15:23.160 |
If we migrate into a slightly different sense, 00:15:36.780 |
that will allow us to get that degree of granularity 00:15:44.120 |
Where we can show that A, produces and leads to B. 00:15:52.720 |
If I smell the same rose, I can describe it also. 00:16:03.060 |
But it's close enough that we can both pretty much say 00:16:08.060 |
that it has the following features or other determinants. 00:16:13.440 |
But no question that your experience is different than mine. 00:16:19.760 |
- The fact that it's good enough for us to both survive, 00:16:25.360 |
that your perception of yellow and my perception of yellow, 00:16:35.300 |
by a colleague of ours, Marcus Meister at Caltech. 00:16:40.800 |
but in a review that I read by Marcus at one point, 00:16:45.500 |
he said that the basic function of perception 00:17:11.920 |
- But what I like about that is that it seems like the, 00:17:16.180 |
we know the brain is a very economical organ in some sense, 00:17:25.700 |
from one individual to the next at once seems like a problem 00:17:29.920 |
because we're all literally seeing different things. 00:17:34.840 |
We function well enough for most of us to avoid death 00:17:42.560 |
and to enjoy some aspects of life, one hopes. 00:17:45.600 |
So is there a general statement that we can make 00:17:49.040 |
about the brain, not just as a organ to generate perception, 00:17:54.920 |
but also an organ that is trying to batch our behaviors 00:18:09.640 |
broadly speaking, you could categorize a lot of behaviors 00:18:24.600 |
We're, you know, the choices are not necessarily binary, 00:18:56.320 |
where we do things that we should not be doing. 00:19:38.840 |
And that goes beyond that simple set of categories, 00:20:08.960 |
the extensive variety of food, flora and fauna in New York 00:20:19.880 |
Let's talk about taste because while you've done 00:20:28.840 |
you could spend all day on, taste is fascinating. 00:20:32.440 |
First of all, I'd like to know why you migrated 00:20:39.520 |
you could highlight to us why we should think about 00:20:42.260 |
and how we should think about the sense of taste. 00:20:48.240 |
as I highlighted before, how the brain does its magic. 00:20:54.660 |
Ideally, I like to help contribute to understand all of it. 00:21:04.580 |
How do you encode and decode memories and actions? 00:21:10.860 |
How do you transform detection into perception? 00:21:17.100 |
But one of the key things in science, as you know, 00:21:23.100 |
is ensuring that you always ask the right question 00:21:27.280 |
so that you have a possibility of answering it. 00:21:35.440 |
or reduced to an experimental path that helps you resolve it 00:21:43.580 |
but not necessarily answering the important problem 00:21:54.320 |
The hardest question, the most important question is, 00:21:57.220 |
what question are you going to try and answer? 00:22:00.080 |
- And so, for example, I will have to understand 00:22:13.140 |
I mean, at the molecular level, that's what we do. 00:22:15.780 |
How do the circuits in your brain create that sense? 00:22:40.320 |
So if I want to begin to study these questions 00:22:42.680 |
about brain function that can cover so many aspects 00:22:52.920 |
But in a way that I can ask questions that give me answers. 00:23:06.200 |
of being stories, memories, of creating emotions, 00:23:11.000 |
of giving you different actions and perceptions 00:23:18.520 |
When you're hungry, things taste very differently 00:23:27.080 |
this amazing meal you had with your first date. 00:23:32.120 |
So if I want to begin to explore all of these things 00:23:40.880 |
a sensory system that affords some degree of simplicity 00:23:45.880 |
in the way that the input output relationships 00:23:53.400 |
are put together and in a way that still can be used 00:23:56.760 |
to ask every one of these problems that the brain 00:24:00.040 |
has to ultimately compute, encode, and decode. 00:24:04.060 |
And what was remarkable about the taste system 00:24:10.800 |
is that nothing was known about the molecular basis of taste. 00:24:17.260 |
We knew that we could taste what has been usually defined 00:24:30.840 |
Umami is a Japanese word that means yummy, delicious. 00:24:42.160 |
And in humans, it's mostly associated with the taste of MSG, 00:24:47.160 |
monosodium glutamate, one amino acid in particular. 00:24:52.600 |
some foods that are rich in the umami-evoking stimulation? 00:25:11.880 |
is that the lines of input are limited to five. 00:25:16.040 |
You know, sweet, sour, bitter, salty, and umami. 00:25:20.160 |
And each of them has a predetermined meaning. 00:25:24.560 |
You are born liking sugar and disliking bitter. 00:25:34.020 |
But of course, you can learn to dislike sugar 00:25:41.720 |
But in the wild, let's take humans out of the equation, eh? 00:26:08.940 |
And bitter and sour are innately predetermined 00:26:30.540 |
can we assume that the sensation of very bitter, 00:26:35.300 |
or of activation of bitter receptors in the mouth 00:26:37.860 |
activates a neural circuit that causes closing of the mouth, 00:26:43.300 |
retraction of the tongue, and retraction of the body, 00:26:53.260 |
The activation of the receptors in the tongue 00:26:56.820 |
that recognize sweet versus the ones that recognize bitter 00:27:04.280 |
And that program that we can refer as appetitiveness, 00:27:10.320 |
or aversion, it's composed of many different subroutines. 00:27:15.320 |
In the case of bitter, it's very easy to actually look at, 00:27:22.960 |
because the first thing you do is you stop licking, 00:27:25.680 |
then you put a unhappy face, then you squint your eyes, 00:27:58.480 |
Now, let me say that this palette of five basic tastes 00:28:02.560 |
accommodates all the dietary needs of the organism. 00:28:05.980 |
Sweet to ensure that we get the right amount of energy. 00:28:21.980 |
Bitter to prevent the ingestion of toxic, nauseous chemicals. 00:28:28.120 |
Nearly all bitter tasting things out in the wild 00:28:34.220 |
And sour, most likely to prevent the ingestion 00:29:02.180 |
with texture, with temperature, with the look of it 00:29:15.660 |
into its basic elements so we can begin to break it apart 00:29:25.140 |
and we try to figure out how these lines of information 00:29:30.880 |
and how they signal and how they get integrated 00:29:33.600 |
and how they trigger all these different behaviors, 00:29:39.480 |
So we give the animal sweet or we give them a bitter, 00:29:43.260 |
We avoid mixes because the first stage of discovery 00:29:48.260 |
is to have that clarity as to what you're trying to extract 00:29:56.120 |
so that you can hopefully, meaningfully make a difference 00:30:08.600 |
to create the full array of the color spectrum. 00:30:12.780 |
- Before I ask you about the first and second 00:30:15.380 |
and third stages of taste and flavor perception, 00:30:18.660 |
is there any idea that there may be more than five? 00:30:32.740 |
Like in South America, when I visited Buenos Aires, 00:30:36.240 |
I found that at the end of a meal, they would take a steak, 00:30:38.760 |
the trimming off the edge of the steak, burn it slightly, 00:30:55.380 |
And we like to do things that we should not be doing 00:30:59.680 |
But on the other hand, look at those muscles. 00:31:08.800 |
The listeners of this podcast will immediately, 00:31:30.040 |
because it's the other side of the taste system. 00:31:32.620 |
And so, so missing tastes, you know, one is fat. 00:31:46.520 |
is really the feeling of fat rolling on your tongue. 00:32:04.580 |
It's somatosensory cells, cells that are not responding 00:32:09.480 |
to taste, but they're responding to mechanical stimulation 00:32:20.380 |
- I love the idea that there is a perception of fat 00:32:23.060 |
regardless of whether or not there's a dedicated receptor 00:32:26.180 |
for fat, mostly because it's evoking sensations 00:32:30.660 |
and imagery of the taste of slightly burnt fat. 00:32:39.220 |
You know, I know exactly what it tastes like. 00:32:46.580 |
You know, what are the palettes of that color 00:33:04.160 |
And is there really, you know, a receptor for metallic taste 00:33:18.340 |
just separate lines by the keys of a piano, yeah? 00:33:26.580 |
And that one chord in the case of a piano leads to a note, 00:33:34.680 |
But you play many of them together and something emerges 00:33:38.880 |
that it's different than any one of the pieces. 00:33:43.840 |
And it's possible that metallic, for example, 00:33:50.960 |
just in the right ratio of these added lines. 00:33:59.160 |
your example of the piano provides a perfect segue 00:34:04.320 |
which is if you would describe the sequence of neural events 00:34:19.100 |
of whether or not we indeed have different taste receptors 00:34:22.600 |
distributed in different locations on our tongue 00:34:27.520 |
- Yes, so let's start by debunking that old tale and myth. 00:34:38.820 |
- There are many views, but the most prevalent 00:34:45.920 |
describing the sensitivity of the tongue to different tastes. 00:34:57.760 |
And I'm gonna dip that Q-tip in salt and in quinine 00:35:02.400 |
as something bitter and glucose as something sweet. 00:35:06.640 |
And I'm gonna take that Q-tip, ask you to stick your tongue 00:35:15.480 |
And then I'm going to change the concentration 00:35:20.480 |
of the amount of salt or the amount of bitter 00:35:24.600 |
and ask, can I get some sort of a map of sensitivity 00:35:38.060 |
was simply mistranslated as it was being drawn. 00:35:41.700 |
And of course that led to an entire industry. 00:35:46.600 |
This is the way you maximize your wine experience 00:35:55.320 |
that you're gonna drink from so that it acts maximally 00:36:07.220 |
We have taste buds distributed in various parts of the tongue 00:36:12.320 |
so there is a map on the distribution of taste buds. 00:36:17.320 |
But each taste bud has around 100 taste receptor cells. 00:36:22.300 |
And those taste receptor cells can be of five types, 00:36:36.040 |
have the representation of all five taste qualities. 00:36:41.140 |
Now, there's no question that there is a slight bias 00:37:00.000 |
And so, let's make sure that the very back of your tongue 00:37:13.960 |
and get rid of this that otherwise may kill you, okay? 00:37:19.800 |
- But the notion that all sweet is in the front 00:37:33.640 |
first of all, thank you for dispelling that myth. 00:37:40.240 |
'cause I think that's the number one myth related to taste. 00:37:44.640 |
are there taste receptors anywhere else in the mouth? 00:37:47.760 |
For instance, on the lips. - Yeah, the palate. 00:37:54.360 |
of the oral cavity, the tongue and the palate. 00:37:58.600 |
And the palate is very rich in sweet receptors. 00:38:12.520 |
after the receptors for these five, the detectors, 00:38:17.840 |
the molecules that sends sweet, sour, beet, and salt to mommy, 00:38:37.880 |
that says there is sweet here or there is salt here. 00:38:49.600 |
sweet, beet, salt, to mommy, and most recently sour, 00:38:57.240 |
to really map where are they found in the tongue 00:39:05.160 |
and trying to figure out what you're feeling, 00:39:18.600 |
that says the sweet receptors are found here. 00:39:30.680 |
has receptors for all of the basic taste classes. 00:39:39.680 |
in terms of thinking about how the brain computes 00:39:43.120 |
and codes and decodes this thing we call taste. 00:39:50.040 |
before we get back into the biological circuit, 00:39:56.160 |
of drinking a sip of tea or coffee that is too hot 00:39:59.320 |
and burning my tongue is the way I would describe it. 00:40:13.560 |
have I destroyed taste receptors that regenerate 00:40:22.560 |
so that I no longer taste the way I did before? 00:40:46.040 |
to everything you could range from the nicest 00:40:56.600 |
that these cells are always coming back in a way 00:40:59.600 |
that I can re-experience the world in the right way. 00:41:12.620 |
Again, they're receiving everything that you ingest, 00:41:19.760 |
from the spiciest to the most horrible tasting, 00:41:25.820 |
And again, those intestinal cells whose role is 00:41:39.940 |
Olfactory neurons in your nose is the other example. 00:41:47.960 |
So then A, yes, you're burning a lot of your cells 00:41:54.680 |
The good news is that they're gonna come back. 00:41:57.480 |
But we know that when you burn yourself with tea, 00:41:59.660 |
they come back within 20 minutes, 30 minutes, an hour. 00:42:04.660 |
And these cells are not renewing in that timeframe. 00:42:21.240 |
you're damaging them in a way that they can recover. 00:42:27.780 |
and you also damage your somatosensory cells. 00:42:30.380 |
These are the cells that feel things, not taste things. 00:42:35.300 |
And then, you know, you wait half an hour or so, 00:42:39.840 |
and then, my goodness, thank God, it's back to normal. 00:42:44.480 |
- And most of the time, I don't even notice the transition, 00:42:49.140 |
And later, I'll ask you about the relationship 00:43:06.600 |
but at the same time, perhaps more informative. 00:43:11.440 |
- Let's compare and contrast sweet and bitter 00:43:15.080 |
as we follow their lines from the tongue to the brain. 00:43:25.120 |
If we have to come up with two sensory experiences 00:43:28.320 |
that represent polar opposites, it will be sweet and bitter. 00:43:31.320 |
There are not two colors that represent polar opposites 00:43:33.720 |
because, you know, you could say black and white, 00:43:44.900 |
- Even the political parties have some over them. 00:43:58.580 |
Again, I'm a reductionist, so I'm reducing it in a way 00:44:18.680 |
means the value of that experience, all right? 00:44:35.080 |
an identity, and that's what you and I will refer to 00:44:47.580 |
which makes it incredibly attractive and appetitive. 00:45:00.760 |
In fact, we have been able to engineer animals 00:45:04.280 |
where we completely remove the valence from the stimuli. 00:45:10.800 |
can recognize it as sweet, but it's no longer attractive. 00:45:19.520 |
And that's because the identity and the valence 00:45:24.360 |
are encoded in two separate parts of the brain. 00:45:27.180 |
In the case of bitter, again, it has, on the one hand, 00:45:35.480 |
And you know exactly what bitter tastes like. 00:45:38.840 |
- I can taste it now, even as you describe it. 00:45:41.420 |
- But it also has a valence, and that's a negative valence, 00:45:51.760 |
- And it comes to mind, I remember telling some kids recently 00:45:54.080 |
that we're gonna go get ice cream, and it was interesting. 00:45:55.920 |
They looked up and they started smacking their lip, 00:46:03.860 |
When we talk about the gut brain, maybe we'll get there. 00:46:07.300 |
So then the signals, if we follow now these two lines, 00:46:15.340 |
And you press one key and you activate this cord. 00:46:21.180 |
So you activate the sweet cells throughout your oral cavity, 00:46:25.100 |
and they all converge into a group of sweet neurons. 00:46:29.260 |
In the next station, which is still outside the brain, 00:46:37.080 |
These are the neurons that innervate your tongue 00:46:48.060 |
And there are two main ganglia that innervate 00:46:54.100 |
the vast majority of all taste buds in the oral cavity. 00:47:04.760 |
The brainstem is the entry of the body into the brain. 00:47:08.880 |
And there are different areas of the brainstem, 00:47:12.920 |
and there are different groups of neurons in the brainstem, 00:47:39.000 |
And from there, the sweet signal goes to this other area, 00:47:45.560 |
And then it goes through a number of stations 00:47:51.000 |
where that sweet signal goes from sweet neuron 00:48:03.140 |
that's where meaning is imposed into that signal. 00:48:08.140 |
It's then, and only then, this is what the data suggests, 00:48:13.560 |
that now you can identify this as a sweet stimuli. 00:48:22.680 |
- You know, the timescale of the nervous system, 00:48:36.440 |
Maybe with respect to people and my own poor judgment, 00:48:46.800 |
at each of these stations conceptually, yeah? 00:48:57.360 |
You deliver the stimuli and within a fraction of a second, 00:49:01.460 |
you see now the response in these following stations. 00:49:08.060 |
And now in there, you impose meaning to that taste. 00:49:14.900 |
that represents the taste of sweet in taste cortex 00:49:19.900 |
and a different area that represents the taste of bitter. 00:49:31.340 |
Now we're gonna do a thought experiment, all right? 00:49:41.920 |
and this added different group of neurons in your brain 00:49:46.020 |
represents the taste, the perception of bitter, 00:49:55.780 |
First, I should be able to go into your brain, 00:50:03.020 |
find a way to prevent them from being activated 00:50:09.900 |
and you'll never know that you're tasting sweet. 00:50:12.380 |
And conversely, I should be able to go into your brain, 00:50:39.220 |
- But presumably in humans, it would work similar. 00:50:50.080 |
The first to the predetermined nature of the sense of taste 00:50:55.560 |
because it means I can go to these parts of your brain 00:51:01.200 |
and have you throw the full behavioral experience. 00:51:08.420 |
these bitter neurons, the animal can start gagging 00:51:16.580 |
but the animal thinks that it's getting a bit of stimuli. 00:51:25.140 |
And so, and the second, just to finish the line 00:51:27.560 |
so that it doesn't sound like it teaches two things 00:51:31.960 |
is that it substantiates this capacity of the brain 00:51:40.760 |
to segregate, to separate in these nodes of action, 00:51:45.800 |
the representation of these two diametrically opposed 00:51:50.240 |
percepts, which is sweet, for example, versus bitter. 00:51:55.000 |
The reason I say amazing and that is also amazing 00:51:59.440 |
You told us earlier and you're absolutely correct, 00:52:05.880 |
whether or not it's one group of neurons over here 00:52:11.900 |
electrical activity is the generic common language 00:52:20.320 |
of whether or not those separate sets of neurons 00:52:28.200 |
or whether or not they're simply created, connected, 00:52:30.840 |
excuse me, to sets of neurons that evoke distinct behaviors 00:52:34.440 |
of moving towards and inhaling more and licking or aversive. 00:52:40.400 |
and our micro responses or are micro responses 00:52:44.080 |
and our behaviors the consequence of the percept? 00:52:48.540 |
So first the answer is they go into an area of the brain 00:52:53.540 |
where valence is imposed and that area is known 00:52:58.540 |
as the amygdala and the sweet neurons go to a different area 00:53:10.500 |
because I think your audience might appreciate this. 00:53:22.060 |
and so that's expected because now it's, you know, 00:53:28.700 |
Now, this is Moses transforming water into wine. 00:53:32.120 |
In this case, we're gonna, and today is Passover 00:53:34.300 |
so then it's an appropriate, you know, example. 00:53:42.420 |
But how do I know, how do I know that activating them 00:53:47.520 |
is evoking a positive feeling inside, a goodness, 00:53:52.160 |
a satisfaction, I love it versus I'm just increasing licking 00:53:57.160 |
which is the other option because all we're seeing 00:54:05.680 |
that he's feeling something really good versus you know what? 00:54:08.960 |
That piano line is going back straight into the tongue 00:54:11.720 |
and all he's doing is forcing it to move faster. 00:54:15.720 |
Well, we can actually separate this by doing experiments 00:54:19.640 |
that allow us to fundamentally distinguish them. 00:54:26.080 |
I'm gonna take the animal and I'm gonna put them 00:54:31.520 |
and the two sides have features that make them different. 00:54:35.520 |
One has yellow little toys, the other one has green toys. 00:54:48.400 |
I take the mouse, put them inside this arena, 00:54:54.880 |
and poach around both sides with equal frequency. 00:54:58.860 |
And now what I'm going to do is I'm gonna activate 00:55:05.000 |
every time the animal is on the side with the yellow stripes 00:55:15.160 |
and if that is creating a positive internal state, 00:55:24.040 |
It will want to stay on the side with the yellow stripes. 00:55:33.320 |
every time I'm activating these neurons, okay? 00:55:47.520 |
that the activation of a group of neurons in the brain 00:56:03.440 |
the animal will actively want now to stay away from the side 00:56:18.440 |
with the experience of going to a restaurant, 00:56:23.040 |
and then fortunately it doesn't happen that often 00:56:29.540 |
that this is one strong example of one trial learning 00:56:34.540 |
that from that point on, it's not the restaurant 00:56:42.480 |
but it's my notion of it had to have been the shrimp 00:56:47.240 |
that leads me to then want to avoid shrimp in every context, 00:56:54.400 |
I can imagine all the evolutionarily adaptive reasons 00:57:00.440 |
Do we have any concept of where in this pathway that exists? 00:57:03.720 |
- We know actually a significant amount at a general level. 00:57:36.880 |
- It's like uni by the way, texture is hard to get over 00:57:48.720 |
is kind of the famous little-- - Oh my goodness. 00:57:50.280 |
- And they have amazing uni and I've tried it twice 00:57:55.560 |
Somehow the texture outweighs any kind of the deliciousness 00:57:59.960 |
- It's a very acquired taste, it's like beer. 00:58:05.760 |
That's where the accent comes from in case anyone wonder. 00:58:08.560 |
And by the time I came here to graduate school, 00:58:12.280 |
I was 19, too old to overcome my heavy Chilean accent. 00:58:17.280 |
So here I am 40 years, 50 years late, not quite, 40 plus. 00:58:24.880 |
- And I still sound like I just came off the boat. 00:58:28.080 |
So in Chile, you don't drink beer when you're young. 00:58:31.640 |
You drink wine, Chile is a huge wine producer. 00:58:34.560 |
So when I came to the US, all of my classmates 00:58:40.800 |
were drinking beer because they had finished college 00:58:45.040 |
where they were all beer drinking and graduate school, 00:58:52.840 |
The way they relax, let's go and have some beers. 00:58:57.880 |
And we were being clearly underpaid, may I add. 00:59:12.600 |
And if you take all the beer I've drunk in my entire life, 00:59:16.120 |
I would say they add to less than an eight ounce 00:59:30.640 |
This is the connection to uni and to oysters. 00:59:36.320 |
You know, this is the great thing about our brains. 00:59:38.840 |
Certain things we need to repeat a hundred times 00:59:47.160 |
And then she'll give it to you over the phone, 01:00:08.480 |
We park our car and if we're lucky by the end of the day, 01:00:16.680 |
We remember the birthdays of every one of our children 01:00:22.640 |
Well, there are events that a single event is so traumatic 01:00:38.280 |
And there is a phenomenon known as condition taste aversion. 01:00:43.360 |
You can pair an attractive stimuli with a really bad one. 01:00:52.560 |
And you can make an animal begin to vehemently 01:00:58.440 |
And that's because you've conditioned the animals 01:01:02.960 |
to now be averse to this otherwise nice taste 01:01:11.640 |
And when you do that, now you could begin to ask, 01:01:13.920 |
why does change in the signal as it travels from the tongue 01:01:19.440 |
to the brain in a normal animal versus an animal 01:01:31.520 |
how the brain changes the nature, the quality, 01:01:36.440 |
the meaning of a stimuli as a function of its state. 01:01:41.440 |
- I have a number of questions related to that, 01:01:54.380 |
is distinct from taste because flavor involves smell, 01:01:56.760 |
texture, temperature, and some other features. 01:02:17.520 |
or rather to insert a perception into context is so powerful. 01:02:22.520 |
And there's an element of kind of mystery about it, 01:02:27.240 |
but if we start to think about some of the more nuance 01:02:30.760 |
that we like to live at the edge, as you say, 01:02:39.320 |
to go back to your analogy earlier, the color dial, 01:02:45.920 |
So for instance, I don't think I like bitter taste, 01:02:56.660 |
How much plasticity do you think there is there, 01:03:01.000 |
Because I think one of the most salient examples of this 01:03:03.280 |
is that kids don't seem to like certain vegetables, 01:03:07.520 |
but they all are hardwired to like sweet tastes. 01:03:11.200 |
And yet you could also imagine that one of the reasons 01:03:13.760 |
why they may eventually grow to incorporate vegetables 01:03:25.720 |
the distribution, the number, the sensitivity, et cetera, 01:03:37.480 |
- I'm going to take the question slightly differently, 01:03:46.840 |
between the olfactory system and the taste system 01:04:05.800 |
it's claimed that we can smell millions of different others. 01:04:14.040 |
none of them have an innate predetermined meaning. 01:04:21.920 |
meaning is imposed by learning and experience, 01:04:27.440 |
So I'm gonna give you, I'm gonna make it differently. 01:04:30.600 |
There are a handful of the millions of others 01:04:34.360 |
that were claimed that you could immediately tell me 01:04:42.800 |
- So vomit, it's not correct because I can assure you 01:04:50.440 |
where things which are far less appealing than vomit 01:05:14.200 |
And that's why you like broccoli and I despise broccoli 01:05:18.460 |
because I remember my mother forcing me to eat broccoli. 01:05:32.000 |
you have neurons at every station that are for sweet, 01:05:57.300 |
evolve a system where you put together a pathway 01:06:05.800 |
where you have the capacity to associate every other 01:06:11.440 |
in a specific context that now gives it the meaning. 01:06:17.340 |
Now, let's go back to the original question then. 01:06:28.160 |
because it's fundamental basis and neural organization. 01:06:39.180 |
that it's not modulated by learning or experience. 01:06:59.980 |
It's because it has an associated gain to the system. 01:07:19.200 |
And in the case of beer, of course, is alcohol. 01:07:48.940 |
it's restricted in what you could do with it, 01:07:59.540 |
The goal of the olfactory system is very different. 01:08:12.540 |
to identify ecological niches they wanna be in. 01:08:41.400 |
I'm being overly simplistic and reductionist, 01:08:50.580 |
- I don't think you're being overly simplistic. 01:08:52.960 |
I think it illustrates the key intractable nature 01:08:56.760 |
of this system and the way you've approached it, 01:08:59.600 |
and I think it's important for people to hear that, 01:09:04.020 |
is mystified with empathy and love, et cetera. 01:09:09.280 |
I'm gonna ask a sort of a high-level question 01:09:34.980 |
for lack of a better word, between two people, 01:09:42.660 |
- I didn't know this was that kind of a podcast. 01:09:53.600 |
but we were commenting not about our own experience 01:09:58.640 |
but of someone that she was now very excited about. 01:10:15.120 |
And she said, "I have a feeling something about it 01:10:17.760 |
is in smell and something about it is actually in taste." 01:10:27.440 |
And I thought that it was a very interesting example 01:10:33.580 |
because it gets to the merging of odor and taste, 01:10:39.800 |
of course, the context of a new relationship, 01:10:46.680 |
but I've had the experience of the odor of somebody's breath 01:11:01.160 |
- But that's because you associate it with other others 01:11:06.160 |
that trigger that negative aversive reaction, by the way. 01:11:16.920 |
There are certain perfumes to me that are aversive. 01:11:28.240 |
So I've experienced both sides of this equation myself, 01:11:37.160 |
where people inhale it and then they drink it, 01:11:43.160 |
that certain things and people smell delicious. 01:11:47.480 |
Even mothers describing the smell of their baby's head. 01:11:51.920 |
I mean, our own babies when they're in their necks, 01:12:01.540 |
so I know exactly what Rio, that's his name, smells like. 01:12:07.360 |
It's always more fun to think about the beautiful, 01:12:11.520 |
The smell of the back of your grandson's neck. 01:12:19.120 |
So what is going on in terms of the combination 01:12:24.600 |
given that these two systems are so different? 01:12:35.700 |
where they come together to integrate the two 01:12:39.960 |
into what we would call that sensory experience. 01:12:45.200 |
And I'll tell you an experiment that you could do 01:12:53.940 |
to get a sense of how we approach these problems 01:12:56.280 |
so that we can get meaningful scientific answers. 01:12:59.080 |
So we know where the olfactory cortex is in the brain. 01:13:05.080 |
We know where the taste cortex is in the brain. 01:13:32.220 |
And then we reason that if odor and taste come together 01:13:51.100 |
that maybe this is the area in the brain of the mouse, 01:14:08.020 |
And if this is true, we could do the following experiment. 01:14:38.100 |
then they're incorrect and they get no reward. 01:15:14.360 |
And after you train them, this mice with 90% accuracy 01:15:18.500 |
will tell you when you randomize now the stimuli, 01:15:43.380 |
If you get taste alone, go to this other part 01:15:48.480 |
And if you get the two together, do this something else. 01:15:57.700 |
when it's sensing taste alone, odor alone or the mix. 01:16:09.340 |
and go to this area that we now uncover, discover 01:16:13.780 |
as being the site of multi-sensor integration 01:16:21.140 |
Prevent it from being activated experimentally. 01:16:31.600 |
the animal should still be able to recognize the taste alone. 01:16:35.140 |
They still should be able to recognize the odor alone, 01:16:38.180 |
but should be incapable now to recognize the mix. 01:16:42.980 |
And exactly as predicted, that's exactly what you get. 01:16:50.220 |
- The brain is basically a series of engineered circuits. 01:16:56.000 |
And our task is to figure out how can we extract 01:17:12.980 |
- And why certain people's breath tastes so good 01:17:31.100 |
to the visual system or differences to the visual system. 01:17:34.100 |
The visual system, there are a couple of phenomenon 01:17:36.260 |
that I wonder if they also exist in the taste system. 01:17:44.580 |
and activate the given receptors long enough, 01:17:49.340 |
We offset this with little micro eye movements, et cetera, 01:17:55.780 |
Everyone seems to call it something different, 01:18:13.320 |
With olfaction, I'm familiar with the odor in a room 01:18:16.900 |
I don't like or I like, and then it disappearing. 01:18:33.840 |
are interested in eating not more sugar, but less sugar. 01:18:37.740 |
- I think we have better ways to approach that, 01:18:39.680 |
and we can transition from taste into these other circuits 01:18:44.520 |
that makes sugar so extraordinarily impossible 01:19:03.420 |
And it's, I think, happening at multiple stations. 01:19:15.540 |
i.e. the cells in your tongue that are sensing that sugar. 01:19:23.300 |
and it's triggering activity after activity after activity, 01:19:29.780 |
Again, I'm using terms which are extraordinarily loose. 01:19:33.720 |
- But for sake of this discussion, it's fine. 01:19:41.680 |
where it undergoes a set of changes, chemical changes, 01:19:50.620 |
or it even gets removed from the surface of the cell. 01:19:54.660 |
And now what will happen is that the same amount of sugar 01:20:14.620 |
that happens by continuous activation of the circuit 01:20:20.860 |
You know, there is from the tongue to the ganglia, 01:20:23.720 |
from the ganglia to the first station in the brainstem, 01:20:32.220 |
So there are multiple steps that this signal is traveling. 01:20:35.140 |
Now, you might say, why, this is a label line, 01:20:41.020 |
And that's because the taste system is so important 01:20:43.780 |
to ensure that you get what you need to survive, 01:21:00.860 |
not necessarily to change the way that something tastes, 01:21:18.100 |
Salt is very appetitive at low concentrations, 01:21:36.900 |
that you can transfer these electrical signals 01:21:44.380 |
But at high concentrations, let's say ocean water, 01:21:50.800 |
And we all know this because we've gone to the ocean 01:21:52.720 |
and then when you get it in your mouth, it's not that great. 01:22:00.200 |
and we can do this in experimental models quite readily, 01:22:05.160 |
now this incredibly high concentration of salt, 01:22:10.240 |
one molar sodium chloride becomes amazingly appetitive 01:22:22.480 |
but your brain is telling you, I don't care, you need it. 01:22:27.380 |
And this is what we call the modulation of the taste system 01:22:42.640 |
on the money, no, no, this is exactly correct. 01:22:45.980 |
Or if you're thirsty and hungry, you suppress hunger 01:22:50.980 |
so that you don't waste water molecules in digesting food. 01:22:58.240 |
Because if you're thirsty and you have no water, 01:23:03.260 |
But you can go on a hunger strike as long as you have water 01:23:16.040 |
So you could see that there are multiple layers at which 01:23:21.000 |
the taste system that guides our drive and our motivation 01:23:27.320 |
to consume the nutrients we need has to be modulated 01:23:35.780 |
And of course, internal state itself has to be modulated 01:23:53.800 |
Because you have to ensure that at each step, 01:23:57.560 |
you give the system that level of flexibility 01:24:07.700 |
- I have a question that has just been on my mind 01:24:10.920 |
for a bit now, because I was drinking this water 01:24:18.300 |
- Is there any kind of signal for the absence of taste 01:24:23.520 |
And here is why I ask, what I'm thinking about is saliva. 01:24:33.060 |
that does change how I experience more bland foods. 01:24:53.640 |
Because in the visual system there is, right? 01:24:56.220 |
You close the eyes and you start getting increases 01:24:58.520 |
in activity in the visual system as opposed to decreases, 01:25:02.580 |
But there are reasons for that because everything 01:25:04.720 |
is about signal to noise, signal to background. 01:25:09.160 |
I can tell you that most of our work is trying to focus 01:25:11.880 |
on how the taste system works, not how it doesn't work. 01:25:27.280 |
- All right, listen, I was weaned in this system of, 01:25:34.800 |
with a very prominent podcast at Lex Friedman Podcast. 01:25:41.560 |
I won't say whether or not we are sitting in the presence 01:25:46.240 |
but in any event, I know the sorts of ribbing 01:25:51.160 |
For those listening, this is the kind of hazing that happens, 01:26:02.160 |
- And it's always about the science in the end. 01:26:29.400 |
that I'd love to see someone in your class do. 01:26:33.560 |
- Yes, no, no, no, but that we can figure it out. 01:26:38.800 |
the saliva in a fed state is distinct from the saliva 01:27:01.660 |
- No, no, we, I don't mean that you go to Walgreens 01:27:12.040 |
And you can take, you know, taste cells in culture 01:27:23.340 |
And what happens is that the system is being engineered 01:27:35.520 |
And there is no difference on the state of the animal. 01:27:40.380 |
- Great, well, this is the reason to do experiments. 01:27:48.160 |
- You know that curiosity kills the cut, yeah? 01:28:01.740 |
Okay, so if it's not saliva, and apparently it is not, 01:28:09.700 |
And what aspects of the internal milieu are relevant? 01:28:14.000 |
Because there's autonomic, there's asleep and awake, 01:28:17.140 |
One of the questions that I got from hundreds of people 01:28:20.260 |
when I solicited questions in advance of this episode was, 01:28:22.980 |
why do I crave sugar when I'm stressed, for instance? 01:28:33.200 |
It activates what I'm going to generically refer to 01:28:43.180 |
in a way that dramatically changes our internal state. 01:28:48.180 |
This is, you know, why do we eat a gallon of ice cream 01:28:55.100 |
to go into this entirely different world, yeah? 01:28:59.800 |
Of the body telling your brain what you need, 01:29:04.800 |
in important things like sugar and fat, yeah? 01:29:44.140 |
there are active research programs, and beautiful work. 01:30:04.740 |
because of the quote-unquote gut feeling aspect, 01:30:12.740 |
that drive or change our perceptions and behaviors 01:30:35.960 |
This is the only way that the brain can ensure 01:30:39.300 |
that every one of those organs are working together 01:30:55.320 |
but I think what hadn't been fully appreciated, 01:31:03.080 |
but is now modulating back what the body needs to do. 01:31:09.080 |
And that includes all the way from monitoring 01:31:21.400 |
to what happens when you ingest sugar and fat. 01:31:37.780 |
what we would refer to contextual associations 01:32:25.940 |
And we will call that, neurologically speaking, 01:32:36.440 |
Neurons in the brain that form that association 01:32:42.840 |
and they're sending a signal to motor neurons 01:32:46.760 |
to go into your salivary glands to squeeze them 01:32:50.040 |
so you release saliva because you know food is coming. 01:32:58.040 |
is that those animals are also releasing insulin 01:33:08.540 |
This illustrates one part of this two-way highway, 01:33:20.560 |
that no food is coming and send a signal somehow 01:33:31.560 |
All right, this goes back to the magic of the brain. 01:33:35.460 |
It's a never-ending source of both joy and intrigue. 01:34:04.960 |
and with great force over the last five years. 01:34:19.060 |
has been what we now refer to as the gut-brain axis, 01:34:23.680 |
and the highway is a specific bundle of nerves, 01:34:27.780 |
you know, which emerge from the vagal ganglia, 01:34:31.780 |
the nodal ganglia, and so it's the vagus nerve 01:34:46.880 |
and now the brain going back down and saying, 01:35:09.180 |
that diseases that we have normally associated 01:35:12.480 |
with metabolism, physiology, and even immunity 01:35:25.940 |
I don't think obesity is a disease of metabolism. 01:35:31.460 |
I believe obesity is a disease of brain circuits. 01:35:44.220 |
because, you know, the molecules that we're dealing with 01:35:50.580 |
led us to view, of course, these issues and problems 01:35:54.820 |
as being one of metabolism, physiology, and so forth. 01:35:58.500 |
They remain to be the carriers of the ultimate signal, 01:36:03.180 |
but the brain ultimately appears to be the conductor 01:36:07.220 |
of this orchestra of physiology and metabolism. 01:36:11.380 |
All right, now let's go to the gut, brain, and sugar. 01:36:19.780 |
No, I mean, the vagus nerve has, in popular culture, 01:36:26.440 |
has been kind of converted into this single meaning 01:36:35.100 |
I actually have to tip my hat to the yogic community, 01:36:38.560 |
was among the first to talk about vagus on and on and on. 01:36:47.260 |
so-called parasympathetic pathways within the vagus, 01:36:49.880 |
but I think that the more we learn about the vagus, 01:36:52.740 |
the more it seems like an entire set of neural connections 01:36:59.680 |
because I think a lot of people have heard about the vagus. 01:37:02.000 |
It turns out experimentally in the laboratory, 01:37:04.520 |
many neuroscientists will stimulate the vagus 01:37:10.020 |
when animals or even people, believe it or not, 01:37:19.580 |
that it's just this way of calming oneself down. 01:37:26.680 |
So the vagus nerve is made out of many thousands of fibers, 01:37:31.680 |
you know, individual fibers that make this gigantic bundle. 01:37:45.480 |
Not necessarily one by one, maybe five fibers, 01:38:08.880 |
This is telling your brain about its nutritional state. 01:38:17.440 |
And they are, again, to make the same simple example, 01:38:30.320 |
showing that activating the entire vagal bundle 01:38:36.960 |
has very meaningful effects in a wide range of conditions. 01:38:42.920 |
In fact, it's being used to treat untractable depression. 01:39:18.680 |
Yet 10,000 volts of 1,000 watts each have just come on. 01:39:35.200 |
So here you activate the bundle of thousands of fibers. 01:39:43.820 |
to make a meaningful difference in depression 01:39:48.040 |
or to make a meaningful difference in epileptic. 01:40:06.240 |
that among all the things that are being done, 01:40:20.440 |
is the fact that you have these thousands of fibers 01:40:27.260 |
And our goal, and along with many other great scientists, 01:40:37.280 |
including Steve Liverless, that started a lot of, 01:40:43.080 |
on this vagal gut-brain communication line at Harvard, 01:40:48.080 |
is trying to uncover what are each of those lines doing? 01:40:54.520 |
What are each of those keys of this piano playing? 01:41:00.160 |
Just as a brief update, I know Stephen Lee release, 01:41:03.020 |
I think I was there when he got his Howard Hughes 01:41:07.740 |
Always great to get beat by excellent people. 01:41:11.380 |
because that way you can focus on this amazing podcast. 01:41:21.120 |
It's not good to get beat out by excellent people. 01:41:28.960 |
the molecular constituents of different elements 01:41:43.040 |
some that might be involved in a gastric movement. 01:41:52.400 |
and you feel full in part because your gut gets distended, 01:41:58.680 |
And then there are little sensors that are reading that 01:42:08.500 |
on the basis of the liberalese and other work. 01:42:18.820 |
put together this holistic view of how the brain, 01:42:25.900 |
it's truly changing body physiology, metabolism, and immunity. 01:42:39.000 |
but it's an exciting, thrilling journey of discovery 01:42:44.000 |
is how the signal comes back to now change that biology. 01:42:50.080 |
You know, the example I gave you before with Pavlov's dog. 01:42:56.900 |
how the association created this link between the bell, 01:43:00.680 |
but then how does the brain tell the pancreas 01:43:15.760 |
and our insatiable appetite for sugar and fat. 01:43:29.440 |
And this is a story about the fundamental difference 01:43:38.920 |
Liking sugar is the function of the taste system. 01:43:46.080 |
And it's not really liking sugar, it's liking sweet. 01:43:55.680 |
Wanting sugar, our never-ending appetite for sugar 01:44:00.680 |
is the story of the gut-brain axis, liking versus wanting. 01:44:19.020 |
And you can now engineer mice that lack these receptors. 01:44:26.000 |
So in essence, these animals will be unable to taste sweet. 01:44:35.680 |
And if you give a normal mouse a bottle containing sweet, 01:44:42.160 |
and we're gonna put either sugar or an artificial sweetener. 01:44:50.840 |
but that's simply because artificial sweeteners 01:44:57.600 |
But as far as the sweet receptor is concerned, 01:45:07.260 |
of a bottle containing sugar or a sweetener versus water, 01:45:19.120 |
Animal goes, samples each one, leaks a couple of leaks, 01:45:24.120 |
because it's repetitive and because I love it. 01:45:27.320 |
- So it prefers sugar to artificial sweetener? 01:45:32.920 |
this experiment, I'm gonna put only sweet in one bottle. 01:45:36.760 |
And it could be either sugar or artificial sweetener. 01:45:51.200 |
Sweet means anything that tastes sweet, all right? 01:45:54.800 |
And sugar is one example, and Splenda is another example. 01:45:59.240 |
- Aspartame, monk fruit, stevia, doesn't matter. 01:46:03.440 |
- Yeah, I mean, there's some that only humans can taste, 01:46:12.560 |
But we have put the human receptor into mice. 01:46:31.460 |
An option, my goodness, they will leak to know 01:46:35.760 |
from the sweet side, 10 to one at least versus the water. 01:46:49.660 |
So these mice no longer have in their oral cavity 01:46:56.760 |
be that sugar molecule, be it an artificial sweetener, 01:47:05.340 |
between sweet versus water, sugar versus water, 01:47:15.540 |
Because it doesn't have the receptors for sweet, 01:47:23.820 |
Now we're gonna do the experiment with sugar. 01:47:34.140 |
Normal mouse will drink from the sugar, sugar, sugar, sugar, 01:47:41.000 |
Knock out the sweet receptors, eliminate them. 01:47:47.600 |
But if I keep the mouse in that cage for the next 48 hours, 01:47:54.180 |
something extraordinary happens when I come 48 hours later 01:47:59.940 |
and I see what the mouse is leaking or drinking from. 01:48:31.140 |
Now, how does the mouse identify that bottle? 01:48:43.480 |
Sugar at high concentrations is kind of goopy. 01:48:46.100 |
The side-ness in which the bottle is in the cage. 01:48:53.420 |
But the mouse realize there is something there 01:48:56.800 |
that makes me feel good and that's what I want. 01:49:05.700 |
of our unquenchable desire and our craving for sugar 01:49:14.420 |
The first clue is that it takes a long time to develop. 01:49:18.380 |
Immediately suggesting a post-ingestive effect. 01:49:23.880 |
So we reason if this is true and it's the God brain axis 01:49:34.900 |
then there should be a group of neurons in the brain 01:49:41.880 |
And lo and behold, we identify a group of neurons 01:49:45.260 |
in the brain that does this and these neurons 01:49:48.140 |
receive their input directly from the God brain axis. 01:50:08.240 |
Now you ingest it and now it activates a selective group 01:50:12.300 |
of cells in your intestines that now send a signal 01:50:25.400 |
The tongue doesn't know that you got what you need. 01:50:33.940 |
that it's going to be used, which is the gut. 01:50:53.600 |
So these are gut cells that recognize the sugar molecule, 01:51:02.860 |
- And this sends a signal through the gut brain axis 01:51:07.220 |
to the cell bodies of these neurons in the vagal ganglia 01:51:19.580 |
One, you mentioned that these cells that detect sugar 01:51:23.300 |
within the gut are actually within the intestine. 01:51:28.220 |
I always think gut as stomach, but of course, intestine. 01:51:36.620 |
that you had successful ingestion and breakdown 01:51:41.280 |
of whatever you consume into the building blocks of life. 01:51:50.060 |
And so you want to make sure that once they are in the form 01:52:12.540 |
that now drives the development of our preference for sugar. 01:52:23.700 |
is that the sensors in the gut that recognize the sugar 01:52:28.580 |
do not recognize artificial sweeteners at all. 01:52:37.040 |
- Generically speaking, one can make that by this 01:52:42.260 |
because it's a very different type of receptor. 01:52:45.060 |
- It turns out that it's not the tongue receptors 01:53:01.420 |
of ultimately artificial sweeteners in curbing our appetite, 01:53:07.220 |
our craving, our insatiable desire for sugar. 01:53:12.260 |
Since they don't activate the gut brain axis, 01:53:18.080 |
they'll never satisfy the craving for sugar like sugar does. 01:53:23.080 |
And the reason I believe that artificial sweeteners 01:53:27.760 |
have failed in the market to curve our appetite, 01:53:36.540 |
is because they beautifully work on the tongue, 01:53:40.320 |
the liking to recognize sweet versus non-sweet, 01:53:48.120 |
but they fail to activate the key sensors in the gut 01:54:01.720 |
- So the issue of wanting, can we relate that 01:54:06.720 |
to a particular set of neurochemicals upstream? 01:54:15.440 |
that's communicated through presumably the nodose ganglion 01:54:21.100 |
- Very good, and from there, where does it go? 01:54:25.820 |
I, you know, of course I think molecules like dopamine, 01:54:31.480 |
"The Molecule of More," et cetera, et cetera. 01:54:34.400 |
Dopamine is a very diabolical molecule, as you know, 01:54:37.720 |
because it evokes both a sense of pleasure-ish, 01:54:41.520 |
but also a sense of desiring more, of craving. 01:54:49.800 |
are failing as a means to satisfy sugar craving 01:54:55.920 |
And yet, if we trigger this true sugar-evoked 01:55:02.640 |
wanting pathway too much, and we've all experienced this, 01:55:10.900 |
Now that could also be insulin dysregulation, 01:55:26.800 |
You know, we're facing a unique time in our evolution 01:55:32.460 |
where diseases of malnutrition are due to overnutrition. 01:55:42.600 |
I mean, historically, diseases of malnutrition 01:55:57.720 |
the activation of these circuits that control our wanting, 01:56:15.860 |
it's the lines of communication that are informing the brain, 01:56:25.380 |
the presence of intestinal sugar, in this example, 01:56:28.640 |
it's a very important target in the way we think about, 01:56:37.980 |
So I make your brain think that you got satisfied 01:56:42.980 |
with sugar, even though I'm not giving you sugar. 01:56:49.760 |
are the receptors for glucose in these gut cells 01:56:53.640 |
susceptible to other things that are healthier for us? 01:57:01.260 |
And I think an important goal will be to come up 01:57:06.020 |
with a strategy and identify those very means 01:57:11.020 |
that allow us to modulate the circuits in a way that, 01:57:15.620 |
certainly for all of those where this is a big issue, 01:57:28.440 |
- I could be wrong about this, and I'm happy to be wrong. 01:57:38.000 |
that don't just sense sugar, glucose, to be specific, 01:57:47.260 |
I could imagine a scenario where one could train themselves 01:57:57.660 |
that are rich in essential fatty acids, amino acids, 01:58:01.460 |
perhaps less caloric or less insulin-disregulating 01:58:09.540 |
I've always enjoyed sweets, but in the last few years, 01:58:12.420 |
for some reason, I've started to lose my appetite for them, 01:58:15.500 |
probably because I just don't eat them anymore. 01:58:21.900 |
- Yeah, and you're not reinforcing the circuits. 01:58:24.920 |
And so you're in essence are removing yourself, 01:58:32.580 |
We have a huge, incredible large number of people 01:58:58.860 |
- Yeah, I mean, we can have a long discussion 01:59:02.060 |
on the importance of coming up with strategies 01:59:11.780 |
But I wanna just go back to the notion of these brain centers 01:59:18.200 |
that are ultimately the ones that are being activated 01:59:39.780 |
that dedicated brain circuits would have evolved 01:59:56.800 |
And indeed, you know, animals evolved these two systems. 02:00:03.740 |
One is the taste system that allows you to recognize them 02:00:17.820 |
Oh my goodness, this tastes so good, it's so sweet. 02:00:23.540 |
And you know, oh my God, this is so delicious, 02:00:25.900 |
it's fatty or umami, recognizing amino acids. 02:00:41.900 |
You wanna make sure that these things get to the place 02:00:43.980 |
where they're needed, and they're not needed in your tongue. 02:01:25.700 |
that association now guides me to that's the one I want. 02:01:39.500 |
because these are the ones that activate the right circuits, 02:02:06.860 |
- Is immediate, and yet this is a slow system. 02:02:09.140 |
And so in a beautiful way, but in a kind of mysterious way, 02:02:13.800 |
the brain is able to couple the taste of a sweet drink 02:02:17.420 |
with the experience of nutrient extraction in the gut 02:02:20.380 |
under a context where the mouse and the human 02:02:30.260 |
- Yeah, but you have to think of it not as humans. 02:02:33.300 |
Remember, we inherited the circuits of our ancestors, 02:02:38.300 |
and they, through evolutionary, from their ancestors. 02:02:53.160 |
So forget, as humans, let's look at animals in the wild, 02:02:57.940 |
okay, which is easier now to comprehend the logic. 02:03:01.380 |
Why should this take a long time of continual reinforcement, 02:03:14.220 |
You wanna make sure that this source of sugar, 02:03:16.860 |
for example, in the wild, is the best, is the richest, 02:03:37.020 |
And if the system simply responds immediately 02:03:43.460 |
you're gonna form the association with those sources of food 02:03:48.460 |
which are not the ones that you should be eating from. 02:03:52.620 |
- Don't fall in love with the first person you encounter. 02:03:58.380 |
And so, evolutionarily, by having the taste system 02:04:08.820 |
to reinforce it only when sustained repeated exposure 02:04:22.520 |
And so, when we remove it from the context of, 02:04:30.980 |
We're not hunting there in the wild where I need to form. 02:04:36.100 |
And so, what's happening is that highly processed foods 02:04:41.100 |
are hijacking, co-opting the circuits in a way 02:05:09.140 |
that I think about supermarkets and restaurants. 02:05:16.460 |
and this slower signaling and the utility of having both 02:05:19.160 |
makes me realize that supermarkets and restaurants 02:05:23.700 |
are about the most unnatural thing for our system ever. 02:05:28.140 |
Almost the equivalent of living in small villages 02:05:31.980 |
with very few suitable mates versus online dating, 02:05:36.100 |
- Look, I'm not gonna make a judgment call there 02:05:41.300 |
- Yeah, but, and so do supermarkets, thank God. 02:05:49.420 |
are reliance on foods that are not necessarily healthy. 02:06:03.120 |
don't fall in love with the first, it need to work. 02:06:20.460 |
that you used to cook that spike, let's say with sugar. 02:06:29.620 |
And you equalize in both where they both provide 02:06:55.660 |
Now you're providing now a fully ready to use, 02:07:02.720 |
In the tangerine, that sugar, it's mixed in the complexity 02:07:20.180 |
that need a huge amount of work by your stomach, 02:07:29.060 |
So you're using a huge amount of energy to extract energy. 02:07:40.640 |
than when I take this process, highly extracted tangerine. 02:07:45.640 |
By the way, I use tangerines because I had a tangerine 02:07:52.940 |
- And so this goes back to the issue of supermarkets. 02:07:57.940 |
And so to some degree, you know, A, given a choice, 02:08:03.500 |
you don't wanna eat processed, highly processed foods 02:08:05.580 |
because everything's already been broken down for you. 02:08:11.100 |
And so you are co-opting, hijacking the circuits 02:08:15.660 |
in a way that they're being activated at a timescale 02:08:25.360 |
and I think a lot of data are now starting to support 02:08:27.580 |
the idea that while indeed the laws of thermodynamics apply, 02:08:54.700 |
depending on how we are receiving these nutrients. 02:09:20.660 |
and how ultimately and hopefully we can improve human health 02:09:28.620 |
Now it's very easy to try to connect the dots, 02:09:38.640 |
And I think there's a lot more complexity to it, 02:09:41.860 |
but I do think that the lessons that are emerging 02:09:46.340 |
out of understanding how the circuits operate 02:09:50.700 |
can ultimately inform how we deal with our diets 02:09:55.700 |
in a way that we avoid what we're facing now as a society. 02:10:18.580 |
from the training of the neuroscientist and vice versa. 02:10:33.260 |
Ultimately is the arbiter of many of these pathways. 02:10:38.280 |
- As a final question and one which is simply 02:10:43.880 |
of the listeners, what is your absolute favorite food? 02:10:59.620 |
- Yes, look, we, unlike every other animal species, 02:11:30.760 |
Rather than the food itself, to me is the whole thing. 02:11:36.540 |
there've been these experiments done in psychophysics, yeah? 02:11:39.260 |
I'm gonna take a salad made out of 11 components. 02:11:42.480 |
And I'm gonna mix them all up in potpourri of greens 02:11:52.160 |
in a beautiful arrangement and I'm gonna put it 02:11:54.240 |
behind a glass cabinet and I'm gonna sell them. 02:11:57.060 |
And I'm gonna sell one for $2 and one for $8. 02:12:10.620 |
And people will pay the $8 because, you know what? 02:12:38.260 |
I don't mean the everyday, let me have some chicken wings 02:13:14.780 |
- Yeah, because I know that it's not necessarily 02:13:17.140 |
the healthiest thing, red meat I'm talking about, yeah? 02:13:23.980 |
I'm talking seven days a week, Chile and Argentina, 02:13:33.220 |
Now maybe I have red meat, I know, once every four weeks. 02:13:42.760 |
Part of it is because I haven't had it in four weeks, eh? 02:13:46.180 |
But I love sushi, but I love the art of sushi. 02:14:08.140 |
That was the main reason I wanted to come to New York. 02:14:12.000 |
- There's also that Columbia University that's- 02:14:14.700 |
- I came here because I wanted to be with people 02:14:18.100 |
that are thinking about the brain the same way 02:14:22.100 |
can we solve this big problem, this big question? 02:14:26.180 |
- And certainly you're making amazing strides 02:14:29.740 |
And I love your answer because it really brings together 02:14:35.240 |
and the phenomenon we've been talking about today, 02:14:36.940 |
which is that while it begins with sensation and perception, 02:14:44.020 |
to person, place, and time, and many, many other things. 02:14:50.740 |
and certainly on behalf of all the listeners, 02:14:54.540 |
for the incredible work that you've been doing now 02:15:05.540 |
And I feel quite lucky to have been on the sidelines 02:15:09.420 |
seeing this over the years and hearing the talks 02:15:14.260 |
but also for your time today to come down here 02:15:35.820 |
If you're learning from and/or enjoying this podcast, 02:15:40.060 |
That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us. 02:15:50.480 |
If you have feedback for us in terms of comments 02:15:53.260 |
related to topics we've covered, or questions, 02:15:55.640 |
or topics, or guests that you'd like us to cover 02:15:57.640 |
on future episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast, 02:16:00.040 |
please put those in the comment section on YouTube. 02:16:04.120 |
On today's episode of the Huberman Lab Podcast, 02:16:07.700 |
but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast, 02:16:10.660 |
we talk about supplements that are useful for sleep, 02:16:17.600 |
If you're interested in some of those supplements, 02:16:24.820 |
and that map directly onto specific protocols 02:16:31.920 |
If you're not already following us on social media, 02:16:34.440 |
we are @hubermanlab on Twitter and @hubermanlab on Instagram. 02:16:45.960 |
but much of which is distinct from the information 02:16:49.800 |
We also have a newsletter in which we spell out protocols 02:16:52.760 |
and some summaries from previous podcast episodes. 02:16:56.620 |
you can go to hubermanlab.com, go to the menu, 02:16:59.300 |
and you'll find the Neural Network Newsletter. 02:17:11.000 |
Go to the menu and look for the Neural Network Newsletter. 02:17:13.540 |
And if you'd like examples of previous newsletters, 02:17:23.200 |
and the biology of the perception of taste in particular.