back to index

Dr. Robert Malenka: How Your Brain’s Reward Circuits Drive Your Choices | Huberman Lab Podcast


Chapters

0:0 Dr. Robert Malenka
2:37 Sponsors: ROKA & Levels
5:21 Dopamine & Reward Circuitry
11:31 Reward, Arousal, Memory & Dopamine
17:34 Context, Cues & Dopamine Modification
25:38 Memory & Reward Scaling
30:50 Dopamine, “Addictive Liability” & Route of Administration
39:7 Sponsor: AG1
40:4 Drugs of Abuse & Brain Changes; Addiction & Individual Variability
50:51 Reinforcement vs. Reward, Wanting vs. Liking
57:50 Opioids, Psychostimulants & Dopamine
63:38 Sponsor: LMNT
64:51 Self-Doubt, Confidence & Career
72:40 Autism Spectrum Disorder
79:29 Pro-Social Interaction & Reward; Oxytocin, Serotonin & Dopamine
90:30 Nucleus Accumbens & Behavior Probability
98:28 Reward for Pro-Social Behavior
103:13 Social Media & “Addictive Liability”; Gambling
112:17 Pain, Social Behavior & Empathy
122:19 Empathy Circuitry, Dopamine & Serotonin
130:7 Autism Spectrum Disorder & Social Interactions, Empathy
137:23 MDMA, Serotonin & Dopamine; Addiction & Pro-Social Effects
148:13 Autism Spectrum Disorder, Social Behavior, MDMA & Pharmacology
157:18 Serotonin, MDMA & Psychedelics
160:16 Psychedelics: Research & Therapeutic Potential
167:57 Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Momentous, Social Media, Neural Network Newsletter

Whisper Transcript | Transcript Only Page

00:00:00.000 | - Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
00:00:02.280 | where we discuss science and science-based tools
00:00:04.880 | for everyday life.
00:00:05.900 | I'm Andrew Huberman,
00:00:10.320 | and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
00:00:13.200 | at Stanford School of Medicine.
00:00:15.200 | Today, my guest is Dr. Robert Malanka.
00:00:18.020 | Dr. Robert Malanka is a professor
00:00:19.840 | of psychiatry and behavioral sciences
00:00:21.680 | at Stanford University School of Medicine.
00:00:24.160 | He is both a medical doctor and MD,
00:00:26.640 | and a researcher, a PhD.
00:00:29.280 | His laboratory is famous for having discovered
00:00:31.480 | some of the key components allowing neuroplasticity,
00:00:34.700 | that is the nervous system's ability to change
00:00:36.960 | in response to experience.
00:00:39.040 | In addition, Dr. Malanka's research
00:00:41.400 | is considered central to the textbook knowledge
00:00:44.240 | about how reward systems in the brain
00:00:46.160 | are organized and function.
00:00:47.880 | Indeed, Dr. Malanka's research over the last 10 or 15 years
00:00:51.140 | has merged what was once two disparate fields,
00:00:53.740 | the first being the study of neuroplasticity,
00:00:56.080 | again, the nervous system's ability to change
00:00:58.180 | in response to experience,
00:00:59.640 | and the other field being the field of dopamine
00:01:02.600 | as it relates to pleasure and addiction.
00:01:04.900 | His laboratory has shown, for instance,
00:01:07.120 | that when we seek out particular forms of pleasure,
00:01:09.620 | regardless of whether or not they are healthy for us,
00:01:11.960 | that changes the way that our reward circuitry works
00:01:14.780 | and actually changes the way that dopamine is released
00:01:17.760 | and how it impacts the brain.
00:01:19.820 | And his work has also informed
00:01:21.680 | how we seek out healthy pleasures,
00:01:23.660 | including healthy food and social connection.
00:01:26.820 | Today's discussion explores all of these topics,
00:01:28.900 | and by the end of today's discussion,
00:01:30.800 | you will have a rich understanding
00:01:32.600 | of how neurochemicals like dopamine and serotonin
00:01:35.680 | work in parallel to reinforce,
00:01:37.980 | that is, to increase the probability
00:01:40.320 | that we will engage in certain types
00:01:41.960 | of thinking and behaviors.
00:01:43.920 | So if you are somebody interested in neuroplasticity,
00:01:46.460 | that is, how the nervous system can change
00:01:48.240 | in response to experience,
00:01:49.900 | and/or you are interested in reward systems,
00:01:52.600 | what motivates us and what we are likely to pursue
00:01:55.440 | in the future, given our choices of past?
00:01:58.760 | And if you are interested in things like social connection
00:02:01.340 | and empathy or lack thereof,
00:02:03.460 | today's discussion encompasses all of those topics.
00:02:06.380 | It is worth mentioning that Dr. Malenka is a true luminary
00:02:09.800 | in all of the fields I just mentioned,
00:02:11.560 | as well as several other fields.
00:02:13.260 | In fact, when you look out on the landscape
00:02:14.880 | of modern neuroscience, what you'll discover
00:02:17.520 | is that a very large percentage of the top laboratory
00:02:20.440 | studying neuroplasticity and reward systems and so on
00:02:23.440 | all stemmed from having trained in Dr. Malenka's laboratory.
00:02:27.060 | So it's a real honor and pleasure
00:02:28.520 | to be able to host him today,
00:02:30.080 | and I'm sure that our discussion
00:02:31.960 | is going to greatly enrich the way that you think
00:02:33.960 | about brain function, neuroplasticity, and reward.
00:02:37.440 | Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast
00:02:40.180 | is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
00:02:42.940 | It is, however, part of my desire and effort
00:02:45.040 | to bring zero cost to consumer information
00:02:47.040 | about science and science-related tools
00:02:48.920 | to the general public.
00:02:50.240 | In keeping with that theme,
00:02:51.280 | I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
00:02:54.140 | Our first sponsor is Roca.
00:02:56.020 | Roca makes eyeglasses and sunglasses
00:02:58.080 | that are of the absolute highest quality.
00:03:00.480 | The company was founded
00:03:01.320 | by two all-American swimmers from Stanford,
00:03:03.320 | and everything about Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses
00:03:05.720 | were designed with performance in mind.
00:03:07.940 | I've spent a lifetime working
00:03:09.040 | on the biology of the visual system,
00:03:10.600 | and I can tell you that your visual system has to contend
00:03:13.060 | with an enormous number of challenges
00:03:14.680 | in order for you to be able to see clearly.
00:03:16.920 | Roca understands those challenges
00:03:18.520 | and the biology of the visual system,
00:03:20.300 | such that they've designed sunglasses and eyeglasses
00:03:23.000 | that always allow you to see with crystal clarity.
00:03:25.680 | Now, initially, Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses
00:03:27.960 | were designed for sports performance,
00:03:29.420 | and as a consequence, all of their glasses
00:03:31.560 | are designed to be very lightweight
00:03:33.280 | and to not slip off your face if you get sweaty.
00:03:35.800 | However, the design of the glasses
00:03:37.800 | includes some that are specifically for sport
00:03:39.840 | and others whose aesthetic
00:03:41.320 | really allows you to use them for sport
00:03:42.940 | as well as out to dinner or to work, et cetera,
00:03:44.720 | and that's how I use them.
00:03:46.040 | If you'd like to try Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses,
00:03:48.460 | you can go to roca.com.
00:03:50.120 | That's R-O-K-A.com and enter the code Huberman
00:03:53.260 | to save 20% off your first order.
00:03:55.340 | Again, that's Roca, R-O-K-A.com,
00:03:57.680 | and enter the code Huberman at checkout.
00:04:00.160 | Today's episode is also brought to us by Levels.
00:04:02.960 | Levels is a program that lets you see
00:04:04.580 | how different foods and behaviors affect your health
00:04:07.120 | by giving you real-time feedback on your diet
00:04:09.280 | using a continuous glucose monitor.
00:04:11.480 | One of the most important factors
00:04:12.820 | impacting your immediate and long-term health
00:04:15.280 | is the way that your body manages its blood glucose,
00:04:17.960 | or sometimes referred to as blood sugar levels.
00:04:20.620 | To maintain energy and focus throughout the day,
00:04:23.200 | you want to keep your blood glucose steady
00:04:24.960 | without big spikes or dips.
00:04:26.960 | Using Levels, you can monitor how different types of foods
00:04:29.720 | and different food combinations,
00:04:31.580 | as well as food timing and things like exercise,
00:04:34.180 | combine to impact your blood glucose levels.
00:04:36.780 | I started using Levels a little over a year ago,
00:04:39.200 | and it gave me a lot of insight
00:04:40.580 | into how specific foods were spiking my blood sugar
00:04:43.240 | and then leaving me feeling tired
00:04:44.800 | for several hours afterwards,
00:04:46.380 | as well as how the spacing of exercise and my meals
00:04:49.580 | was impacting my overall energy.
00:04:51.620 | And in doing so, it really allowed me to optimize
00:04:53.900 | how I eat, what I eat, when I exercise, and so on,
00:04:57.900 | such that my blood glucose levels and energy levels
00:05:00.600 | are stable throughout the day.
00:05:02.140 | If you're interested in learning more about Levels
00:05:03.980 | and trying a continuous glucose monitor yourself,
00:05:06.640 | go to levels.link/huberman.
00:05:09.440 | Right now, Levels is offering an additional
00:05:11.320 | two free months of membership.
00:05:12.640 | Again, that's levels.link, L-I-N-K/huberman
00:05:16.520 | to get two free months of membership.
00:05:18.560 | And now for my discussion with Dr. Robert Malenka.
00:05:22.060 | Dr. Malenka, Rob, welcome.
00:05:24.580 | - Yeah, thanks for having me.
00:05:25.940 | - Delighted to have you here,
00:05:27.120 | both for sake of your medical knowledge and training
00:05:30.440 | as a psychiatrist, and of course,
00:05:32.320 | as a luminary in the field of neuroplasticity,
00:05:36.840 | dopamine and reward systems, social systems,
00:05:39.640 | your knowledge of autism and social interactions,
00:05:42.760 | a newer interest in, or perhaps old interest in psychedelics
00:05:48.640 | and what they're doing and potential for mental health.
00:05:51.600 | There are just so many things that you've done
00:05:53.760 | in this field.
00:05:54.600 | I've been a long, long time fan of your work
00:05:57.040 | since your days as an assistant professor.
00:05:58.720 | I've tracked your career.
00:05:59.840 | I've learned a tremendous amount from you by observing you
00:06:03.000 | and from being your colleague.
00:06:05.200 | So really delighted to have you here.
00:06:07.520 | - You're making me blush,
00:06:08.600 | and I don't blush easily.
00:06:09.980 | - Well, it's all true.
00:06:12.040 | And I will say as well,
00:06:13.760 | you've also trained an enormous number
00:06:16.580 | of incredible scientists, Carl Deisseroth,
00:06:20.820 | the Carl Deisseroth.
00:06:22.820 | Anna Lemke always speaks incredibly highly of you
00:06:24.960 | as a mentor and somebody she's learned
00:06:27.400 | a tremendous amount from.
00:06:28.280 | And pretty much anyone that's worked on neuroplasticity,
00:06:31.440 | on dopamine and reward systems, addiction,
00:06:34.280 | and now in the fields of autism and soon psychedelics
00:06:37.200 | as well references as you often,
00:06:39.240 | and you've been mentioned many times before in this podcast,
00:06:41.440 | if not by name, by work.
00:06:43.080 | So again, thank you for being here.
00:06:46.160 | I'd love to kick off the conversation
00:06:47.840 | by talking about something which is very fundamental
00:06:50.680 | to everything we're going to talk about,
00:06:53.340 | but certainly fundamental to our daily lives,
00:06:55.040 | which is dopamine.
00:06:56.400 | We hear so much about dopamine.
00:06:57.840 | People talk about dopamine hits.
00:06:59.600 | People think about dopamine as pleasure, dopamine reward.
00:07:03.640 | For the novice, how would you frame the dopamine system?
00:07:07.920 | I mean, it does a bunch of different things
00:07:09.520 | in different areas of the brain and body,
00:07:11.160 | but to you, what does dopamine represent
00:07:14.520 | as its major function in the brain?
00:07:16.380 | And could you give us a general contour
00:07:19.200 | of the neural circuits that allow this chemical
00:07:22.800 | to more or less put value on our experiences?
00:07:27.720 | - Yeah, that's very well put.
00:07:31.240 | As you point out, dopamine is one of the major,
00:07:34.560 | what we term neuromodulators in the brain,
00:07:37.280 | a chemical signaling messenger that the brain uses
00:07:40.760 | to mediate a complex array of actions.
00:07:45.140 | Its best well-known function is in what we call
00:07:49.160 | the brain's reward circuitry.
00:07:51.460 | So this is a circuit in the brain,
00:07:54.680 | and when we use the term circuit,
00:07:56.160 | what we really mean is one part of the brain
00:07:58.940 | communicating with another part of the brain,
00:08:01.820 | because the brain is this very complex,
00:08:04.780 | it's the most complex organ in the universe
00:08:09.280 | with lots of different nerve cells
00:08:11.120 | talking to each other simultaneously.
00:08:13.040 | And as neuroscientists, we try to parse
00:08:17.360 | what different brain areas are doing
00:08:19.860 | and what different neuromodulators might be doing.
00:08:23.060 | And dopamine was discovered, oh, I should know this,
00:08:26.500 | many decades ago, and it's, as I said,
00:08:31.500 | the major chemical messenger molecule
00:08:36.120 | in the so-called brain's reward circuitry.
00:08:39.640 | And when you're talking about,
00:08:40.620 | so what is the brain's reward circuitry?
00:08:42.700 | This is a part of the brain that tells us something
00:08:47.700 | is reinforcing in our environment, some stimuli,
00:08:54.220 | or in quotes is rewarding, makes us feel better or good,
00:08:59.220 | although that's a gross oversimplification.
00:09:04.240 | And before getting into the details of dopamine
00:09:07.560 | and its function in the reward circuitry,
00:09:09.640 | I think it's useful to talk about
00:09:11.760 | why do we need a reward circuitry?
00:09:13.780 | Why do we need something in our brain
00:09:16.560 | that tells us this feels good or this feels bad?
00:09:20.560 | And it goes back to evolution.
00:09:23.200 | I am a biological scientist.
00:09:25.680 | That means I believe in evolution.
00:09:27.900 | And if you think about the evolution of our species,
00:09:32.120 | everything is driven by developing mechanisms
00:09:37.580 | that increase our survival.
00:09:39.640 | And it's really useful.
00:09:41.460 | You need something in your nervous system
00:09:44.220 | that tells you some stimuli in your environment
00:09:47.480 | is important for your survival,
00:09:49.580 | or some stimulus in your environment is dangerous.
00:09:53.800 | So it's not magic that sugary high fat laden foods
00:09:58.800 | are highly reinforcing and rewarding.
00:10:05.360 | It's not an accident.
00:10:06.520 | There has to be a mechanism in the brain
00:10:08.320 | that tells us that.
00:10:09.620 | It's not an accident that most of the time for most of us,
00:10:14.600 | a sexual experience is pretty reinforcing,
00:10:17.760 | is pretty rewarding.
00:10:19.200 | It's not an accident that warmth feels really good
00:10:23.260 | when you're cold, that water tastes much better
00:10:26.800 | when you're really thirsty.
00:10:29.020 | What evolved is a mechanism to tell our nervous systems
00:10:33.900 | and tell our brains this feels pretty good.
00:10:37.400 | I should repeat the behavior that leads
00:10:40.680 | to that rewarding experience.
00:10:42.580 | And similarly, it's really important
00:10:45.320 | when there is an event in your life that's highly dangerous
00:10:50.320 | for some mechanism in your brain to say,
00:10:53.640 | "Whoa, I don't wanna go back to where that line was."
00:10:57.160 | And we can get into that.
00:10:58.480 | So this was a long-winded way of saying
00:11:00.960 | what the reward circuitry tells us is this event,
00:11:05.240 | this stimulus, it could be an external stimulus.
00:11:08.640 | Like I said, a Krispy Kreme donut, which I happen to love,
00:11:12.960 | and I have to be very disciplined so I don't eat too many
00:11:15.520 | of them, it could be a drug of abuse,
00:11:19.560 | and maybe we'll talk about that a little bit.
00:11:21.860 | All of these stimuli seem to activate and cause the release
00:11:27.800 | of dopamine in this brain's reward circuitry.
00:11:31.640 | So now we need to get into a little bit of detail.
00:11:35.000 | Neuroscientists use these very unfriendly terms
00:11:41.120 | to describe different brain regions.
00:11:44.920 | So the home of dopamine cells,
00:11:47.880 | or brain cells are called neurons,
00:11:50.920 | so the home of dopamine neurons are in a part of the brain,
00:11:55.840 | sort of what we call the lower midbrain.
00:11:58.860 | The dopamine neurons that are part of the reward circuitry
00:12:02.960 | are found in this area called the ventral tegmental area,
00:12:07.240 | which I'm sorry to have to use such technical jargon.
00:12:10.600 | And we call it the VTA, that's the acronym.
00:12:13.640 | - I think the roof of the midbrain is the tectum,
00:12:17.180 | it means roof and the base of the midbrain,
00:12:20.280 | it means floor, which is tegmentum.
00:12:22.800 | I think that's the, so there's a rationale,
00:12:24.560 | but it doesn't help much at all to know the names.
00:12:27.040 | - And in fact, you are absolutely correct,
00:12:29.560 | and I always forget that, so thank you for pointing that out.
00:12:33.240 | - It's a side effect of teaching your anatomy.
00:12:35.040 | - And then, which I once did back in the early '80s,
00:12:38.840 | but I've forgotten everything I taught.
00:12:41.760 | Anyhow, so these dopamine neurons,
00:12:43.360 | and we can talk about other types of dopamine neurons,
00:12:46.300 | they send messages, what we call projections,
00:12:50.760 | using telegraph wires that we call axons.
00:12:54.840 | They send projections to many different brain regions.
00:12:58.120 | The key one in the brain's reward circuitry being an area,
00:13:02.800 | again, with a very complicated name
00:13:05.480 | called the nucleus accumbens,
00:13:07.640 | and maybe, Andrew, you know,
00:13:09.120 | I actually don't know how that name evolved,
00:13:12.440 | the nucleus accumbens, and I'm sure I should know,
00:13:15.480 | because I've been studying it for 30 years,
00:13:19.280 | but I have never looked up the genesis of that name.
00:13:22.640 | - Well, the fortunate thing about this podcast
00:13:24.680 | is it's both on audio platforms like Spotify and Apple,
00:13:28.000 | but also on YouTube, and so now we can be absolutely sure
00:13:30.800 | that somebody has put it into the YouTube comments
00:13:34.080 | underneath this episode,
00:13:35.560 | and therefore everyone will learn, including us.
00:13:37.520 | So I don't know the origins of the word nucleus accumbens.
00:13:40.560 | - And it's a gross oversimplification,
00:13:45.160 | but it's the activity of these dopamine neurons
00:13:48.120 | in the ventral tegmental area
00:13:50.200 | that then cause the release
00:13:53.440 | of this powerful neuromodulated, neuromodulator dopamine
00:13:58.440 | in the nucleus accumbens,
00:14:00.820 | which is part of another brain structure
00:14:04.760 | with a tough-to-remember name called the ventral striatum,
00:14:09.760 | and then magic happens, and when I say magic happens,
00:14:13.480 | even though we've been studying
00:14:15.400 | how dopamine modifies the properties of cells
00:14:20.400 | in this nucleus accumbens,
00:14:23.680 | the truth is we don't have a deep mechanistic understanding
00:14:28.200 | why, when dopamine is released in the nucleus accumbens,
00:14:33.000 | we experience that as, I'm being very cautious here.
00:14:37.440 | The simple way would be to say as highly rewarding,
00:14:41.160 | but it's a little more complicated than that.
00:14:43.520 | What it tells us is that there's something
00:14:46.360 | really important happening in our environment.
00:14:49.840 | - So could we say that it cues the arousal system?
00:14:53.600 | - It gets the arousal system going.
00:14:56.640 | There's close ties to our memory systems,
00:15:00.560 | which hopefully intuitively makes some sense.
00:15:02.880 | If something really important
00:15:04.760 | is happening in your environment.
00:15:06.580 | Because again, I think what's helpful for your audience
00:15:10.440 | is to always be thinking about how these systems evolved
00:15:13.760 | from an evolutionary perspective.
00:15:16.040 | And if dopamine is signaling something really important
00:15:19.520 | and salient is happening in your environment,
00:15:23.400 | you wanna remember that.
00:15:25.200 | It could be a highly rewarding experience,
00:15:27.720 | like a source of food.
00:15:30.600 | For me, I like all donuts.
00:15:33.720 | So I don't wanna emphasize any one manufacturer
00:15:37.600 | of one donut versus the other.
00:15:39.480 | I like sugar-laden, fat-laden foods.
00:15:42.880 | That's why I never eat them because I like them so much
00:15:45.640 | and I use that as an example.
00:15:47.320 | But because that was an important event for my survival,
00:15:50.880 | this reward circuitry,
00:15:52.420 | yes, it stimulates my arousal system.
00:15:55.360 | It gets me to pay attention.
00:15:57.800 | It also has very close ties to memory systems.
00:16:01.720 | And to go off on a little bit of a tangent,
00:16:04.660 | I think the one...
00:16:06.120 | I don't wanna say it's a mistake.
00:16:10.440 | I think perhaps somewhat oversimplification
00:16:14.640 | of how people conceptualize dopamine's role in the brain
00:16:19.520 | is even though it's a major important role,
00:16:22.920 | is for it to be active and released
00:16:25.780 | during highly reinforcing experiences,
00:16:28.700 | like sex, like really good food, like drugs of abuse.
00:16:32.840 | It also can get activated subdivisions of this system
00:16:36.780 | during painful stimuli and during aversive stimuli,
00:16:41.260 | which again are really important for you to be aware of,
00:16:44.480 | to say, "Oh my God, that's really bad for me."
00:16:48.960 | And so the dopamine system, this reward circuitry
00:16:53.140 | and its subcomponents that maybe perhaps signal
00:16:57.400 | more salience or aversion in the environment
00:17:00.740 | are closely tied to arousal systems and memory systems.
00:17:05.380 | Again, hopefully for somewhat obvious reasons.
00:17:09.440 | You wanna remember powerfully reinforcing events
00:17:14.200 | in your life as well as powerfully emotionally
00:17:18.600 | or physically painful events in your life.
00:17:21.480 | So I hope I answered your question to a modest degree.
00:17:26.480 | - Far better than a modest degree.
00:17:29.380 | That's an excellent description of the dopamine system
00:17:31.880 | from a true expert.
00:17:33.540 | And the question I have is about some of the context
00:17:38.000 | and nuance of the system.
00:17:39.680 | But in sort of real world terms,
00:17:42.960 | how should I think about this?
00:17:44.460 | Even in my training as a neuroscientist,
00:17:46.080 | I know neurons can be a little active, a lot active,
00:17:48.920 | everything in between.
00:17:49.840 | They can be active over long periods of time
00:17:51.440 | or short periods of time.
00:17:52.720 | But let's use the example of the donut.
00:17:55.280 | I like a glazed old fashioned donut.
00:17:57.840 | I actually don't have a craving for sweet things,
00:17:59.880 | but donuts is an exception.
00:18:02.320 | I like the glazed old fashioned donut.
00:18:04.420 | But if I were to see just a little piece
00:18:06.980 | of a glazed old fashioned donut
00:18:09.520 | versus a full glazed old fashioned donut,
00:18:12.560 | could I expect that more dopamine is released
00:18:16.480 | to the anticipation of the complete donut?
00:18:19.280 | And then the other question is,
00:18:20.840 | how does context influence the dopamine system?
00:18:23.920 | For instance, if I'm very full,
00:18:26.000 | a glazed old fashioned donut might be aversive to me.
00:18:29.160 | Whereas if I'm just a little bit hungry
00:18:31.640 | or if I'm actually more on a schedule of rewarding myself
00:18:35.360 | for abstaining from sweet, fatty foods,
00:18:39.200 | then abstaining from the food
00:18:40.640 | might be its own form of reward.
00:18:43.000 | And so to me, the dopamine system seems incredibly simple
00:18:45.960 | and yet incredibly prone to immediate context
00:18:50.000 | and the kinds of nuance that...
00:18:52.240 | I mean, we're constantly juggling...
00:18:55.000 | I'll interrupt myself to say that we're constantly juggling
00:18:57.000 | a bunch of different reward contingencies.
00:18:59.160 | We want to have good health metrics
00:19:01.760 | and maybe have a certain aesthetic qualities to our body,
00:19:04.080 | but we also want the donut.
00:19:05.320 | And so how does a system as simple
00:19:08.600 | as a one neuromodulator system
00:19:10.520 | and the VTA to nucleus accumbens
00:19:13.000 | and with some connections to the memory area,
00:19:15.520 | how does it balance all of that information in real time?
00:19:19.680 | To me, that's just like staggeringly complex,
00:19:22.360 | but also incredibly interesting.
00:19:24.440 | - I think you beautifully put, very eloquent description.
00:19:30.360 | You just said it, it's staggeringly simple,
00:19:33.520 | simultaneously staggeringly complex.
00:19:36.280 | And you asked several different questions.
00:19:39.520 | So context makes an enormous importance.
00:19:42.800 | And that's one of the reasons I became interested
00:19:46.680 | in the dopamine reward circuitry is, as you know,
00:19:51.080 | as a colleague in the academic neuroscience world,
00:19:53.680 | but your listeners probably don't,
00:19:56.040 | I started out my career studying
00:19:57.720 | very basic mechanisms of plasticity.
00:20:00.560 | How does the brain modify itself?
00:20:03.560 | And what makes the brain different
00:20:06.120 | than the computer hardware is the physical connections
00:20:11.120 | in the brain are constantly changing,
00:20:14.680 | the strength of the communication.
00:20:16.680 | Similarly, for the dopamine reward circuitry,
00:20:20.240 | it's highly plastic and it's highly contextually dependent.
00:20:24.200 | And so you gave the example of donuts and feeding,
00:20:29.160 | and I'll answer your question about the cues.
00:20:32.360 | Yes, I used to give the example of Thanksgiving.
00:20:35.800 | So let me give that example.
00:20:38.080 | In the morning of Thanksgiving,
00:20:40.120 | all for most of us in the United States,
00:20:44.960 | the morning of Thanksgiving,
00:20:46.440 | if you're at home visiting your parents,
00:20:48.480 | the smells of the apple pie,
00:20:50.480 | the smells of the turkey cooking
00:20:52.880 | are highly repetitive, highly reinforcing.
00:20:56.440 | You're anticipating that fun event.
00:20:58.920 | You're anticipating Uncle Joe
00:21:00.720 | coming to visit you for Thanksgiving.
00:21:03.720 | And that's all because these cues,
00:21:07.480 | the smells, the anticipation of Uncle Joe's,
00:21:10.600 | your previous experiences are part of your memory system,
00:21:15.080 | sort of talking to, in a simple way, your reward circuitry.
00:21:19.960 | So you're building up this anticipation.
00:21:22.320 | One could almost say this craving,
00:21:24.120 | which maybe we'll talk about in the context of addiction,
00:21:27.160 | and then make a long story short,
00:21:29.680 | think about that evening at the end of Thanksgiving,
00:21:34.280 | those exact same cues,
00:21:37.240 | the exact same smell of the apple pie,
00:21:39.320 | turkey, and Uncle Joe himself.
00:21:41.800 | At the very least, they're no longer repetitive,
00:21:45.600 | meaning they might actually be a versa.
00:21:48.280 | The last thing you want is a piece of apple pie.
00:21:51.120 | You can't wait for Uncle Joe
00:21:52.600 | to leave your Thanksgiving dinner.
00:21:54.800 | And I always argue that does not happen magically.
00:21:58.000 | That happens because your brain has been modified
00:22:02.120 | by the context in which it sits.
00:22:04.800 | And this very important modulatory system,
00:22:07.160 | this reward circuitry, is responding
00:22:09.720 | to the exact same stimuli with a very different response.
00:22:14.600 | So I'm just telling you, I'm repeating what you said,
00:22:17.360 | the phenomenology, and again, my other favorite example
00:22:20.960 | is any of us who have been in an intimate relationship
00:22:24.680 | knows that the love of your life
00:22:27.360 | can turn to the bane of your existence in 20 seconds.
00:22:31.680 | And again, that doesn't happen magically.
00:22:34.280 | This person who you crave, who you love, does something,
00:22:38.400 | and two minutes later, your brain is saying,
00:22:40.600 | "Oh my God, I may have to break up with this person,"
00:22:44.920 | or, "This is an incredibly painful emotional experience."
00:22:49.120 | And what fascinates me about the brain
00:22:51.320 | is how does the brain mediate that rapid change?
00:22:55.000 | So now back to, so yes, context is everything
00:23:00.000 | about how this powerful neuromodulatory system
00:23:03.560 | that uses dopamine works.
00:23:05.680 | And the truth is we don't know.
00:23:08.000 | It's because the inputs onto these dopamine neurons,
00:23:11.520 | the other nerve cells that are driving the activity
00:23:14.840 | of the dopamine neurons, and I've actually studied this
00:23:17.760 | in my lab at Stanford University with a colleague
00:23:21.440 | you know well, Li-Chen Luo,
00:23:23.560 | who's a world-class neuroscientist.
00:23:27.640 | We've studied the complexity of the neuroanatomy
00:23:30.840 | of the dopamine system, and these dopamine neurons
00:23:34.080 | in the ventral tegmental area,
00:23:36.120 | the source of the reward circuitry dopamine,
00:23:39.200 | are receiving inputs from all over the brain.
00:23:42.480 | They're receiving indirectly or directly inputs
00:23:46.240 | from visual areas, from somatosensory areas.
00:23:50.280 | And I'm not giving you a really good answer
00:23:53.320 | because that's one of the goals of my research,
00:23:56.440 | to try to understand how context,
00:23:58.880 | how the history that you've had with these cues,
00:24:03.040 | which we're gonna get back to, of the donut or of a drug,
00:24:06.360 | how has that modified
00:24:08.000 | how this neuromodulatory system responds?
00:24:13.000 | Similarly, the nucleus accumbens,
00:24:16.280 | the target of this powerful modulator dopamine,
00:24:21.160 | is receiving communications, what we call inputs,
00:24:27.040 | from all sorts of brain regions that you know about, Andrew,
00:24:31.160 | your audience may not.
00:24:32.640 | It receives inputs from an area called the hippocampus,
00:24:36.080 | which you may have covered in previous podcasts,
00:24:39.040 | which is very powerfully, very important for memories,
00:24:43.360 | both establishing new memories.
00:24:45.520 | And again, remember that makes sense.
00:24:47.560 | You want this system, this dopamine reward circuitry
00:24:51.320 | to be very connected to memory systems.
00:24:54.200 | So the activity in the nucleus accumbens
00:24:59.200 | is modulated by dopamine,
00:25:02.200 | while it is receiving information from the hippocampus,
00:25:05.760 | which helps encode new memories,
00:25:07.680 | while it's receiving information
00:25:09.440 | from a brain area called the amygdala,
00:25:12.680 | which is a part of the brain
00:25:16.000 | involved in our emotional experiences.
00:25:18.960 | The accumbens also receives inputs
00:25:21.720 | from the prefrontal cortex,
00:25:24.160 | which is this brain area, as you know better than me, Andrew,
00:25:29.160 | is important for decision-making,
00:25:35.160 | for planning our activity, and I could go on and on.
00:25:38.560 | - Well, could we talk about prefrontal cortex for a moment?
00:25:41.200 | Because it always was surprising to me
00:25:46.200 | that prefrontal cortex is talked about
00:25:50.160 | as this higher executive function area.
00:25:53.200 | But then when you look at the neuroanatomy,
00:25:55.120 | it's, as we say, monosynaptically, as you and I know,
00:25:59.040 | one connection away from structures like the amygdala,
00:26:03.320 | one connection away from structures
00:26:04.920 | like the nucleus accumbens.
00:26:06.600 | In other words, prefrontal cortex, to me,
00:26:08.400 | is every bit as ancient as some of these other structures
00:26:12.640 | that we think of as more ancient.
00:26:14.420 | And really the whole ancient evolved thing
00:26:16.160 | gets a little bit dicey because certain areas are,
00:26:18.200 | like the prefrontal cortex, are more elaborated in humans.
00:26:20.680 | But to me, the prefrontal cortex seems to be
00:26:23.420 | especially important in the context of this thing
00:26:26.600 | of scaling the reward response
00:26:29.560 | or context of the reward response,
00:26:32.880 | because it can set rules.
00:26:35.580 | It seems to know, okay, we're recording a podcast now,
00:26:39.940 | and there are certain rules.
00:26:40.780 | There are certain things that we're going to do and not do.
00:26:43.720 | But what's fascinating about the,
00:26:45.580 | and I'm so glad you gave a bunch of different examples,
00:26:47.740 | because what's fascinating, for instance,
00:26:49.640 | about the relationship example is that, yes, at one moment,
00:26:54.280 | we can adore somebody, and another moment later,
00:26:57.040 | if they do something or don't do something,
00:26:59.280 | we can be incredibly frustrated with them.
00:27:01.120 | They can even become aversive to us.
00:27:03.760 | Hopefully that doesn't happen too frequently.
00:27:05.880 | But I think we've all had the experience of a donut,
00:27:10.760 | an event, or a person actually looking different to us
00:27:17.200 | from one moment to the next, hopefully not at random, right?
00:27:20.120 | And so to me, it seems like the prefrontal cortex
00:27:23.560 | is uniquely positioned to really say, okay, right now,
00:27:27.220 | we are in a mode of, for lack of a better word,
00:27:30.040 | love and loving, like be in the verb tense of loving,
00:27:34.520 | be in the verb tense of arguing.
00:27:36.420 | We're now arguing.
00:27:37.860 | We're in the verb tense of reconciliation,
00:27:41.400 | kind of somewhere in between or something of that sort.
00:27:43.800 | And how a structure and a circuit as simple
00:27:48.680 | as the dopamine system, right?
00:27:49.820 | One molecule could suddenly say, oh, you know what?
00:27:52.180 | Now getting over my anger is rewarding.
00:27:54.680 | Whereas five minutes ago,
00:27:55.920 | being right and being the most angry was rewarding.
00:27:58.940 | And then five minutes before that,
00:28:00.960 | again, we're accelerating this movie,
00:28:02.560 | but five minutes or five days or five years before that,
00:28:05.720 | this person could do no wrong.
00:28:07.760 | And the dopamine system is just cranking out dopamine,
00:28:10.760 | saying, whatever you do, I'm just delighted by it.
00:28:13.440 | - Incredible.
00:28:14.580 | Like to me, I can't think of a more interesting system
00:28:16.860 | in neuroscience.
00:28:17.700 | - Well, I mean, that was eloquently put.
00:28:22.160 | I agree with pretty much everything you said.
00:28:25.520 | I don't have much to add because what you're pointing out is
00:28:28.880 | the challenges of studying these systems,
00:28:33.780 | the importance of studying these systems,
00:28:36.440 | and the challenge of presenting how the brain works
00:28:41.800 | to this podcast audience.
00:28:43.820 | Because on the one hand, you have done a fantastic job
00:28:48.820 | over the last few years in your podcast
00:28:51.800 | of making complex subjects accessible to a lay audience
00:28:56.800 | and get them to be thinking about how our modern view
00:29:05.020 | of how the brain works could be used to enhance health,
00:29:09.680 | could enhance mental wellbeing.
00:29:12.620 | But as neuroscientists, academic neuroscientists ourselves,
00:29:17.620 | we know, you know, you are oversimplifying things
00:29:22.680 | and the actual functioning of a system
00:29:25.880 | like the dopamine reward circuitry,
00:29:28.100 | as you just eloquently point out, is so much more complex.
00:29:31.760 | It's modified by these prefrontal inputs,
00:29:37.320 | which are simultaneously telling our memory systems,
00:29:41.160 | you know, pay attention here.
00:29:42.940 | I'm repeating what you just said.
00:29:46.180 | The context makes a big difference,
00:29:47.980 | the history you have with the person or stimuli
00:29:52.980 | with whom you're interacting.
00:29:54.600 | Like to bring this back to your, you know,
00:29:56.540 | which I never, the initial question,
00:29:58.560 | is a small piece of a donut activate the cue
00:30:03.300 | that that small piece of a donut activate
00:30:05.760 | the reward circuitry and cause release of dopamine
00:30:08.560 | to the same extent as the full donut?
00:30:12.480 | Depends on your experience with donuts.
00:30:15.480 | I mean, I think for you and me,
00:30:17.840 | because we seem to both have, you know, like donuts,
00:30:20.960 | they're highly repetitive for us.
00:30:22.980 | Probably doesn't matter because we have learned
00:30:26.120 | even a little piece of a donut activates
00:30:29.740 | all of our memory systems saying,
00:30:31.260 | man, that's an old fashioned glazed donut.
00:30:34.380 | I want to eat that or I want to get one
00:30:36.680 | or I want to have the discipline not to eat it.
00:30:39.880 | So I hope I'm answering your question
00:30:41.180 | and I'm shifting topics completely,
00:30:44.940 | but that's why addiction is so challenging.
00:30:48.680 | - Well, let's talk about that.
00:30:50.260 | Let's talk about that because you've done
00:30:51.680 | a ton of important work in this area of addiction.
00:30:54.720 | I mean, one of the basic questions I have about addiction
00:30:57.200 | is, you know, we hear that certain drugs
00:30:58.940 | are more addicting than other drugs or certain behaviors.
00:31:02.360 | We also hear that we can become addicted to anything.
00:31:04.700 | When Ana Lemki was on this podcast, I said,
00:31:07.200 | what's the most unusual addiction you've ever seen?
00:31:09.920 | And she talked about a patient who sadly committed suicide
00:31:13.600 | at some point later that she told us had been addicted
00:31:17.300 | to water, to drinking of any kind, first alcohol,
00:31:20.840 | but then water eventually.
00:31:22.820 | And so my question about addiction in the dopamine system
00:31:26.520 | is, you know, let's pick a drug like cocaine.
00:31:31.880 | I've never done cocaine, but people who have done cocaine
00:31:36.080 | tell me that it feels very good.
00:31:38.220 | And one of the more salient features of the cocaine high
00:31:42.580 | is that it comes on very fast
00:31:44.700 | and it ends pretty quickly too.
00:31:46.580 | Is the rate of dopamine increase related
00:31:51.400 | to the addictive property of a drug or behavior
00:31:55.600 | as much as how much dopamine is released?
00:31:59.360 | - And that's a very sophisticated question.
00:32:01.380 | And the answer is yes.
00:32:03.180 | And that's usually the lecture I give.
00:32:07.260 | The way I think about addiction,
00:32:09.540 | and obviously my friend and colleague Ana Lemki
00:32:12.460 | is one of the world's experts in terms of the understanding,
00:32:16.000 | the human experience of addiction.
00:32:18.040 | I have studied it as a cellular molecular neuroscientist
00:32:22.820 | trying to understand how addictive substances
00:32:27.920 | modify reward circuitry, modify the connections
00:32:31.560 | in the reward circuitry, modify how dopamine neurons act.
00:32:35.400 | And the way I, you know, like any,
00:32:38.340 | what appears to be a simple term,
00:32:41.720 | it's layered with complexity.
00:32:43.760 | Addiction is somewhat of a continuum.
00:32:47.340 | And I like to think about whether you're talking
00:32:49.620 | about substances like cocaine,
00:32:52.080 | and I will explicitly answer your question soon,
00:32:55.460 | or opioids, as you know, we're going in this country,
00:32:59.740 | there is an opioid epidemic.
00:33:03.040 | I do like to think about addictive liability.
00:33:06.800 | And it is, in my view, it is pretty clear
00:33:10.280 | that when we're talking about drugs,
00:33:12.620 | they have different degrees of addictive liability.
00:33:16.900 | I mean, I had a cup of coffee this morning.
00:33:19.300 | And many of us listening to this podcast,
00:33:24.640 | it's really hard to start our day
00:33:26.300 | without getting that hit of caffeine.
00:33:28.620 | But are we addicted to caffeine?
00:33:31.200 | That's a tricky question,
00:33:32.640 | because I've never heard of anybody robbing a bank
00:33:35.260 | to get caffeine, destroying their personal life
00:33:40.260 | to get caffeine.
00:33:41.740 | So I would say caffeine causes tolerance,
00:33:46.520 | but I would not say it has a particularly
00:33:48.600 | high addictive liability.
00:33:51.180 | Whereas drugs like psychosimulants, like cocaine,
00:33:55.520 | have a very, or opioids,
00:33:56.940 | have a very high addictive liability.
00:33:58.760 | So to answer your mechanistic question,
00:34:01.620 | there have been some famous studies done
00:34:04.620 | by the director of the National Institute on Drug Abuse,
00:34:08.620 | Nora Volkoff.
00:34:09.840 | Simultaneously, there have been studies
00:34:13.360 | in animal models of addiction where you nailed it.
00:34:18.980 | In a rough way, the addictive liability of a substance
00:34:23.620 | is directly correlated with two aspects of dopamine,
00:34:28.620 | how much dopamine is released in the accumbens
00:34:31.180 | and the kinetics of the dopamine release, as you said,
00:34:34.140 | how rapidly it's released.
00:34:36.520 | To get a little technical,
00:34:38.060 | even with a drug like cocaine or opioids,
00:34:41.140 | it's not only the drug itself,
00:34:43.900 | it's the root of administration,
00:34:46.180 | because the root of administration influences the kinetics,
00:34:50.640 | meaning how fast that drugs gets into your brain,
00:34:54.280 | influences the reward circuitry
00:34:57.160 | and how fast it causes a big rapid release of dopamine.
00:35:01.840 | And some of your podcast listeners may be old enough
00:35:06.200 | to remember the crack cocaine epidemic or freebase cocaine.
00:35:11.200 | And cocaine does have, like methamphetamine,
00:35:14.580 | a very high addictive liability.
00:35:17.780 | I give lectures to students at Stanford
00:35:22.660 | about neurobiology of addiction
00:35:24.300 | as part of a team-taught course.
00:35:28.860 | I have kids who I had to deal with.
00:35:31.800 | And what I always say is,
00:35:33.900 | it's not that if you use this drug,
00:35:36.820 | you're automatically gonna become an addict,
00:35:39.660 | but you're taking that risk.
00:35:43.320 | And it is impossible to become addicted to a substance
00:35:46.300 | if you've never used it, by definition.
00:35:48.220 | But back to the root of administration.
00:35:49.940 | So I went off on a tangent there.
00:35:51.780 | - Well, that's actually an interesting statement.
00:35:53.540 | Because I think we may have heard that in high school,
00:35:55.740 | although I, to be honest,
00:35:57.980 | wasn't the most attentive high school student,
00:35:59.620 | and I regret that high school students pay attention.
00:36:01.660 | - You did okay for yourself.
00:36:02.500 | - Eventually I came around,
00:36:03.860 | but it was an uphill battle there.
00:36:06.360 | But that you can't become addicted
00:36:08.100 | to something that you've never done,
00:36:10.100 | which I just wanna earmark that,
00:36:12.620 | because I think it's a profound statement,
00:36:14.640 | because it points to the importance of the memory system,
00:36:18.820 | but also plasticity.
00:36:20.300 | And so I wanna make sure that eventually we get around
00:36:22.640 | to talking about how the amount of dopamine released
00:36:26.160 | and the kinetics, how that might influence plasticity.
00:36:28.860 | Basically what I'm asking here,
00:36:30.200 | queuing up in the back of your mind,
00:36:32.420 | is whether or not addiction is just related
00:36:37.340 | to the sensation that we have when we indulge in a behavior,
00:36:41.380 | or when we are under the influence of a drug,
00:36:44.340 | or whether or not it actually modifies neural circuitry
00:36:48.060 | in a way that makes a broader range of drugs
00:36:53.060 | or experiences attractive to us.
00:36:56.200 | - It's probably the latter.
00:36:57.860 | But so let me get back,
00:36:59.460 | and I will answer that in a second,
00:37:00.960 | to the point I was making.
00:37:02.140 | So it's not only the substance,
00:37:03.960 | it's the root of administration.
00:37:08.540 | As I said, you can't develop a problem with a substance
00:37:12.640 | and develop a substance abuse problem if you never take it.
00:37:16.500 | But snorting cocaine is a different experience
00:37:21.500 | than smoking it or injecting it.
00:37:24.300 | And one of the reasons the crack cocaine epidemic
00:37:27.820 | was so powerful is it gets into,
00:37:34.660 | when you're smoking it or injecting it,
00:37:36.500 | and people do this now with methamphetamine,
00:37:40.260 | I mean, meth addicts, most of them,
00:37:42.360 | and that is another epidemic in our country,
00:37:45.580 | most of them smoke it.
00:37:47.140 | And the danger of that is the drug,
00:37:50.380 | whether it's cocaine, methamphetamine,
00:37:51.900 | gets into your brain almost instantaneously,
00:37:55.880 | causes a very rapid, powerful surge of dopamine
00:38:00.880 | in the accumbens, in this reward circuitry,
00:38:04.440 | and that the feeling you get,
00:38:06.500 | which, and we're gonna get into this,
00:38:07.940 | is not necessarily a happy feeling.
00:38:11.380 | And it only lasts, it can last for tens of seconds
00:38:16.380 | or a few minutes.
00:38:18.180 | And it's a feeling that gives you
00:38:22.740 | this overwhelming compulsion and urge,
00:38:27.660 | I wanna do it again.
00:38:29.460 | So even though it may not actually feel all that good,
00:38:34.460 | and again, this gets into,
00:38:38.260 | we didn't have an addiction problem
00:38:41.360 | for any substance other than alcohol
00:38:44.600 | for most of humanity's existence,
00:38:49.800 | because these substances like cocaine,
00:38:52.620 | methamphetamine, synthetic opioids like fentanyl,
00:38:56.080 | they didn't exist.
00:38:57.700 | And the truth is our brains are not designed
00:39:02.700 | to handle those kinds of very powerful substances.
00:39:07.980 | - As many of you know,
00:39:08.900 | I've been taking AG1 daily since 2012,
00:39:11.580 | so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.
00:39:14.020 | AG1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink
00:39:16.340 | that's designed to meet
00:39:17.160 | all of your foundational nutrition needs.
00:39:19.400 | Now, of course, I try to get enough servings
00:39:21.020 | of vitamins and minerals through whole food sources
00:39:23.320 | that include vegetables and fruits every day,
00:39:25.580 | but oftentimes I simply can't get enough servings.
00:39:28.240 | But with AG1, I'm sure to get enough vitamins and minerals
00:39:31.220 | and the probiotics that I need,
00:39:32.980 | and it also contains adaptogens to help buffer stress.
00:39:36.140 | Simply put, I always feel better when I take AG1.
00:39:38.940 | I have more focus and energy and I sleep better,
00:39:41.480 | and it also happens to taste great.
00:39:43.700 | For all these reasons, whenever I'm asked,
00:39:45.580 | if you could take just one supplement, what would it be?
00:39:48.280 | I answer AG1.
00:39:49.920 | If you'd like to try AG1,
00:39:51.640 | go to drinkag1.com/huberman
00:39:54.480 | to claim a special offer.
00:39:56.100 | They'll give you five free travel packs
00:39:57.860 | plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
00:40:00.520 | Again, that's drinkag1.com/huberman.
00:40:04.760 | - So where do you want to go from here?
00:40:05.820 | You asked a question about, you know,
00:40:07.740 | the neural mechanisms of what we call addiction.
00:40:12.660 | - Yeah, I'd like to know about the role
00:40:13.900 | of neuroplasticity in addiction.
00:40:15.740 | I do want to highlight something you said,
00:40:17.220 | and I apologize for interrupting a moment ago.
00:40:20.300 | - No, absolutely not. - But it was an interruption
00:40:22.420 | based on real excitement because a person I know quite well
00:40:27.420 | who is a recovered cocaine addict told me,
00:40:32.120 | and by the way, folks, this isn't I have a friend
00:40:34.340 | and I'm actually, you know, I truly have never tried cocaine.
00:40:37.680 | And this person said that the first time they did cocaine,
00:40:41.280 | his thought was I hate this and I can't wait to do it again.
00:40:47.140 | And that's exactly how you described it.
00:40:49.540 | And I think that is a fairly common experience
00:40:54.540 | with people suffering from an addiction disorder.
00:40:59.580 | We're not supposed to use the word addicts anymore
00:41:02.140 | because that's a little bit judgmental.
00:41:04.440 | - That's the new nomenclature.
00:41:06.460 | - Something along those lines.
00:41:07.660 | - Got it, calling someone an addict
00:41:09.820 | as opposed to being addicted.
00:41:10.980 | - Yeah, being, and that is a beautiful description.
00:41:15.980 | I hate it, but I want to do it again.
00:41:19.380 | And again, it just shows the power of this system,
00:41:23.460 | which remember, evolved for our survival.
00:41:27.040 | So a very simple way of thinking about it
00:41:28.860 | is these drugs are tricking the reward circuitry
00:41:32.740 | to say this stimulus, this experience
00:41:36.780 | is really important for my survival.
00:41:38.860 | I have to go do it again and again.
00:41:41.180 | And again, a side question is the huge question
00:41:44.180 | is why does some people develop an addiction problem
00:41:48.900 | and others who have used this substance just don't?
00:41:53.380 | And again, as a world-class neuroscientist yourself,
00:41:57.300 | you know the answer.
00:41:58.260 | It's always a complex combination of underlying genetics,
00:42:03.220 | the environment in which they find themselves,
00:42:06.040 | the environment in which they grew up
00:42:08.340 | and how that modified their reward circuitry.
00:42:12.500 | So to get at your question,
00:42:16.500 | one set of experiments my lab did,
00:42:19.820 | which other labs did too,
00:42:21.780 | I don't deserve the sole credit for this,
00:42:24.140 | is showing that drugs of abuse cause powerful plasticity
00:42:29.140 | in the neurons that make up the cells
00:42:33.140 | that make up the reward circuitry.
00:42:35.180 | And in fact, drugs of abuse like cocaine, methamphetamine,
00:42:39.580 | opioids like morphine, heroin,
00:42:44.180 | change the synapses.
00:42:46.740 | The synapses are the connections from other nerve cells
00:42:51.560 | onto dopamine neurons,
00:42:53.300 | onto the nerve cells in the accumbens
00:42:55.740 | and these connections, these synapses can change
00:42:59.100 | and drugs of abuse cause powerful changes
00:43:03.200 | in those connections and therefore powerful changes
00:43:06.900 | in the activity of the dopamine neurons
00:43:09.940 | and the neurons in the ventral, in the nucleus accumbens.
00:43:13.780 | And in fact, the types of changes that occur
00:43:16.980 | appear to be similar to the types of changes
00:43:21.000 | that have evolved for good uses,
00:43:24.540 | for adaptive forms of learning and memory.
00:43:27.580 | So again, this is an example
00:43:30.460 | that this superficially simple dopamine reward circuitry
00:43:35.460 | is changing all the time.
00:43:40.500 | It is highly plastic and can become more sensitive
00:43:45.060 | to certain experiences, et cetera, et cetera.
00:43:48.620 | - Well, could I ask you a question
00:43:49.660 | about some of the general contours of the plasticity
00:43:52.220 | in the dopamine system?
00:43:53.860 | You said before, and I love this statement,
00:43:56.820 | even though it's very simple,
00:43:58.380 | but in its simplicity, it's really elegant
00:44:00.340 | that we can't become addicted to a substance
00:44:02.560 | or a behavior that we haven't taken or partaken in.
00:44:07.220 | So is there data to support the idea
00:44:12.020 | that just one exposure to cocaine
00:44:15.820 | or one exposure to some sort of behavior
00:44:19.300 | can lead to a lasting change in the dopamine system
00:44:23.580 | such that one's propensity to be addicted to that substance,
00:44:28.580 | again, if one were to indulge in the future
00:44:31.620 | or behavior again in the future is increased.
00:44:34.820 | And I have a very particular reason for asking this,
00:44:36.880 | but I'm very curious what the answer is.
00:44:38.600 | - I mean, in the work my lab and other labs have done
00:44:43.600 | in preclinical rodent models, the answer is yes,
00:44:48.760 | a single administration of a drug of abuse like cocaine,
00:44:54.300 | like morphine, can cause relatively several days,
00:45:00.220 | several weeks of changes in the connections
00:45:05.060 | onto dopamine neurons and onto the neurons
00:45:09.500 | in the nucleus accumbens.
00:45:11.080 | Those changes, that does not mean these changes
00:45:14.480 | are permanent or irreversible,
00:45:19.120 | but the changes last a long time.
00:45:21.460 | And again, the big question for understanding
00:45:25.820 | the neurobiology of addiction is,
00:45:28.260 | those changes are probably happening
00:45:30.760 | in most people who take the drug in this case,
00:45:33.720 | and we can talk about other stimuli,
00:45:36.720 | non-drug stimuli that can become, in quotes, addictive.
00:45:40.640 | Again, why in certain individuals?
00:45:43.600 | To be honest, it's not a big deal.
00:45:46.300 | Yeah, I did cocaine at this party.
00:45:48.560 | It was nice, but I don't feel any craving
00:45:51.200 | or urge to do it again, whereas other individuals,
00:45:55.080 | it sets them down a very bad path
00:45:59.440 | and really badly affects their life.
00:46:02.000 | And that's a huge question in the research field,
00:46:05.560 | because obviously if we could make predictions
00:46:08.280 | on which individuals are more susceptible,
00:46:12.880 | and not to get too political here,
00:46:16.000 | but whether you develop a problem with addiction or not
00:46:22.040 | is influenced by the other parts of your life.
00:46:28.880 | - Do you have other ways of getting reinforcing stimuli,
00:46:32.640 | getting satisfaction, having an outlet,
00:46:35.760 | that other ways of activating your reward
00:46:38.240 | or dopamine circuitry?
00:46:39.840 | - Healthy ways. - Healthy ways.
00:46:41.720 | Like as you have articulated, I think, in your podcast,
00:46:46.040 | getting exercise.
00:46:47.960 | You and I both like to get exercise.
00:46:50.420 | I feel really good.
00:46:52.300 | Sometimes it's painful during the exercise,
00:46:54.300 | but afterwards I feel great.
00:46:57.680 | - Almost the inverse of the cocaine response.
00:47:00.000 | The desire and then the I hate this,
00:47:03.360 | but I can't wait to do it again.
00:47:04.420 | It seems like exercise is often the opposite.
00:47:07.160 | It's I hate this, I don't want to do this.
00:47:08.700 | And then afterwards, gosh, I always feel better
00:47:11.440 | and I'd be happy to do it again.
00:47:12.880 | - I mean, yes.
00:47:14.500 | I mean, I like to exercise chasing a ball
00:47:17.240 | because that gets me off thinking about this hurts.
00:47:20.800 | So anyhow, back to addiction.
00:47:25.080 | So yes, these drugs can cause,
00:47:28.440 | I don't want to, definitely not permanent changes
00:47:30.960 | from a single exposure.
00:47:32.680 | And the types of studies I'm talking about
00:47:36.360 | were all done in experimental animals.
00:47:41.280 | So how that relates to what happens in our brains,
00:47:46.280 | in human subjects' brains is not completely clear,
00:47:50.380 | but I think there are parallels.
00:47:52.040 | So the changes might last a few days, a week or two,
00:47:57.000 | but one can see if somebody,
00:47:59.080 | there have been studies done where in an animal model,
00:48:04.000 | if you give repeated administration of a drug like cocaine,
00:48:09.000 | the changes get stronger and they last longer,
00:48:12.960 | which is kind of intuitively obvious.
00:48:15.240 | But again, the big question is why in human subjects,
00:48:22.200 | there are people who can use these substances
00:48:25.080 | and not develop a serious problem
00:48:27.300 | and there are others where they're very, very damaging.
00:48:30.620 | And then that's why I still make the point,
00:48:34.840 | if you're a young person, do you want to take that risk?
00:48:39.080 | Is it worth it to have that experience?
00:48:42.180 | And that's an individual decision.
00:48:44.920 | - We've done some podcast episodes
00:48:46.600 | about alcohol, cannabis, et cetera,
00:48:48.200 | and there does seem to be a pretty wide variation
00:48:50.640 | in people's response to the information.
00:48:52.640 | I think because there are people out there who,
00:48:55.040 | well, I've got friends who are recovered alcoholics,
00:48:58.200 | who will tell me the first drink they took,
00:49:01.800 | they use language like,
00:49:03.400 | it combined with the chemistry of my body
00:49:08.600 | in a way that nothing before ever had
00:49:12.000 | and they felt like it was like this magic elixir, right?
00:49:14.960 | That has not been my experience with alcohol.
00:49:18.360 | - And I've heard the same stories
00:49:20.360 | and it's hard for me to relate because like you,
00:49:22.600 | alcohol does not have that effect on me.
00:49:26.200 | And that's where, it's hard to believe
00:49:29.520 | that kind of immediate response to alcohol
00:49:33.800 | is due to the environment in which they grew up,
00:49:38.160 | although that can have an influence.
00:49:39.740 | That just feels almost more genetically encoded.
00:49:43.400 | And there is evidence that issues with the use of alcohol
00:49:49.000 | in developing alcohol use disorder does run in families.
00:49:53.560 | And obviously if it runs in family,
00:49:55.160 | you have to worry about how the environment
00:49:56.960 | of that family influences.
00:49:58.360 | There's a lot of studies saying there is a genetic component.
00:50:01.760 | Maybe like you, if I have a drink or two in the afternoon,
00:50:05.500 | I just fall asleep.
00:50:06.840 | And it does not have that effect on me.
00:50:09.360 | And anyone could imagine similar things
00:50:14.240 | for other drugs of abuse.
00:50:16.600 | There are people who have used cocaine,
00:50:19.040 | have used methamphetamine,
00:50:20.840 | who find it modestly enjoyable,
00:50:23.640 | but it's not the be all or end all.
00:50:28.080 | It isn't this incredibly powerful experience.
00:50:31.000 | And you just talked about, I think, a friend or a colleague
00:50:34.440 | who said, "I hate it, I hate that,
00:50:36.280 | "but I wanna do it again."
00:50:37.880 | And that's fascinating.
00:50:39.320 | - Yeah, they're now a recovered alcoholic and cocaine addict
00:50:42.360 | and they've abstained for many years,
00:50:44.020 | but still get a little bit of a gleam in their eye
00:50:46.580 | when they talk about alcohol or cocaine
00:50:49.140 | in a way that I just can't relate to.
00:50:51.580 | - I can relate, I mean, can I tell you a little vignette
00:50:53.780 | about me, which I love to tell?
00:50:55.480 | - Sure, sure.
00:50:56.580 | - And it gets into how the reward circuitry
00:50:59.900 | is so closely associated with memory systems
00:51:04.900 | and how cues associated with powerful experiences
00:51:11.400 | develop their own reinforcing or aversive quality.
00:51:15.780 | So long story short, when I was a young kid,
00:51:18.820 | in, I can't remember, in my 20s, maybe 20,
00:51:21.960 | I spent a few weeks in Paris.
00:51:24.300 | I started smoking cigarettes.
00:51:25.840 | I mean, this is a long time ago.
00:51:28.580 | And I got, cigarettes are very interesting.
00:51:31.220 | Nicotine is highly addictive,
00:51:34.180 | as the tobacco companies were fully aware of.
00:51:37.380 | - High addictive liability?
00:51:38.780 | - Very high addictive liability.
00:51:40.220 | - People will rob people for the money to buy cigarettes?
00:51:43.020 | - They may not rob because,
00:51:48.440 | although my understanding is they've become quite expensive,
00:51:52.580 | but counterfeit cigarettes are a huge market
00:51:57.580 | for organized crime.
00:51:59.880 | There are parts of our, in the world,
00:52:04.200 | third world countries where organized crime
00:52:07.720 | produce counterfeit cigarettes
00:52:10.360 | and are making hundreds of millions or billions of dollars.
00:52:14.020 | And so I think nicotine as it is delivered in cigarettes,
00:52:20.180 | as you know, I mean, tobacco companies put in a lot of work
00:52:24.460 | to figure out the exact dose of nicotine
00:52:28.060 | that will make you get that kind of feeling
00:52:31.980 | that only lasts for a few minutes.
00:52:34.680 | So you wanna do it again and again.
00:52:37.500 | So we can talk about, and nicotine, you know,
00:52:41.140 | what becomes a problem in a specific society with addiction
00:52:46.140 | is not only based on the neurobiological actions,
00:52:52.320 | if we're talking still about drugs
00:52:53.960 | or substances of that substance,
00:52:56.000 | it's heavily influenced by the availability
00:52:58.260 | of the substance too.
00:52:59.440 | But my little story is I smoked some cigarettes in Paris.
00:53:02.580 | I learned why people like to smoke.
00:53:06.800 | It was very satisfying to have a cigarette
00:53:09.980 | in a Parisian cafe.
00:53:11.460 | It just, you know, and it's very interesting
00:53:13.480 | because the first few times you inhale tobacco,
00:53:16.160 | you get dizzy, it's kind of aversive,
00:53:18.340 | and it's exactly what you articulated.
00:53:20.940 | Despite that, you wanna do it again.
00:53:23.360 | So it was just a lot of fun for me.
00:53:26.260 | I enjoyed it.
00:53:27.300 | And I was disciplined, you know, at some point,
00:53:30.300 | whenever this was, I came back to the United States.
00:53:32.240 | I didn't smoke because I knew it was bad for you.
00:53:36.020 | But to this day, 40 years later,
00:53:39.760 | every time I go back to Paris, I get cravings.
00:53:43.640 | I actually just wanna get a pack of cigarettes.
00:53:47.580 | I want to have that feeling again of inhaling the smoke.
00:53:52.060 | But the point is of how powerful
00:53:55.940 | these rewarding experiences can be
00:53:58.020 | or reinforcing experiences.
00:54:00.180 | And for your audience, technically, you know,
00:54:03.660 | what I have been taught by some of my psychology colleagues
00:54:08.260 | is we use the term reinforcing
00:54:10.380 | in a very behaviorally defined way.
00:54:14.860 | Something is reinforcing is if the behavior
00:54:19.660 | that led to that stimuli,
00:54:22.240 | it makes you wanna do that behavior again.
00:54:24.780 | Rewarding means it actually felt, in quotes, good.
00:54:29.340 | - No, it's an important distinction.
00:54:31.340 | - They actually can be different.
00:54:33.100 | Again, as you defined by your friend who,
00:54:37.300 | I forget, I think it was cocaine.
00:54:38.780 | Cocaine was highly reinforcing,
00:54:41.420 | but it was not necessarily enjoyable or rewarding.
00:54:45.100 | And isn't that fascinating?
00:54:46.620 | I have some colleagues in the addiction field.
00:54:50.660 | One of them is retired now,
00:54:53.660 | Kent Barrage and Terry Robinson.
00:54:56.080 | They distinguish between the terms wanting and liking.
00:55:01.440 | Wanting and liking.
00:55:03.620 | And think about that.
00:55:05.400 | Liking something means it's something you like, you enjoy.
00:55:10.400 | Wanting means you want it,
00:55:15.420 | but you don't necessarily like it or enjoy it.
00:55:19.480 | And that's a description
00:55:21.360 | of your friend's experience with cocaine.
00:55:24.080 | Some of us have been in destructive relationships
00:55:27.800 | where you want that individual,
00:55:30.040 | but you're not sure you necessarily like that individual.
00:55:32.880 | - Sometimes people will be in relationships
00:55:34.360 | where they actively dislike the other person,
00:55:37.400 | which is a bit foreign of a concept to me,
00:55:39.840 | but it's interesting this separation
00:55:42.040 | of reinforcing and rewarding, wanting and liking,
00:55:44.220 | because one of the things that's very prominent
00:55:47.340 | in 12-step programs is to create rewards
00:55:51.000 | around abstaining from the drug or behavior.
00:55:54.160 | And I should mention that programs like 12-step,
00:55:56.360 | when followed, seem to have very high success rates.
00:55:58.680 | At least that's what Anna Lemke tells me.
00:56:00.720 | That in some ways they are modifying
00:56:05.840 | the wanting and liking.
00:56:07.560 | They're splitting the wanting and liking of alcohol,
00:56:11.000 | for instance, creating a liking of sobriety
00:56:14.480 | more than the wanting of alcohol, for instance.
00:56:17.040 | - That's beautifully put.
00:56:19.260 | And I think that's right.
00:56:20.560 | How that plays out in the neural mechanisms,
00:56:24.680 | that as a neuroscientist, I'm interested in it.
00:56:27.320 | Man, that's a tough one.
00:56:28.920 | But I think that's why those programs
00:56:35.220 | are pretty successful.
00:56:37.480 | It's helping the person make those dissociations.
00:56:41.640 | And I don't know that much about those programs,
00:56:45.580 | because I have not seen patients myself
00:56:49.200 | for whatever, it's been 27, 28 years,
00:56:52.520 | but I think part of them are to help that individual
00:56:56.960 | find both other sources of liking and reward,
00:57:01.960 | getting some satisfaction and reward
00:57:08.360 | from the actual abstinence,
00:57:10.800 | being able to cognitively teach themselves
00:57:14.220 | that I deserve a pat on the back, I deserve credit,
00:57:17.480 | I feel good that I did not take a drink at that party,
00:57:22.480 | that I did not use that substance again.
00:57:26.160 | And how that plays out in our brains is a really tough one.
00:57:30.160 | - Yeah, those are, the way you described it
00:57:32.200 | is exactly right, those programs are highly reinforcing
00:57:35.200 | for abstinence behaviors,
00:57:37.080 | everything from the social connection,
00:57:38.600 | which we're gonna get to social connection, as you know,
00:57:42.060 | to the way that people start to conceptualize
00:57:44.980 | their addict self versus other self.
00:57:46.900 | It actually involves a splitting of the self
00:57:48.880 | in interesting ways.
00:57:50.200 | As long as we're talking about donuts, cigarettes,
00:57:54.880 | alcohol, cocaine, I'm curious before we move
00:57:58.640 | to a bit more on neuroplasticity,
00:58:01.260 | is there anything that people ought to know
00:58:05.560 | about how different substances and behaviors
00:58:09.060 | that are addicting might impact
00:58:10.560 | the dopamine reward circuitry differently?
00:58:13.480 | So for instance, we talked about cocaine
00:58:15.420 | as having this very rapid onset,
00:58:16.900 | big increase in dopamine, then a crash, as we know,
00:58:19.720 | a certain pattern of kinetics, as you describe it,
00:58:23.860 | the opioid crisis is incredibly serious problem right now,
00:58:28.860 | as is methamphetamine, but it sounds like methamphetamine
00:58:32.300 | functions a bit like cocaine and in terms of its kinetics.
00:58:35.860 | So an opioid is a very different chemical than cocaine,
00:58:40.860 | but it sounds like it impacts the dopamine system.
00:58:45.580 | Is the dopaminergic activity caused by opioids
00:58:51.540 | responsible for the addictive properties of opioids?
00:58:53.860 | Or do people also like the feeling of being under opioids?
00:58:57.540 | I personally hate it coming out of surgery,
00:58:59.260 | like they gave me Vicodin once and I hated it.
00:59:03.100 | I'd rather have the pain, post-operative pain,
00:59:06.440 | than take something like Vicodin or a Valium or fentanyl
00:59:11.440 | or anything like that to me is just completely aversive.
00:59:14.820 | But I realized that there are many millions of people
00:59:16.920 | that feel quite differently.
00:59:18.460 | - It's a great question.
00:59:19.540 | So I think all the studies, both in human beings
00:59:23.720 | and preclinical animal models, yes, would suggest
00:59:28.720 | that the addictive liability of opioids
00:59:35.020 | and psychostimulants, which are cocaine and methamphetamine,
00:59:39.160 | have the common final action of causing massive release
00:59:43.820 | of dopamine in this target of the dopamine neurons,
00:59:48.540 | the nucleus accumbens.
00:59:50.580 | They do it, if we wanna get a little scientifically
00:59:54.100 | technical here, via very different mechanisms.
00:59:59.000 | So cocaine and methamphetamine,
01:00:02.300 | the drugs known as psychostimulants,
01:00:05.380 | actually bind to a protein in the brain
01:00:09.320 | or a molecule in the brain that is responsible
01:00:12.440 | for sucking up, it's a vacuum cleaner,
01:00:17.020 | sucking up the dopamine after it's been released.
01:00:20.260 | And cocaine prevents that dopamine from being vacuumed up
01:00:24.500 | so the cocaine hangs around longer.
01:00:27.060 | Meth not only prevents the dopamine from being vacuumed up,
01:00:31.860 | it actually causes the reverse.
01:00:35.020 | It actually causes the direct release of dopamine
01:00:38.400 | from what we call nerve terminals,
01:00:41.460 | from the site where dopamine's released.
01:00:44.060 | Opioids work very differently.
01:00:46.100 | They actually primarily, not solely,
01:00:49.140 | work where the dopamine neurons live.
01:00:53.300 | And it's a little complicated if it's not that critical,
01:00:57.020 | but they indirectly increase the activity
01:01:02.020 | within the dopamine neurons themselves,
01:01:05.000 | causing a big massive,
01:01:07.260 | bigger than normal release of dopamine.
01:01:09.840 | So that's one commonality.
01:01:13.280 | But anybody who has used these drugs or read about
01:01:16.120 | these drugs, the subjective experience of the drugs
01:01:19.720 | are dramatically different.
01:01:21.520 | And that's because of the actions they're having,
01:01:24.360 | not only in the reward circuitry, but throughout the brain.
01:01:27.960 | So, and it's interesting you talked about Vicodin.
01:01:30.560 | I've taken Vicodin because I've had several knee surgeries
01:01:33.800 | and things, like you, I didn't like it.
01:01:36.760 | I've gotten other opioids for pain relief
01:01:40.920 | that were great.
01:01:44.280 | I mean, they took away a lot of pain
01:01:46.720 | after my ligament repair.
01:01:49.340 | And that's a different question,
01:01:52.720 | that even when you're talking about opioids,
01:01:55.360 | all drugs are not, they're not identical.
01:01:58.720 | Fentanyl has a much larger addictive liability
01:02:03.720 | because of its molecular properties
01:02:06.520 | and how it's interacting with the opioid system
01:02:10.720 | in our brains and the receptors,
01:02:13.120 | the actual proteins in the brain that it interacts with.
01:02:17.460 | But the subjective experience of opioids,
01:02:20.580 | I mean, it's interesting.
01:02:22.100 | Some people love it.
01:02:24.200 | That's, you know, if we go back in history, as you know,
01:02:27.380 | there were the opium dens throughout
01:02:30.680 | Asia.
01:02:33.880 | There were wars about opioids,
01:02:37.040 | the famous opioid wars between China
01:02:39.480 | and the United Kingdom.
01:02:41.300 | I mean, showing you how powerful
01:02:43.680 | the availability of a substance like an opioid can be.
01:02:48.080 | So I'm going off on a tangent, I apologize.
01:02:51.040 | But commonality is dopamine release in the accumbens.
01:02:54.660 | But it's a, if you remember what a Venn diagram is,
01:02:58.680 | all these drugs have some common actions,
01:03:01.220 | usually on directly or indirectly
01:03:03.960 | causing the massive release of dopamine in the accumbens.
01:03:06.680 | But then they have their own individual actions
01:03:10.000 | because obviously when you take cocaine or methamphetamine,
01:03:13.320 | it's a stimulator, you know,
01:03:15.560 | people are grinding their teeth, they're hyped up.
01:03:18.480 | For most people, opioids are the exact opposite.
01:03:22.120 | I mean, in opium dens from the movies I watched
01:03:26.480 | and watching Narcos and all those TV shows,
01:03:29.560 | you're often, you're lying down,
01:03:32.440 | you're kind of in almost a dreamlike state.
01:03:35.440 | So very different subjective experiences.
01:03:38.560 | - I'd like to just take a brief break
01:03:40.140 | and thank one of our sponsors, which is Element.
01:03:43.000 | Element is an electrolyte drink
01:03:44.440 | that has everything you need and nothing you don't.
01:03:46.660 | That means plenty of salt, sodium, magnesium, and potassium,
01:03:50.280 | the so-called electrolytes, and no sugar.
01:03:53.100 | Now, salt, magnesium, and potassium are critical
01:03:55.960 | to the function of all the cells in your body,
01:03:57.800 | in particular to the function of your nerve cells,
01:04:00.440 | also called neurons.
01:04:01.640 | And we now know that even slight reductions
01:04:04.260 | in electrolyte concentrations or dehydration of the body
01:04:07.480 | can lead to deficits in cognitive and physical performance.
01:04:11.060 | Element contains a science-backed electrolyte ratio
01:04:13.580 | of 1,000 milligrams, that's one gram, of sodium,
01:04:16.680 | 200 milligrams of potassium, and 60 milligrams of magnesium.
01:04:20.200 | I typically drink Element first thing in the morning
01:04:22.260 | when I wake up in order to hydrate my body
01:04:24.400 | and make sure I have enough electrolytes.
01:04:26.160 | And while I do any kind of physical training
01:04:28.440 | and after physical training as well,
01:04:29.940 | especially if I've been sweating a lot,
01:04:31.740 | and certainly I drink Element in my water
01:04:34.560 | when I'm in the sauna and after going in the sauna,
01:04:37.040 | because that causes quite a lot of sweating.
01:04:38.720 | If you'd like to try Element,
01:04:39.800 | you can go to drink element, that's lmnt.com/huberman
01:04:44.280 | to claim a free Element sample pack with your purchase.
01:04:46.600 | Again, that's drink element lmnt.com/huberman.
01:04:50.800 | - Yeah, I had an experience with opioid recently,
01:04:53.640 | not voluntarily over the Christmas holiday.
01:04:57.080 | We went to visit friends and before going to sleep,
01:05:00.580 | I wanted some tea.
01:05:02.080 | And I asked if they had any non-caffeinated tea.
01:05:04.720 | So they gave me this tea.
01:05:06.680 | And that night I had the most bizarre dreams I've ever had.
01:05:10.720 | And I slept for 14 hours.
01:05:13.280 | The next morning I was like, what was that tea?
01:05:16.200 | And I felt off in the morning.
01:05:18.240 | And I went, it was actually a blue lotus flower tea
01:05:21.420 | that is actually illegal in the United States,
01:05:23.580 | but it is sold and it has morphine-like compounds in it.
01:05:27.860 | And I am one of those people that's very susceptible
01:05:31.200 | to even low doses of any kind of novel drug.
01:05:34.340 | - So interesting, have you ever taken cough syrup
01:05:36.900 | with dextromethorphan?
01:05:38.480 | - No, I avoid that stuff.
01:05:40.400 | - Well, I have a tendency when I get a cold,
01:05:44.560 | it gets into my lungs, I cough a lot.
01:05:47.260 | And I think this has been reported.
01:05:49.480 | This is my anecdotal experience.
01:05:51.280 | I'm confirming what you said.
01:05:52.960 | Dextromethorphan is a different sort of opioid.
01:05:56.900 | And actually some people develop a problem with it.
01:06:00.220 | For me, it gives me really bizarre dreams,
01:06:03.420 | really similar to what you were describing.
01:06:05.020 | - Yeah, it was a very unusual experience.
01:06:06.220 | - And that's a whole different conversation
01:06:08.180 | about what makes us dream
01:06:09.800 | and what are the meaning of dreams.
01:06:12.160 | Fascinating, I hope you've covered,
01:06:13.920 | maybe you've covered that.
01:06:14.760 | - We have not yet, but we are intending
01:06:16.460 | to do a whole series on sleep and dreaming.
01:06:18.120 | - Yeah, I think that would be wonderful.
01:06:19.240 | - We will definitely get into it.
01:06:20.080 | - I started out in sleep research,
01:06:21.760 | so I have a fondness for it.
01:06:23.240 | - Well, drug research and sleep research
01:06:24.720 | have a long history of overlap with Alan Hobson's work
01:06:27.680 | on LSD. - I worked with Alan Hobson.
01:06:29.380 | - Okay, by the way, folks, if you're interested
01:06:31.260 | in the relationship between hallucinations and dreaming,
01:06:34.420 | Alan Hobson is a good name to start your rabbit hole.
01:06:37.940 | - In 19, oh my God, I'm dating myself.
01:06:41.700 | 19, oh, 70, I can't remember if it was 76 or 77,
01:06:46.700 | I worked with Alan Hobson as an undergraduate.
01:06:50.140 | - At Harvard Medical School.
01:06:51.040 | - No, as an undergraduate at Harvard.
01:06:52.860 | - He was at Harvard Medical School, yeah.
01:06:54.920 | - Amazing, I love his writing and I learned a lot from it.
01:06:57.360 | He was really ahead of his time.
01:06:58.960 | - Yes, he was.
01:06:59.800 | Anybody who knows me won't believe this,
01:07:03.840 | but back then, I was a very shy, insecure,
01:07:08.080 | 20-year-old kid. - I would not have guessed.
01:07:09.960 | - Oh, and even in medical school,
01:07:12.960 | I literally was not confident of my opinions at all.
01:07:16.680 | I was very shy, thought all of the ideas I had
01:07:21.040 | must be obvious and I should never say them out loud.
01:07:23.780 | - Do you mind if I ask, since you raised this,
01:07:25.960 | I think it's really important.
01:07:27.340 | I mean, you have this incredible career track record.
01:07:31.740 | You're adored by your colleagues, you're highly respected.
01:07:35.580 | You've won just about every award there is
01:07:37.820 | to win in neuroscience.
01:07:38.900 | So was there something in particular
01:07:40.440 | that was in an overnight thing where one day you woke up
01:07:43.420 | and thought, I actually believe in myself.
01:07:46.540 | But if you wouldn't mind sharing that,
01:07:48.040 | 'cause I think before we get back into some of the science,
01:07:50.800 | I, you know, science is a human endeavor
01:07:53.240 | and most people listening are probably not scientists,
01:07:55.780 | but I think everybody deals with these issues of self-doubt
01:07:59.640 | and people appear to have varying levels of confidence,
01:08:02.780 | but what happened?
01:08:05.700 | - So thank you for asking.
01:08:07.140 | No, for me, it was a very gradual process
01:08:11.920 | and I'm not, as an undergraduate, as a medical student,
01:08:15.900 | even as a postdoc, yeah, I was very unsure
01:08:20.640 | of my ideas, of my intellectual abilities,
01:08:24.700 | of whether what I was thinking was really worthwhile.
01:08:29.700 | So it was a very gradual process.
01:08:32.980 | I think the increase in my confidence, I think,
01:08:36.520 | began when I was a postdoc, which is a training period
01:08:41.260 | after you've received a PhD or an MD
01:08:44.700 | where you get additional research training.
01:08:47.480 | And I worked with a guy named Roger Nicollet, UCSF,
01:08:50.140 | and Roger was a very intellectually intense,
01:08:54.040 | very forceful individual.
01:08:57.480 | And I got involved in a field where,
01:09:01.480 | I mean, people, a little bit of a tangent.
01:09:04.400 | Your listeners may think that scientists
01:09:08.420 | are these geeky individuals wearing white coats
01:09:13.260 | with no passion or emotion
01:09:15.880 | and nothing could be further from the truth.
01:09:18.840 | The most successful scientists that I know
01:09:21.960 | are pretty passionate and pretty intense
01:09:25.360 | about what they're working on and driven.
01:09:29.080 | I mean, and this is a gross generalization.
01:09:32.100 | So anyhow, during my postdoc, I started getting involved
01:09:35.760 | in a topic where there were vigorous arguments
01:09:40.220 | about phenomenology we were studying.
01:09:43.940 | So I had to develop a tougher and thicker skin.
01:09:48.360 | I had to be able to argue my side
01:09:51.160 | of the hypotheses we were generating.
01:09:55.080 | So it started developing as a postdoc
01:09:57.320 | and then it slowly evolved as an assistant professor.
01:10:01.240 | And for your listeners who don't know,
01:10:03.860 | I don't like to admit this, but I'm in my late 60s.
01:10:08.800 | I have been running my own lab for almost 40 years.
01:10:12.900 | So I have been, so gradually as an assistant professor,
01:10:17.400 | I realized, hey, I can do this.
01:10:20.040 | I can do science.
01:10:21.280 | I can write papers that my colleagues
01:10:23.280 | seem to be interested in.
01:10:25.360 | And then gradually, over then the next 10, 20, 30 years,
01:10:29.580 | I gained more and more confidence.
01:10:33.720 | So for me, it was this very gradual buildup
01:10:38.720 | of many different experiences
01:10:44.160 | where I developed some confidence that not all of my ideas
01:10:49.160 | are great, of course they're not,
01:10:51.800 | but it's okay to voice my opinion.
01:10:55.760 | It's okay to state my ideas and why I believe this
01:10:59.640 | and why I don't believe that.
01:11:01.600 | So that was my experience.
01:11:03.600 | - Thank you for sharing that because I think
01:11:06.400 | people struggle with that very issue.
01:11:08.940 | And clearly showing up again and again
01:11:12.000 | over a long period of time is helpful.
01:11:13.580 | But as you said, learning to trust one's ideas,
01:11:16.400 | just a brief anecdote when I was coming up in neuroscience,
01:11:21.120 | a few years behind you.
01:11:23.000 | - 20 years behind me.
01:11:24.880 | - Not too many, I mean, but I recall the incredible number
01:11:29.880 | of high profile papers on neuroplasticity
01:11:34.400 | and long-term potentiation, long-term depression.
01:11:36.120 | These are terms related to the modification of synapses
01:11:40.360 | that Rob Malenka and Roger Nicole pioneered
01:11:44.180 | a big segment to that work.
01:11:45.580 | And I remember seeing your names on papers
01:11:47.420 | and I thought Roger worked for you.
01:11:50.020 | Sorry, Roger.
01:11:50.840 | - I'd love to hear that, I'd love to hear that.
01:11:53.500 | - I was under the impression he worked for you
01:11:55.700 | and only later did I learn that you were his postdoc.
01:11:58.620 | - And then we collaborated as equals.
01:12:01.020 | - You became peers very quickly.
01:12:02.340 | - Very quickly.
01:12:03.180 | - Yeah, so you've had a-
01:12:04.220 | - And Roger, you know, I-
01:12:05.620 | - Roger's wonderful.
01:12:06.460 | - I did have the confidence even as a postdoc
01:12:09.980 | and he actually, even as a grad student,
01:12:11.640 | even though I was a little insecure about my ideas,
01:12:14.780 | I wanted to be treated as an equal.
01:12:16.800 | That's the one thing I did have.
01:12:18.180 | I never felt that I was working for somebody else.
01:12:21.560 | I always felt that I was working for myself
01:12:24.460 | and that we were colleagues,
01:12:26.640 | even though my mentors had more experience
01:12:29.880 | and I could learn from them, but-
01:12:31.600 | - I like that you're working for yourself
01:12:33.260 | even though you have mentors.
01:12:34.860 | I think there's some real gems in what you just described.
01:12:38.240 | So thank you for taking the time to do that.
01:12:40.360 | - Sure.
01:12:41.260 | - I'd like to discuss one aspect of reward circuitry
01:12:45.520 | that I don't think most people think about, right?
01:12:48.680 | Fairly straightforward nowadays.
01:12:50.520 | I like to think more people know what dopamine is
01:12:52.800 | and understand it thanks to your work and Ana's work
01:12:56.000 | and some discussions have taken place on our podcasts,
01:12:58.240 | other podcasts, but, you know, all too often we think
01:13:01.600 | dopamine, reward, wanting, liking, drugs.
01:13:04.440 | Okay, all of that is great.
01:13:07.020 | But what about the truly adaptive stuff, right?
01:13:10.080 | Because it's easy to fall into a discussion around dopamine
01:13:13.980 | of, you know, the things that are bad for us.
01:13:16.060 | But what I'm thinking about here is social interaction.
01:13:19.400 | Clearly we are a social species and a lot of your work
01:13:24.600 | in the last decade and a half or so
01:13:27.960 | has focused on the relationship
01:13:30.680 | between the reward circuitry,
01:13:32.160 | which you beautifully described for us,
01:13:34.240 | and social interaction and connection.
01:13:37.880 | And where I'm going with this is ultimately
01:13:40.140 | this has huge implications for autism
01:13:43.780 | and autism spectrum disorders.
01:13:45.940 | I don't know if nowadays, is it okay?
01:13:47.740 | You're not supposed to call autism a disease.
01:13:49.360 | Is that right?
01:13:50.200 | You hear about neuro-typical and neuro-atypical,
01:13:52.720 | but I have friends who have children
01:13:55.660 | who are severely autistic.
01:13:57.540 | And I don't know many parents who would elect
01:14:00.580 | to have a severely autistic kid.
01:14:03.100 | And so those people often will talk about it as autism
01:14:07.360 | or a child having autism.
01:14:08.940 | So first of all, before we get into the social piece,
01:14:11.640 | maybe because I just tabled it,
01:14:13.300 | how are we supposed to talk about autism nowadays?
01:14:17.140 | - I am very interested in the pathophysiology
01:14:23.020 | of what the medical profession
01:14:26.160 | terms autism spectrum disorder.
01:14:28.900 | As you pointed out, the individuals living
01:14:33.580 | with an autism spectrum disorder are quite heterogeneous.
01:14:38.340 | And it can range from individuals
01:14:42.200 | with severe intellectual impairments
01:14:45.960 | and quite severe impairments in social interactions,
01:14:50.960 | impairments in sensory processing,
01:14:54.080 | impairments in lots of different aspects of our behaviors
01:14:58.600 | that are important.
01:14:59.860 | And I think nobody would say,
01:15:02.300 | would argue those individuals on the severe spectrum
01:15:05.240 | do not have some sort of, in quotes, disorder.
01:15:09.780 | The issue we have to be sensitive to
01:15:13.480 | is it's a heterogeneous disorder like many brain issues
01:15:18.480 | that psychiatrists deal with, like depression.
01:15:22.000 | We all like obsessive compulsive disorder,
01:15:25.360 | like various anxiety disorders.
01:15:26.800 | It's always on a continuum and a spectrum.
01:15:30.340 | So for autism spectrum disorder,
01:15:34.060 | there are individuals who are high functioning,
01:15:37.260 | who one could argue have a different style
01:15:42.260 | of interacting socially,
01:15:44.020 | may have a different way of processing sensory information,
01:15:47.820 | but who would prefer not to be viewed as having an illness,
01:15:54.140 | but rather would be viewed as having a different style
01:15:58.380 | of living and interaction.
01:16:00.080 | And I think we need to respect that.
01:16:02.180 | So the challenge is, again,
01:16:05.520 | not oversimplifying a complex heterogeneous disorder
01:16:10.520 | and both being respectful of the people
01:16:17.440 | who don't wanna be defined as having
01:16:19.460 | a neuropsychiatric or brain disorder,
01:16:22.240 | while equally being respectful of people like your friends
01:16:25.800 | with severely impaired children who deserve help,
01:16:30.640 | who deserve research.
01:16:32.400 | And it's a tough one because my understanding from,
01:16:35.760 | to be honest, just reading articles in the lay press
01:16:39.000 | and going to websites from organizations
01:16:42.600 | that philanthropically support research related to autism,
01:16:46.940 | within that community of individuals
01:16:50.240 | who are not researchers,
01:16:52.400 | but who have family members or are themselves
01:16:56.800 | dealing with some degree of autism spectrum disorder,
01:17:01.280 | there's disagreements about what terminology to use,
01:17:06.160 | how to deal with them.
01:17:07.200 | And it's complicated.
01:17:08.620 | I think we just have to respect everybody.
01:17:10.560 | And if you're interacting with individuals,
01:17:15.240 | I think it's appropriate.
01:17:16.320 | What do you prefer?
01:17:18.360 | - I do know as a medical professional,
01:17:22.020 | and especially when you're dealing with children,
01:17:25.000 | there are children who need help.
01:17:28.960 | And we're not doing them a service
01:17:32.020 | by saying they don't have an issue
01:17:34.560 | that we should be helping them with and working on.
01:17:36.720 | So I hope that answers your question.
01:17:38.520 | - Beautifully, I think it beautifully answers it
01:17:40.880 | and encompasses all sides so that we can move forward.
01:17:44.800 | And I think, so as we use the term autism
01:17:47.500 | or children or people with autism,
01:17:49.520 | that's what we're referring to.
01:17:51.720 | - I think people are very sensitive,
01:17:53.220 | especially those individuals who are neuro-atypical,
01:17:57.960 | who previously might be diagnosed
01:18:00.000 | as autism spectrum disorder,
01:18:01.760 | but would prefer to not be labeled
01:18:05.320 | as having a brain illness, that's fine.
01:18:08.980 | It's kind of, once you are an adult,
01:18:11.860 | you can make that decision for yourself.
01:18:14.340 | - We certainly have colleagues at Stanford and elsewhere
01:18:16.640 | who, at least by my non-clinical assessment,
01:18:19.360 | seem to fall somewhere on that spectrum.
01:18:21.360 | - And again, it's a continuum,
01:18:22.640 | just like the experience of depression is a continuum.
01:18:26.280 | - And as with depression,
01:18:28.400 | you wouldn't love a child or an adult any less
01:18:30.360 | because they have depression,
01:18:31.380 | nor would you love a child or adult any less
01:18:34.200 | because of expression of some autism symptoms.
01:18:37.400 | - I know the point.
01:18:38.500 | People don't, you know.
01:18:39.600 | And so we are being trained in the medical profession
01:18:43.920 | to be very, and our society is going this way too,
01:18:48.040 | very careful with the terms we use
01:18:52.560 | and the labeling of individuals.
01:18:54.920 | So I've been taught you can say individuals
01:18:59.160 | living with an autism spectrum disorder,
01:19:02.200 | some people don't like using a term,
01:19:05.300 | oh, that individual is autistic,
01:19:07.580 | because that has some, can have some,
01:19:11.300 | I don't wanna say derogatory meaning,
01:19:13.360 | but some labeling kind of,
01:19:15.200 | but you know, sometimes this gets out of control too,
01:19:17.960 | as we both know.
01:19:18.800 | - Well, for sake of fluid conversation,
01:19:20.480 | we will do our best,
01:19:21.520 | but we will acknowledge from the outset
01:19:23.820 | that we are well-meaning,
01:19:24.880 | but far from perfect in how we'll handle this.
01:19:27.320 | - Well put, well put.
01:19:29.000 | - So in thinking about social interactions
01:19:30.400 | and leaving aside anything related to autism for the moment,
01:19:33.400 | it appears that the circuits in the brain
01:19:36.720 | that mediate the desire to spend time
01:19:38.900 | with others of the same species,
01:19:40.840 | maybe even with other species like a dog,
01:19:43.300 | are fairly hardwired, but modifiable.
01:19:48.160 | We were born with the capacity to build them up
01:19:51.320 | and that social behavior is highly rewarded.
01:19:56.820 | Is it rewarded through the dopamine system?
01:20:00.080 | And what, if any, involvement is there
01:20:03.080 | of the serotonergic system?
01:20:05.040 | And we haven't talked about serotonin yet,
01:20:06.600 | but I'd love to bring up serotonin at this point.
01:20:08.760 | Maybe you could educate us a little bit about serotonin
01:20:11.240 | because, gosh, if dopamine is fascinating,
01:20:14.660 | serotonin is at least as incredible.
01:20:18.420 | - Yeah, great question.
01:20:19.520 | So I think for me, the easiest way for me to answer it
01:20:22.840 | is actually just tell you my research history
01:20:27.320 | and how a lab like mine at Stanford
01:20:30.720 | that at one point was studying
01:20:33.200 | what you and I would call
01:20:35.560 | fairly hardcore molecular mechanisms of neuroplasticity.
01:20:40.560 | How do connections between nerve cells change
01:20:44.880 | and what molecules are changing
01:20:47.160 | and pretty hardcore molecular stuff.
01:20:50.540 | How did I end up studying social behaviors in mice
01:20:54.080 | and what I hope we'll end up talking about,
01:20:56.380 | even developing behavioral models
01:20:58.520 | of what I will define as empathy in mice?
01:21:02.080 | The answer is very simple.
01:21:03.600 | My lab was working on roles
01:21:07.360 | of classic dopamine reward circuitry
01:21:10.200 | and how it changes in models of addiction.
01:21:14.000 | We haven't talked about depression, models of depression,
01:21:16.960 | because just intuitively,
01:21:18.380 | hopefully your listeners can understand
01:21:20.840 | if one component of depression is what we call anhedonia,
01:21:25.800 | the inability to experience reward.
01:21:29.720 | Eating a donut is no longer satisfying.
01:21:32.100 | Having sex is no longer that much fun,
01:21:34.760 | which is a component of depression.
01:21:36.920 | If there's a mechanism in the brain
01:21:39.160 | that tells you something is rewarding,
01:21:41.180 | by definition, that's not functioning normally
01:21:43.780 | in severe depression.
01:21:45.380 | So we were doing models of depression
01:21:47.360 | to figure out how the dopamine reward circuitry
01:21:50.280 | was changing, as were many other labs.
01:21:52.960 | We were studying addiction.
01:21:54.420 | Those were the obvious ones.
01:21:56.180 | And I mean, it might be entertaining to your audience
01:21:59.840 | to learn how academic scientists think.
01:22:02.480 | I was thinking, those are fascinating topics.
01:22:05.160 | They're pretty competitive.
01:22:07.300 | Lots of other labs were working on it.
01:22:09.920 | And I started thinking, what other experiences
01:22:14.600 | might be modifying the reward circuitry?
01:22:17.440 | I actually made some attempts to look at feeding behavior,
01:22:20.560 | but I don't wanna, I mean,
01:22:21.800 | we actually never pursued that for a variety of reasons.
01:22:25.320 | And that's obviously important because of,
01:22:27.880 | there is an obesity epidemic in this country.
01:22:32.160 | And we can talk about how the reward circuitry
01:22:36.280 | and some of the things we've learned
01:22:37.960 | from our studies of addiction may be helpful
01:22:41.560 | to understanding obesity.
01:22:42.960 | But back to social interaction, I started thinking,
01:22:46.480 | well, for most of us, what I call a prosocial,
01:22:51.480 | non-sexual experience is highly reinforcing.
01:22:56.280 | Andrew, you're a pretty social guy.
01:22:58.160 | I'm a pretty social guy.
01:22:59.880 | Most of the time, I'd rather go to a movie,
01:23:03.040 | a sporting event, a dinner with friends.
01:23:06.500 | Actually, for me, the most meaningful component of my life,
01:23:12.000 | other than spending time with my children,
01:23:14.520 | is spending time with my close friends.
01:23:17.100 | And I started thinking, well, why is that?
01:23:19.440 | Why do I have such a good time going to a ball game
01:23:22.940 | with my best friend or going out to dinner
01:23:25.220 | with another couple and interacting?
01:23:28.980 | It's because, well, it's highly reinforcing.
01:23:31.080 | And if it's highly reinforcing,
01:23:32.760 | it must involve the reward circuitry.
01:23:35.380 | And then I started thinking evolutionarily,
01:23:37.700 | it makes a lot of sense because if you are part
01:23:41.000 | of a social species, there's a lot of evolutionarily,
01:23:45.820 | a lot of advantages for your survival to be hanging out
01:23:52.820 | with other members of your species in a non-aggressive way.
01:23:57.040 | It can increase your likelihood
01:23:59.800 | to find a mate and reproduce.
01:24:02.040 | It can protect you from predators.
01:24:05.000 | I mean, that's why any of your listeners
01:24:07.760 | who ever watch wildlife shows or National Geographic shows,
01:24:12.760 | there's a reason all these animals hang out together.
01:24:17.320 | It's for protection from predators.
01:24:19.680 | So there are all these reasons.
01:24:21.400 | So about whenever it was, 13 or 14 years ago,
01:24:26.400 | my lab decided to start looking at how the reward circuitry
01:24:31.180 | may play a role in what I am gonna call
01:24:34.840 | positive prosocial non-aggressive interactions.
01:24:39.840 | Another word we use is just sociability.
01:24:43.520 | And for a variety of reasons back then,
01:24:48.120 | this is at least 13 years ago, maybe 15 years ago,
01:24:53.120 | a postdoc joined my lab named Gul Dolan.
01:24:56.180 | She's now a professor at Johns Hopkins.
01:24:58.560 | And she had an interest in oxytocin.
01:25:02.100 | And as your listeners know,
01:25:05.440 | oxytocin is this evolutionarily conserved neuropeptide
01:25:11.040 | that's very important for parturition,
01:25:15.540 | having a baby born, for milk being produced.
01:25:18.940 | And it's gotten a lot of attention
01:25:20.500 | as a potential love neuropeptide
01:25:23.540 | is something that is released in our brains
01:25:26.660 | during a positive social interaction.
01:25:29.140 | There's a well-known researcher in social behavior
01:25:34.140 | and bonding research called Larry Young.
01:25:37.840 | And he did some very important,
01:25:39.440 | now somewhat classic work studying a species called the vole
01:25:44.300 | in particular the prairie vole.
01:25:46.400 | And prairie voles are a species where they mate for life.
01:25:51.260 | It's called pair bonding.
01:25:53.080 | So one vole will find another vole.
01:25:55.220 | They basically get married, they have kids,
01:25:58.260 | and they hang out together for the rest of their life.
01:26:01.920 | - No divorce, no 50% divorce rate.
01:26:03.780 | - No 50% divorce rate.
01:26:06.480 | And what Larry elegantly showed in part,
01:26:10.080 | in early days in collaboration with a guy named Tom Insull,
01:26:13.520 | who is a famous academic psychiatrist,
01:26:16.400 | they showed that oxytocin action
01:26:20.300 | within the nucleus accumbens, within this reward circuitry
01:26:23.520 | was required and really important
01:26:26.660 | for this monogamous pair bonding.
01:26:29.880 | Having said that, there was just a paper
01:26:32.820 | that called into question that, but that's--
01:26:36.500 | - But there's 30 years of research prior to that.
01:26:39.140 | I'm glad you brought that up
01:26:40.100 | because we'll keep this contemporary
01:26:41.780 | and the reality is that that recent paper
01:26:44.720 | got a lot of attention.
01:26:45.740 | - You know the paper I'm talking about.
01:26:46.580 | - Yeah, that maybe oxytocin isn't playing
01:26:48.060 | as prominent a role in pair bonding as people had thought.
01:26:50.460 | And yet, folks, that could be true.
01:26:54.860 | We have to be scientific about this and be open-minded.
01:26:57.060 | But there's three decades of the work
01:26:59.820 | that speaks to the contrary.
01:27:01.480 | So I think we wanna weigh the evidence.
01:27:04.620 | - Yeah, exactly.
01:27:05.460 | And again, the investigators who presented the work
01:27:08.760 | saying oxytocin may not be as important,
01:27:10.920 | there are limitations to the manipulations they did,
01:27:13.560 | which they would agree with.
01:27:14.780 | So I'm just telling you,
01:27:16.600 | so Guldolin was a postdoc in my lab
01:27:19.680 | and we formulated a project to look at the actions
01:27:22.820 | of oxytocin in the nucleus accumbens in mice.
01:27:27.540 | And the reason we study mice is they're what are known
01:27:32.460 | as a genetically tractable organism.
01:27:35.620 | We have all sorts of really cool
01:27:38.920 | and sophisticated tricks we can do
01:27:42.620 | to probe brain circuitry,
01:27:45.780 | the actions of neuromodulators like dopamine and serotonin
01:27:49.300 | and oxytocin in ways that we can't do in other species.
01:27:54.300 | And I'm gonna get back to dopamine in a second.
01:27:58.940 | And what we found was that oxytocin action
01:28:03.380 | in the nucleus accumbens was indeed important
01:28:07.480 | for promoting sociability,
01:28:10.760 | probably for promoting the reinforcing component
01:28:15.760 | of a social interaction.
01:28:17.980 | And that surprised us.
01:28:20.100 | It was like, wow, it's oxytocin seems to be causing,
01:28:24.800 | enhancing the release of serotonin in the nucleus accumbens.
01:28:29.180 | And that, perhaps we'll get to this,
01:28:31.920 | that led me off on a whole series of experiments
01:28:35.580 | trying to figure out how serotonin works,
01:28:38.040 | studying this drug we may talk about called MDMA,
01:28:41.340 | which is ecstasy or molly,
01:28:43.340 | which actually causes release of serotonin.
01:28:46.100 | So we did that work and that got us working in serotonin.
01:28:49.340 | Simultaneously, there were some other papers reporting
01:28:53.580 | that dopamine release in the accumbens,
01:28:56.180 | that dopamine is released in the accumbens
01:28:58.900 | during a social interaction,
01:29:00.560 | a positive non-aggressive social interaction.
01:29:03.220 | Truth be told, it may also be released
01:29:05.780 | during an aggressive interaction.
01:29:08.820 | - Some people like to fight.
01:29:10.020 | - Some people like to fight.
01:29:11.500 | And the difference here is the dopamine release
01:29:16.280 | and its role in social interactions,
01:29:18.160 | it's not specific only for social interaction,
01:29:20.740 | as we have talked about.
01:29:22.180 | But nevertheless, that led my lab and other labs
01:29:26.320 | to do a series of papers.
01:29:29.140 | I'm talking about the field now showing that,
01:29:32.700 | and I'm giving you a lot of information here.
01:29:34.780 | So how might dopamine release happen
01:29:38.500 | during a non-aggressive social interaction?
01:29:42.020 | It turns out that oxytocin is not only released
01:29:46.080 | in the nucleus accumbens,
01:29:47.740 | it's released in the home of the dopamine neurons in the VTA.
01:29:52.060 | So my lab and another lab from Northwestern
01:29:55.140 | showed that oxytocin can actually modulate
01:29:59.260 | dopamine neuron activity in the ventral tegmental area.
01:30:03.100 | So I hope I'm making sense here.
01:30:04.620 | I don't wanna get too technical.
01:30:06.060 | But it just shows how we discuss these neuromodulators
01:30:11.060 | like dopamine, I just brought in oxytocin.
01:30:14.620 | We're gonna talk about serotonin in a second.
01:30:17.300 | Unfortunately for your listeners,
01:30:19.540 | they don't work in isolation.
01:30:21.420 | They influence each other in ways
01:30:25.700 | that I think it's important for us
01:30:27.500 | to understand and elucidate.
01:30:29.340 | - That is not too much technical detail.
01:30:33.100 | And I think it's wonderfully rich
01:30:34.300 | with areas for us to discuss.
01:30:36.180 | And I'm so very glad that you brought up
01:30:38.900 | that neither dopamine nor serotonin nor oxytocin
01:30:42.100 | work in isolation because all too often,
01:30:44.140 | and admittedly, sometimes even on my podcast,
01:30:47.940 | I'll talk about these things in isolation
01:30:49.520 | as a way to try and simplify them a bit.
01:30:51.180 | But there's just no way that the brain works that way.
01:30:54.960 | For instance, turning on dopamine and turning off serotonin,
01:30:57.420 | it's a weighting of inputs.
01:30:59.660 | And I think that serotonin, perhaps I should frame it
01:31:02.900 | this way, just as often as dopamine is framed
01:31:05.580 | as this reward molecule and pleasure and dopamine hits,
01:31:08.980 | all too often, I think, in the popular press,
01:31:11.780 | serotonin is discussed, and oxytocin too, for that matter,
01:31:16.540 | as this kind of warm, feel good, everything's mellow,
01:31:20.140 | not really associated with a reward and reinforcement.
01:31:24.020 | And of course, it's not that simple.
01:31:26.220 | So when it comes to social interactions,
01:31:28.780 | it sounds like oxytocin and serotonin
01:31:30.980 | are playing a prominent role, also in the accumbens,
01:31:34.680 | and that dopamine is activated too, have that right?
01:31:39.180 | Okay, so I don't want to take us too far down
01:31:42.700 | the rabbit hole of neural circuit function,
01:31:45.180 | but that to me makes at least a brief discussion
01:31:49.020 | about the nucleus accumbens itself.
01:31:51.180 | Interestingly, okay, so I'm thinking nucleus,
01:31:53.020 | I know that means a pile of neurons,
01:31:55.060 | an aggregation of neurons,
01:31:56.220 | it's talking to this ventral striatum.
01:31:58.580 | So we got a bunch of neurons--
01:31:59.420 | - It's a part of the ventral striatum.
01:32:00.620 | It's a subdivision of it.
01:32:02.260 | - Excuse me, I misspoke.
01:32:04.000 | Yeah, it's part of the ventral striatum,
01:32:05.820 | and the neurons there can be active
01:32:09.500 | and communicate with other brain areas,
01:32:10.960 | but we're talking about a lot of nuance of function.
01:32:13.620 | - Oh man, I'm smiling.
01:32:15.940 | I don't know if your audiences can see me smiling,
01:32:17.900 | because it's so, I sometimes go to bed feeling,
01:32:21.060 | it's so complicated, oh my God.
01:32:23.100 | - It is, and yet, could we say
01:32:25.100 | that within the nucleus accumbens,
01:32:26.740 | there are neurons that are acting
01:32:28.220 | as accelerators and breaks?
01:32:32.020 | Is there a simple analogy that perhaps,
01:32:34.620 | while not exhaustive, can still be true?
01:32:37.140 | 'Cause that's always the goal on this podcast.
01:32:38.940 | There's no way we can be exhaustive,
01:32:40.280 | but we want to be as accurate as possible.
01:32:42.500 | - So a very influential hypothesis,
01:32:45.380 | which has guided my thinking,
01:32:47.420 | and again, the trick, I mean,
01:32:50.140 | you have done a wonderful job of communicating
01:32:54.220 | complex scientific topics to your podcast audience,
01:32:58.900 | and I congratulate you on that.
01:33:00.860 | And it's a really important role.
01:33:04.220 | But as you know, it's always more complicated
01:33:09.860 | than we want it to be as scientists,
01:33:12.260 | especially when you're dealing with brain activity issues
01:33:16.280 | and how the brain mediates all its amazing functions.
01:33:20.500 | So historically, we have thought about
01:33:24.020 | the nucleus accumbens and other components
01:33:28.680 | of this ventral striatal brain area
01:33:33.140 | as primarily being composed to two different cell types.
01:33:38.140 | And as you pointed out, one being sort of an accelerator,
01:33:42.820 | something that promotes certain behaviors,
01:33:46.060 | and then the other cell type somewhat being a break,
01:33:51.060 | saying don't do that behavior,
01:33:53.140 | don't perform that motor action.
01:33:55.100 | And it is true that there are these different cell types.
01:34:01.060 | It is true that they are modulated by these modulators,
01:34:06.140 | like dopamine and serotonin, in different ways.
01:34:10.900 | And that simplistic hypothesis,
01:34:15.460 | or heuristic we call it, has been very useful
01:34:18.860 | in making models about how the accumbens
01:34:22.040 | does all its wonderful things.
01:34:23.980 | What I'm leading up to is,
01:34:25.560 | unfortunately, it's a little more complicated.
01:34:28.140 | But yes, there are two different cell types.
01:34:31.440 | And at least for your audience,
01:34:33.080 | we can think about dopamine driving the activity of one,
01:34:38.080 | promoting certain behaviors,
01:34:40.700 | and inhibiting the activity of the other cell type,
01:34:43.620 | and being a sort of break on certain behaviors.
01:34:47.700 | As long as you and I as scientists appreciate,
01:34:50.820 | it's not quite that simple.
01:34:52.380 | It's a little more complicated.
01:34:54.020 | - So using that as a framework
01:34:55.940 | to think about social behavior, as you said,
01:34:59.260 | pro-social, non-aggressive, non-sexual interactions
01:35:04.260 | involve the choice of a lot of behaviors,
01:35:08.100 | but also the suppression of a lot of behaviors.
01:35:10.700 | And so maybe you're starting to sense what I'm doing here.
01:35:14.180 | I think for people to understand how a single structure
01:35:18.340 | like the accumbens could mediate social interaction
01:35:20.440 | and reward it, what it sounds like it's doing
01:35:22.960 | is rewarding a certain category
01:35:25.940 | and catalog of behavioral options
01:35:28.700 | and punishing, or at least reducing the probability
01:35:31.800 | of the occurrence of other behavioral actions.
01:35:34.100 | 'Cause when I go to dinner with friends,
01:35:35.580 | if I know them really well, I might hug them.
01:35:38.140 | I might even say something mildly inappropriate.
01:35:41.700 | If I know the context to be safe, right?
01:35:44.220 | But at a dinner interview or discussion with somebody
01:35:48.060 | I barely know, I might watch my words a little bit more,
01:35:51.780 | for instance.
01:35:52.620 | - And I think the accumbens and its associated circuit,
01:35:55.620 | I love the way you just put that, probabilities.
01:35:58.540 | It's my probability of having this behavior
01:36:01.460 | in a certain context is increase the probabilities
01:36:05.220 | of not doing certain behaviors.
01:36:07.400 | And I think there's little doubt that this brain area
01:36:11.660 | called the nucleus accumbens and all of its associated
01:36:14.880 | circuitry play a very important role
01:36:18.680 | in what behaviors you choose to do, pursue,
01:36:22.980 | play a very important role in these making the decision
01:36:26.940 | and performing these pro-social, non-aggressive,
01:36:30.680 | non-sexual interactions.
01:36:32.740 | I actually also think it plays a role in empathy
01:36:35.740 | and I'm leading you there.
01:36:37.420 | I wanna have a discussion about that.
01:36:39.500 | - Please.
01:36:40.900 | - Again, as a mechanistically driven neuroscientist,
01:36:45.480 | what is frustrating for me is I know a lot of the connections
01:36:49.740 | it's making and the other brain areas
01:36:51.800 | it's communicating with, but I can't give you
01:36:55.060 | a coherent hypothesis or diagram of how it all happens.
01:37:00.060 | - Yet, yet, you're still going.
01:37:03.860 | - What I can say is even at our current level
01:37:07.780 | of understanding, it is leading to novel hypotheses
01:37:12.780 | that are allowing the, if we bring it back to autism,
01:37:19.160 | that are allowing the development of novel at the moment,
01:37:25.420 | pharmacologic therapeutics that might be helpful
01:37:30.180 | for people who are not having normal pro-social interactions
01:37:35.180 | and would like to have them, would like to be able
01:37:39.260 | to function in that domain in a more adaptive
01:37:44.260 | and productive and meaningful way.
01:37:47.080 | And that's the importance in my view
01:37:49.780 | of the kind of mechanistic work my lab
01:37:51.980 | and many other labs around the country are doing.
01:37:54.360 | Even if we don't have a detailed understanding
01:37:58.320 | of how it's all happening, we can identify drugs
01:38:02.400 | and druggable targets or even behavioral interventions
01:38:06.140 | that might actually help people.
01:38:08.420 | For example, suffering from autism spectrum disorder
01:38:13.420 | of the sort that they actually want and need therapeutic help.
01:38:19.100 | - I think looking at the social connection circuitry
01:38:25.380 | through the lens of autism is going to be very interesting
01:38:27.620 | for us to do.
01:38:28.460 | I do have a question about what is being selected for
01:38:31.980 | in rewarding social interactions?
01:38:34.400 | Because obviously we are living in a time
01:38:37.340 | where we don't have to aggregate in groups necessarily
01:38:40.980 | to protect ourselves physically.
01:38:43.580 | It helps in certain ways and certain circumstances,
01:38:46.980 | but certainly to support ourselves and each other emotionally
01:38:51.980 | having people that we can call on when we're not feeling
01:38:54.180 | so well that we can look to for resources
01:38:57.700 | and that they can look to us.
01:38:59.380 | But when we go out to dinner with friends
01:39:00.980 | or we go to a ball game with friends
01:39:02.220 | or we interact with friends, I'm very familiar
01:39:03.980 | with the feeling of like, well, that felt really good.
01:39:05.860 | It just felt good.
01:39:06.700 | It gives me energy.
01:39:07.800 | It actually gives me energy to go back and do other things
01:39:10.300 | like spend four days alone with a bunch of papers
01:39:13.580 | and lectures preparing for a podcast,
01:39:15.180 | which I also really enjoy.
01:39:16.720 | But when I do that, when I go out to dinner with friends
01:39:20.700 | or see friends, I'm not thinking about buffering myself
01:39:23.180 | against loneliness when I do it.
01:39:24.820 | I just like the interaction.
01:39:26.980 | So what sorts of evolutionary hypotheses
01:39:31.980 | can we come up with as to why the human brain
01:39:36.540 | is so tuned for these social interactions?
01:39:39.540 | Why it's rewarded by not just one dopamine, but also--
01:39:43.300 | - Serotonin. - Serotonin and oxytocin.
01:39:45.500 | Three prominent neuromodulatory chemicals in the brain
01:39:50.200 | are devoted at one site in the brain and others
01:39:53.500 | that it's connected to, of course,
01:39:54.640 | but to making sure that we do this as often as possible
01:39:59.060 | without giving up the rest of our lives.
01:40:01.180 | - Well, I mean, again, I think the answer
01:40:03.300 | I'm gonna be able to give, I hope it's not trite
01:40:06.080 | and it may be a little bit obvious, is,
01:40:09.080 | and in some ways, it's analogous
01:40:12.580 | to why drugs of abuse and addiction
01:40:18.940 | are also a problem is that the circuitry
01:40:22.880 | that is telling us a prosocial positive interaction
01:40:27.720 | is so highly reinforcing evolved over millions of years
01:40:32.520 | or hundreds of thousands of years, whatever that is.
01:40:36.060 | And the only hypothesis I can come up with,
01:40:38.680 | and Andrew, you may be able to come up with better ones,
01:40:41.260 | is what I alluded to earlier is that it was very adaptive
01:40:46.020 | when we were more primitive organisms,
01:40:47.980 | nevermind non-human primates,
01:40:49.740 | but when we were whatever we were to be a social species
01:40:54.740 | for basically primarily two reasons.
01:41:00.100 | For reproductive purposes, it increased your likelihood
01:41:05.100 | of reproducing if you were hanging out
01:41:08.260 | with other members of your species in a non-aggressive way
01:41:12.780 | and for protection against predators.
01:41:16.100 | And there may be other reasons.
01:41:18.300 | - Probably child-rearing, too.
01:41:19.500 | - Oh yeah, thank you.
01:41:20.340 | - In your absence, you want trusted friends
01:41:22.760 | that can watch your offspring.
01:41:24.020 | - Thank you, very good point.
01:41:26.420 | So the circuits, the modulators we use,
01:41:29.460 | that evolved over millennia, and as you pointed out,
01:41:32.800 | eventually, I mean, depending on the society
01:41:38.960 | in which you live, you didn't need those social interactions
01:41:43.820 | for protection against predators, although, you know,
01:41:48.340 | if we look at our world now,
01:41:49.980 | one can make arguments both ways.
01:41:52.060 | If you're in a war zone, is it better to be off by yourself?
01:41:54.900 | Is it better to be with a group of people?
01:41:57.780 | But so the mechanisms evolved for one purpose,
01:42:02.940 | and they don't just disappear,
01:42:05.120 | because there's no disadvantage to having this mechanism
01:42:09.620 | that tells us a social interaction is reinforcing.
01:42:12.660 | And I would still argue there's benefit
01:42:14.500 | for reproductive purposes.
01:42:17.060 | You can't have kids if you're by yourself all the time.
01:42:20.700 | - Well, and this is actually--
01:42:21.540 | - I think it's impossible, at least currently,
01:42:23.660 | and you can't find a partner with whom to have kids
01:42:27.300 | if you're socially isolated, or it makes it much harder.
01:42:30.480 | So I hope I'm answering your question.
01:42:32.020 | I think, and then as you pointed out,
01:42:39.380 | for many of us, there's a lot of positive aspects
01:42:43.340 | to having friendships and hanging out with your friends.
01:42:46.420 | Emotional support, emotional buffering.
01:42:49.540 | - And feeling connected.
01:42:50.620 | There's something about this notion of feeling connected,
01:42:54.080 | and later we'll talk about psychedelics,
01:42:55.620 | but this notion of feeling connected has a lot to do
01:42:59.580 | with buffering loneliness when we are alone.
01:43:02.700 | The memories and the energy, for lack of a better word,
01:43:06.620 | that we feel in recalling social experiences
01:43:10.220 | and anticipating social experiences is really powerful.
01:43:13.620 | You mentioned that people can't have children
01:43:18.040 | if they spend all their time alone.
01:43:19.220 | It's actually, I realize you're not on social media
01:43:21.500 | and more power to you,
01:43:23.300 | but this is actually a prominent discussion on social media.
01:43:25.940 | There's an entire culture of young people,
01:43:29.200 | in particular young men these days,
01:43:30.720 | who, at least from what I understand
01:43:33.220 | in the research literature about this,
01:43:35.600 | are socially isolated, spending all their time online,
01:43:39.660 | maybe not even on social media,
01:43:41.380 | but are spending a lot of time online,
01:43:42.860 | video games hiding in electronic landscapes,
01:43:46.860 | digital landscapes.
01:43:48.060 | And concern about mental health issues there, et cetera.
01:43:53.520 | Concern about porn overuse and addiction there, et cetera.
01:43:57.140 | But social media itself is an incredible phenomenon
01:44:01.420 | to consider in light of everything we're talking about.
01:44:05.320 | I can't say, even though I am on all social media platforms
01:44:09.860 | and I'm quite active there,
01:44:11.960 | I can't say that I've ever been on social media
01:44:15.380 | and experienced the kind of delight and thrill
01:44:18.780 | and persistent energy increase
01:44:22.420 | that I experienced with in-person interaction.
01:44:24.420 | And yet social media, I have to assume,
01:44:27.040 | is capitalizing on some of these same reward mechanisms
01:44:30.420 | in presumably the nucleus accumbens.
01:44:32.900 | So are there any data,
01:44:34.660 | I realize this is a hard experiment to do in mice,
01:44:36.680 | maybe impossible.
01:44:37.880 | Are there any data that you're aware of
01:44:39.580 | that shows that social media has a high addictive liability
01:44:44.580 | or do we even need an experiment?
01:44:46.580 | - I'm not sure we need an experiment.
01:44:49.560 | I think it clearly does.
01:44:52.360 | I agree with the point you're making,
01:44:54.040 | although your podcast audience probably
01:44:57.360 | doesn't know who I am.
01:44:58.680 | I am in my late sixties.
01:45:00.660 | I grew up--
01:45:01.500 | - Well, they know who you are now.
01:45:02.400 | - I grew up before computers, before cell phones.
01:45:07.400 | So I still am a believer, perhaps in an old fashioned way,
01:45:13.320 | that physical interpersonal reactions are really important.
01:45:18.660 | Obviously there are advantages to being able to interact
01:45:23.660 | over social media.
01:45:25.700 | And I mean, for all sorts of reasons,
01:45:28.760 | there's a lot of positive and good from that.
01:45:31.420 | But back to your question, can we get addicted?
01:45:33.740 | I can't speak to social media.
01:45:36.140 | I can speak, and Anna Lemke, I think is much more able
01:45:41.140 | to eloquently describe the issues around here.
01:45:44.140 | I can just talk from my own experience
01:45:46.540 | that my cell phone is,
01:45:50.020 | this isn't social media, but checking my email messages,
01:45:54.860 | checking my text messages,
01:45:59.480 | for me has a compulsive addictive quality to it.
01:46:03.020 | - It's like a lever press.
01:46:03.900 | - It's like a lever press for a mouse.
01:46:06.240 | And part of that is my own personality.
01:46:10.460 | Part of that is the immediate feedback.
01:46:14.860 | So you get from a social media post,
01:46:18.980 | from seeing your name mentioned,
01:46:20.600 | getting a message from one of your friends.
01:46:23.260 | Sure, I like getting messages from my friends.
01:46:26.900 | It means they're thinking about me.
01:46:28.600 | It means I'm part of their world.
01:46:31.860 | I have no doubt it's activating my reward circuitry,
01:46:36.100 | not nearly to the degree that a hit of cocaine
01:46:40.180 | or an opioid would do.
01:46:42.240 | So I don't know what else to say about it.
01:46:48.540 | I think as a society, we have to be aware of these issues,
01:46:56.580 | and it's really complicated how we manage,
01:46:59.760 | especially once you're an adult,
01:47:01.780 | you make your own decisions for better or worse,
01:47:04.780 | but it's a huge issue obviously for anybody
01:47:08.260 | who has children or is planning to have children.
01:47:11.320 | - And it halts on social media.
01:47:13.180 | I see lots of accounts of people that are 18 and older
01:47:16.940 | who they spent a lot of time on there.
01:47:19.220 | And I'm not necessarily saying that's a bad thing.
01:47:21.420 | A lot of people have entire careers
01:47:22.660 | that exist on social media.
01:47:23.820 | It just seems to me that Instagram, Facebook,
01:47:27.820 | LinkedIn, Twitter have capitalized
01:47:30.260 | on this hardwired circuitry.
01:47:33.740 | The release of, I mean, to make it really reductionist,
01:47:35.940 | the release of serotonin, dopamine, and oxytocin
01:47:38.700 | by virtue of someone saying something to us,
01:47:41.380 | maybe not even a positive thing.
01:47:43.340 | Maybe it's a negative thing.
01:47:45.740 | As you said, they're thinking of us.
01:47:47.180 | There's something about being recognized by others.
01:47:49.940 | And maybe this is a good segue,
01:47:51.100 | we're heading towards empathy here,
01:47:52.820 | a discussion about empathy.
01:47:53.900 | - I think that's very well put.
01:47:57.160 | It is capitalizing on these more primitive
01:48:01.120 | neurobiological mechanisms that evolved
01:48:03.900 | for purposes of reproduction and survival.
01:48:07.460 | I think that certainly has to be the case.
01:48:11.060 | And I think it's important,
01:48:13.860 | I mean, thank you for bringing that up,
01:48:15.120 | for us as a society to be at least aware of this.
01:48:20.120 | And it doesn't mean, it's like many things.
01:48:22.340 | It's not all good, it's not all bad.
01:48:24.780 | It has, there are positive uses of social media, I can see.
01:48:28.840 | But mostly we read about the dangers of it.
01:48:33.420 | We read about these kids who are socially isolated,
01:48:37.020 | who make bad decisions based on what they're seeing
01:48:39.820 | with social media.
01:48:41.340 | But anyhow, back to the neuroscience,
01:48:43.860 | you're absolutely correct.
01:48:45.980 | It's capitalizing on these mechanisms
01:48:49.800 | that evolved for physical interpersonal interactions,
01:48:54.800 | because our evolution didn't anticipate it.
01:48:58.540 | - Right, just as pornography is capitalizing
01:49:01.020 | on the sexual arousal reward circuit,
01:49:03.180 | associated reward circuit.
01:49:04.020 | - No question about it, just as the gambling industry does.
01:49:07.980 | I mean, as you know, the Vegas casinos
01:49:11.340 | have full-time people developing algorithms
01:49:15.340 | for how frequently should a slot machine pay off?
01:49:19.920 | What's the perfect amount of payoff
01:49:22.320 | to keep certain individuals coming back?
01:49:25.800 | - So pernicious, someone, you could tell
01:49:28.800 | I've been spending a lot of time around addicts
01:49:30.880 | and former addicts.
01:49:31.720 | I've been researching some things for the podcast.
01:49:34.960 | And a gambling addict told me something interesting.
01:49:38.680 | They said, the real stinger with being a gambling addict
01:49:42.320 | is that the next time really could change everything.
01:49:45.600 | Whereas no alcoholic says that,
01:49:47.180 | that the next drink could change everything for the better.
01:49:49.520 | Or the cocaine addict doesn't think,
01:49:51.980 | oh, the next line of cocaine
01:49:53.820 | could make all of life better now and forever.
01:49:56.480 | Whereas the gambling addict actually holds in mind
01:49:59.180 | the infinitesimally small and yet real potential
01:50:04.000 | that the next time really could wipe out their debt
01:50:06.700 | and perhaps wipe out their,
01:50:08.520 | and yet we know they would lose that too, right?
01:50:10.640 | Whatever winnings they have.
01:50:11.480 | And casinos are fully aware of this.
01:50:14.360 | I have been told by friends who know,
01:50:16.680 | they employ full-time quantitative,
01:50:21.680 | for lack of a better term,
01:50:25.020 | well, I was gonna say computer geek.
01:50:27.360 | I don't mean to that be an innate derogator.
01:50:30.040 | - And probably neuroscientists too.
01:50:32.120 | - I would be amazed if they don't have neuroscientists
01:50:34.720 | who have expertise in what's called neuroeconomics
01:50:37.960 | or behavioral economics.
01:50:39.840 | I'm 95% sure that has to be the case.
01:50:44.840 | - I occasionally sit down to the roulette table
01:50:47.360 | 'cause I just, it's so passive and easy.
01:50:49.180 | And not long ago, actually,
01:50:51.160 | I had the experience of winning a fairly,
01:50:54.000 | not a large sum, but a meaningful sum of money.
01:50:56.960 | And I'll tell you, my sole mission at that point
01:51:00.000 | was to get up and go back to my room
01:51:02.480 | and not stop at another table.
01:51:04.500 | And I confess, I pulled one brief stop at another table,
01:51:07.540 | played it one hand and then lost it.
01:51:09.360 | And then just got back to my room as quickly as possible
01:51:11.400 | and then left Las Vegas as quickly as possible.
01:51:14.260 | - Gambling is fascinating.
01:51:15.100 | - But they'll probably get me the next time.
01:51:16.840 | - Gambling is, again, it all gets back
01:51:19.720 | to this reward circuitry.
01:51:21.480 | And the intermittent rewards are very, very powerful.
01:51:26.480 | - Well, and you mentioned earlier
01:51:28.400 | that the reward system is powerfully tuned to remember
01:51:30.920 | what were the behaviors that led up
01:51:32.360 | to the rewarding experience.
01:51:34.040 | And nobody ever won at the roulette or craps table
01:51:38.080 | or poker table by getting up and leaving, right?
01:51:41.920 | And so I guess my brain was just thinking,
01:51:43.720 | well, how did I win?
01:51:44.560 | I won by sitting down and putting chips on the table,
01:51:47.560 | not by going back to my room.
01:51:49.020 | - Exactly, exactly.
01:51:50.280 | - And yet I have a fair number of degrees
01:51:53.340 | and I like to think my prefrontal cortex is working
01:51:55.440 | and yet it was still challenging in that moment.
01:51:58.160 | - Gambling is really, I mean, yeah,
01:52:00.720 | another human activity that's quite complicated.
01:52:06.600 | It can be enjoyable or it can be incredibly damaging.
01:52:09.880 | - And now people are gonna think I was that gambling addict
01:52:12.240 | that I was referring to, but I swear I'm not.
01:52:13.880 | Fortunately, I feel very blessed that that's not my addiction.
01:52:16.920 | I'd like to talk about empathy and use that as a framework
01:52:21.920 | for eventually returning to our discussion of autism.
01:52:24.120 | But you have this perhaps longstanding interest,
01:52:27.580 | but recent research interest in empathy.
01:52:31.560 | Tell me about this work.
01:52:32.400 | I'm not familiar with it yet.
01:52:33.320 | - Okay, so I'm gonna, I hope it's okay,
01:52:37.840 | drag in some work I've done on this drug called MDMA
01:52:41.320 | because it is related.
01:52:42.720 | So we were working on in my lab, social behaviors,
01:52:48.520 | positive pro-social behaviors that stimulated me
01:52:53.520 | to start thinking about what are components
01:52:56.440 | of a positive pro-social non-aggressive interaction.
01:53:01.320 | A common key component of that is having some empathy
01:53:06.320 | and compassion for the individuals you're hanging out with.
01:53:13.000 | And it is a topic I've been interested in
01:53:15.260 | for many, many decades.
01:53:17.300 | I was once a psychiatrist and to get on my,
01:53:22.080 | whatever the word is, hobby horse, I look at the world today.
01:53:26.680 | I try to be optimistic.
01:53:29.440 | Again, I am a child of the '60s and '70s.
01:53:32.440 | When I look at the world, and I actually just did a trip
01:53:37.080 | to Israel to give a series of lectures,
01:53:39.320 | and I look at the Israeli-Palestinian conflict,
01:53:42.200 | what always enters my mind,
01:53:43.680 | and I've felt this way for decades,
01:53:45.800 | is what is more important for the survival
01:53:48.920 | of the human species than empathy and compassion,
01:53:52.720 | than actually being able to look at another human being,
01:53:56.760 | even if they look different than you,
01:53:58.880 | even if they have a different belief system than you?
01:54:02.520 | What is more important than actually understanding
01:54:05.720 | that 98% of your life is very similar?
01:54:10.720 | You have some differences in how you look
01:54:15.380 | and the beliefs you have, but there's so much in common.
01:54:18.380 | So what's more important than understanding
01:54:20.600 | that when another person is suffering,
01:54:23.480 | they're suffering, it's the same as your suffering,
01:54:27.700 | and having compassion for somebody.
01:54:29.440 | So I started thinking, what is more important?
01:54:31.720 | And I'm not a politician.
01:54:33.400 | As you know, Andrew, I have no social media presence.
01:54:36.760 | I figured the only way I might be able to contribute
01:54:39.840 | to efforts that might help the human species
01:54:46.120 | enhance empathy and compassion
01:54:48.380 | is by studying the neurobiological underpinnings of it.
01:54:53.120 | And I didn't realize I might be able to do that
01:54:55.880 | until I started studying sociability
01:54:59.640 | or prosocial behaviors in mice.
01:55:02.500 | And then I was able to have a young woman scientist,
01:55:07.500 | and I want to give her credit, Monique Smith.
01:55:14.940 | You might want to have Monique on your podcast.
01:55:16.900 | She's a dynamo.
01:55:18.220 | She's now an assistant professor at UCSD,
01:55:22.040 | where you once was.
01:55:23.160 | And Monique introduced me to a series of behavioral assays
01:55:28.160 | that I like to use the phrase,
01:55:33.240 | they are measurements,
01:55:34.240 | they are behavioral antecedents of empathy.
01:55:38.080 | Because in the world of psychologists
01:55:41.420 | and people who use the term empathy,
01:55:43.360 | it has a lot of different meanings to different people.
01:55:46.680 | I'm using it basically to mean one member of a species
01:55:52.020 | manifests some behavior that indicates
01:55:55.900 | it is being influenced by the emotional state
01:56:00.500 | or what we call the affective state,
01:56:02.860 | affective with an A,
01:56:04.360 | of another member of that species
01:56:07.360 | in its immediate environment.
01:56:09.080 | For human interactions, I just think of,
01:56:14.000 | we were talking about friendships.
01:56:17.080 | Any of us who watch a close friend suffer, it's hard.
01:56:21.880 | You want to do anything you can to help them.
01:56:24.020 | That's empathy.
01:56:25.240 | A mother with their child, a good mother, hopefully.
01:56:28.680 | When you have a kid who is sick,
01:56:31.320 | there's nothing worse as a parent.
01:56:33.040 | You just want to take that pain and suffering away.
01:56:36.000 | That's how I'm defining empathy.
01:56:37.900 | So it's my belief that like any complex human behavior,
01:56:42.900 | there are evolutionary reasons why that has been adaptive
01:56:48.020 | and important and maintained.
01:56:49.860 | And if it's evolutionarily evolved,
01:56:54.480 | there are ways of studying it
01:56:55.800 | in more primitive organisms like mice.
01:56:59.220 | So I'll tell you some of the behavioral assays we're doing.
01:57:02.640 | One is, and I get a kick out of this,
01:57:06.420 | because it's pretty new for me.
01:57:09.720 | So one assay, and we published a paper
01:57:11.820 | in a journal called Science about this,
01:57:15.480 | which is if you take one mouse and in a ethical way,
01:57:20.420 | you put it in pain, you make its hind paw,
01:57:23.660 | one of its paws, one of its feet hurt a modest amount,
01:57:27.780 | and you take another mouse and you let that,
01:57:29.960 | what's known as the bystander mouse,
01:57:32.100 | just hang out with the mouse that's in pain for one hour,
01:57:36.140 | just one hour, the bystander mouse,
01:57:38.740 | who has experienced no physical injury whatsoever,
01:57:43.060 | will manifest behaviors indicating it is now in pain.
01:57:47.840 | And it lasts maybe four to 20 hours.
01:57:50.620 | But think about that.
01:57:51.900 | A mouse that is normal,
01:57:54.920 | hanging out with another mouse in pain,
01:57:56.940 | starts feeling pain itself.
01:57:59.700 | - And the mice are able to see one another
01:58:04.200 | and hear one another?
01:58:05.360 | - Good point.
01:58:06.200 | So you're getting to how is that communication happening?
01:58:10.260 | And a lot more work needs to be done on it.
01:58:14.780 | Monique and her previous colleagues and others,
01:58:17.440 | one component of it is probably an olfactory cue
01:58:22.540 | or what we call a pheromone.
01:58:24.220 | - So the mouse that's in pain is secreting an ovary?
01:58:26.580 | - Probably, probably,
01:58:27.780 | because you can take bedding from mice in pain
01:58:31.580 | and expose the bystander mice.
01:58:33.740 | So that's one thing.
01:58:34.580 | And I had never heard of these behavioral assays.
01:58:37.420 | We developed our, and this is pretty cool,
01:58:39.420 | and then I'll tell you two others,
01:58:40.580 | and then I'll tell you how it connects to reward circuitry.
01:58:43.420 | We developed a novel assay,
01:58:46.700 | which is the social transfer of pain relief.
01:58:51.100 | Pain relief is called analgesia.
01:58:54.120 | And I thought this was pretty cool.
01:58:56.040 | So you take, and this is in this paper
01:58:59.200 | that was published in Science a year ago,
01:59:01.900 | you take two mice and they're both in pain, modest pain.
01:59:06.580 | I don't want your listeners to get upset.
01:59:08.560 | We are not hurting these mice too badly.
01:59:12.340 | And it is a tricky issue.
01:59:13.620 | Is it okay to put a mouse in pain so you can,
01:59:17.100 | the goal is to develop better treatments
01:59:19.000 | for human beings in pain, obviously.
01:59:22.340 | So you have two mice in modest pain.
01:59:24.760 | You give one mouse morphine, so it's now analgesic.
01:59:29.760 | It is no longer experiencing pain.
01:59:32.720 | You take another mouse that's in pain
01:59:35.420 | and you just let it hang out with the mouse
01:59:39.000 | that is no longer in pain.
01:59:40.940 | And the mouse that is in pain will show behaviors
01:59:44.620 | indicating it is experiencing analgesia.
01:59:48.180 | It is no longer in as much pain.
01:59:50.340 | Now think about that.
01:59:51.440 | And there's actually evidence from human studies
01:59:54.260 | that I can't speak to in any comprehensive way
01:59:59.260 | where, I mean, it's called social buffering of pain.
02:00:02.920 | If you are, I mean, to be honest,
02:00:05.140 | I've been having some neck pain just because I'm an old guy
02:00:08.860 | and I woke up on the wrong side of the bed.
02:00:11.500 | And if I'm by myself, I focus on that pain
02:00:15.940 | and it bothers me more.
02:00:17.260 | If I'm in a socially engaged, I think it's not only that,
02:00:21.940 | I'm not paying as much attention to the pain,
02:00:23.820 | but I think there's actually some relief
02:00:26.740 | from what's known as the social buffering of pain.
02:00:29.700 | - Well, I'm no hippie, but I actually think that
02:00:33.500 | all species, including humans, are secreting molecules,
02:00:37.060 | mainly odorants that are perhaps even acting
02:00:40.860 | directly as analgesics.
02:00:43.140 | And I can make that statement without ordering too much
02:00:46.880 | that people think I'm completely crazy
02:00:49.040 | because we had a Noam Sobol on the podcast
02:00:51.940 | from the Weizmann who shared with us, you know,
02:00:55.260 | not one, not two, but at least a dozen ways
02:00:58.220 | in which humans are making molecules, typically odors,
02:01:03.460 | and communicating those to one another
02:01:05.460 | to powerfully impact their testosterone levels,
02:01:09.060 | their vasopressin levels, their immune molecules, you know?
02:01:12.740 | And of course, Noam works on olfaction,
02:01:15.020 | so he's going to be biased toward that system,
02:01:16.580 | but that's just one slice of the sensory array.
02:01:19.420 | I mean, what about the way that somebody can look at us
02:01:22.700 | in a way that makes us feel good on a normal day?
02:01:25.020 | Well, when we're in pain,
02:01:26.300 | just even the touch to a shoulder can mean a lot.
02:01:28.540 | I remember going to meetings
02:01:29.620 | when I was a early neuroscientist
02:01:31.940 | and I would probably at that point have, you know,
02:01:36.940 | not been the type to just walk up and say hello to you
02:01:39.520 | 'cause I wasn't in your field
02:01:40.580 | and you're this luminary and stuff.
02:01:41.900 | But I remember as I started-
02:01:43.180 | - I'm a good guy, by the way.
02:01:44.220 | - You are, very, very good.
02:01:46.620 | - I always say hi to everybody.
02:01:48.020 | - I know you are.
02:01:48.940 | And that statement was a reflection on me,
02:01:51.300 | not a reflection on you.
02:01:52.540 | But as I advanced through my career,
02:01:53.960 | what I found was, you know, you'd give a talk or something
02:01:56.060 | and someone in your field more senior to you
02:01:58.060 | who you respected would give a nod or something.
02:02:00.740 | Those nods meant a lot.
02:02:02.300 | - Yeah, absolutely.
02:02:03.140 | - I mean, those nods could carry you a long distance.
02:02:05.260 | I mean, obviously we wanna be intrinsically driven
02:02:07.140 | to do the work we do, but the social communication stuff-
02:02:09.820 | - We're social species.
02:02:10.740 | - I think there's a whole landscape of things.
02:02:13.740 | So what you're describing is incredible,
02:02:16.660 | but I think makes a ton of sense.
02:02:18.940 | - Yeah, so we have these social transfer pain
02:02:20.860 | of analogies here.
02:02:22.020 | We're working on, and there's a little bit of evidence
02:02:25.380 | in the literature suggesting this might work.
02:02:28.860 | And then I'll talk about reward circuitry
02:02:31.300 | and maybe MDMA and is it an empathogen or not
02:02:35.220 | and how that might influence therapeutic efforts for autism.
02:02:39.220 | We're working on behavioral models.
02:02:41.300 | We're asking the question, will one mouse behave
02:02:46.300 | to give another mouse a reward?
02:02:49.180 | So it's the mouse that's behaving,
02:02:50.960 | that has to press a bar or nose poke
02:02:53.880 | or even experience a shock.
02:02:55.740 | Will the mouse do that simply to give one
02:02:58.820 | of its buddies a reward?
02:03:00.780 | - Pure altruism.
02:03:01.840 | - And yeah, it's pure, it's what we call a generosity,
02:03:05.060 | a generosity assay.
02:03:07.020 | In early days, it looks like it might be working.
02:03:09.640 | And that's a generosity assay.
02:03:13.740 | We can also ask the question, will a mouse work
02:03:16.540 | so another mouse doesn't get a shock, doesn't get hurt,
02:03:20.220 | which is compassion.
02:03:21.820 | And I think these things are gonna be working.
02:03:24.000 | And whether you wanna call that empathy,
02:03:27.580 | I would call that, those are behaviors,
02:03:29.800 | I like to use the term behavioral antecedents
02:03:32.600 | of how we define empathy in human beings
02:03:35.820 | and the connection to reward circuitry.
02:03:37.660 | And in the little bit of work we have done on this
02:03:42.660 | is we presented evidence that these behaviors,
02:03:47.780 | we call the social transfer of pain,
02:03:50.740 | one mouse experiencing pain just because it's hanging out
02:03:53.500 | with another mouse, the social transfer of analgesia,
02:03:57.460 | a mouse in pain getting some pain relief
02:04:00.260 | from hanging out with another mouse in pain
02:04:03.780 | who has that pain relief.
02:04:05.900 | It seems to involve one component
02:04:08.660 | of the complex brain mechanisms,
02:04:11.140 | seems to involve a part of the brain
02:04:14.760 | called the anterior cingulate cortex,
02:04:17.540 | which human brain imaging studies suggest is activated
02:04:22.540 | during empathic human responses.
02:04:26.220 | And the projections of that area into the nucleus accumbens,
02:04:30.340 | that's the connection.
02:04:31.580 | And we're interested in whether neuromodulators
02:04:37.380 | like dopamine and serotonin may influence these circuitry,
02:04:42.380 | these connections that are involved in these,
02:04:47.500 | in quotes, empathic behaviors, et cetera, et cetera.
02:04:51.880 | And we think drugs can be used as probes
02:04:54.540 | of those kinds of neuromodulatory mechanisms.
02:04:57.020 | I hope this is all making sense.
02:04:58.360 | - Yeah, it makes excellent sense and it's fascinating.
02:05:01.380 | I'm not one to suggest experiments to colleagues
02:05:05.020 | in areas where I don't work, but I'm going to anyway.
02:05:07.700 | - Yeah, please, you're a really smart guy,
02:05:10.780 | so I will value your suggestions.
02:05:13.900 | - You know, I love the motivational backbone
02:05:16.280 | to what you're describing here because I agree,
02:05:18.360 | the world has a lot of issues
02:05:20.060 | and what could be more important
02:05:21.480 | than to increase the amount of empathy and compassion
02:05:24.500 | in the world.
02:05:25.880 | But one thing that we know inhibits empathy and compassion
02:05:29.800 | is one's own challenges and struggles.
02:05:32.740 | And so I'm wondering if there's a way to introduce something
02:05:34.860 | to this behavioral paradigm such that the working
02:05:39.820 | to provide another animal relief from pain,
02:05:42.460 | one animal working to provide relief
02:05:44.500 | of another animal in pain or a animal working
02:05:47.860 | to provide pleasure, reward for another animal.
02:05:51.000 | You know, if it could be scaled
02:05:52.580 | with how inconvenient that work is, right?
02:05:55.420 | Like if I'm very hungry, I mean, we're all taught
02:05:57.440 | to put our own oxygen mask on first in some way too,
02:06:00.720 | so that we don't all die, so to speak.
02:06:03.340 | But you know, I grew up, for instance, with one parent.
02:06:07.060 | My mother was the kind of person who would see,
02:06:09.300 | at that time, there were far fewer homeless people
02:06:11.540 | on the street.
02:06:12.380 | Maybe they were all institutionalized, I don't know.
02:06:15.200 | But if she saw a homeless person on the street
02:06:18.740 | of the town we lived in, she would literally pull over,
02:06:21.780 | give them money, find hotels.
02:06:23.860 | She had homeless people living in hotels
02:06:25.940 | all over the town we lived in.
02:06:27.460 | It was crazy.
02:06:28.460 | I mean, we couldn't get anywhere.
02:06:29.700 | That was the problem, is we would never arrive anywhere
02:06:31.580 | on time, and that's my excuse for always being late.
02:06:33.580 | I would positively reinforce her being late.
02:06:35.460 | I always run late and I always run, incredible, right?
02:06:37.980 | Just a very strong sense of social connection,
02:06:41.060 | that kind of thing.
02:06:42.060 | But in any case, you know, some people are like that.
02:06:44.380 | Like she could not experience any even modicum
02:06:48.740 | of inconvenience for helping others.
02:06:51.960 | Whereas I think most of us feel like if I'm rushing
02:06:54.340 | to catch a flight and I see someone who's struggling,
02:06:56.820 | I'm probably gonna help them if they're in acute pain
02:06:58.880 | or it seems like a dire circumstance.
02:07:00.900 | But let's be honest.
02:07:02.100 | Most people are probably gonna prioritize
02:07:06.020 | their own stress and priorities, for lack of a better word,
02:07:11.020 | when the situation often calls for us to set those aside
02:07:15.620 | and tend to people that are suffering.
02:07:17.500 | So if there was a way to introduce a probe
02:07:21.360 | of the interplay of circuitries that involve
02:07:24.060 | how convenient or inconvenient it is,
02:07:25.780 | like if we're well fed, it's pretty easy to go out
02:07:28.120 | and gather and distribute food for others.
02:07:30.300 | But if we're hungry, we tend to focus on our own hunger.
02:07:33.820 | - So first, you know, in full disclosure,
02:07:38.220 | even though I'm studying empathy and compassion,
02:07:40.380 | I can look in the mirror and say,
02:07:42.160 | I probably don't practice it nearly as much as I should.
02:07:45.360 | I'm thinking of your example.
02:07:46.680 | If I was late for a plane,
02:07:48.980 | I'm not sure I would stop and help somebody.
02:07:51.180 | And I'm not saying that--
02:07:52.020 | - I guess it depends on what sort of suffering.
02:07:53.500 | - Yeah, exactly.
02:07:54.340 | - I mean, if they're hemorrhaging on the side of the road,
02:07:56.180 | we all would. - Of course, of course.
02:07:57.020 | - But if I'm tired, right, you might think,
02:07:58.940 | oh goodness, like, do I have time for this?
02:08:00.740 | - Yeah, yeah, exactly.
02:08:01.580 | And so I'm not proud of that statement.
02:08:03.520 | But back to your question.
02:08:05.040 | Yes, I think absolutely we can design experiments
02:08:09.640 | where after we've established the basic phenomenology,
02:08:13.640 | then we can take our subject, animal or mouse,
02:08:17.880 | and put it into certain circumstances.
02:08:21.360 | If it's hungry itself,
02:08:23.240 | will it work as hard to give another animal?
02:08:25.600 | I mean, it's a good question
02:08:26.920 | because I'm not sure what the outcome will be.
02:08:28.900 | One could predict it might work harder
02:08:30.800 | because it understands the hunger, in quotes, more.
02:08:34.160 | - I love that answer. - Or it could be,
02:08:36.540 | of course it's not gonna work hard for another animal
02:08:39.480 | to get a food reward because it's starving itself
02:08:42.680 | and it needs to take care of itself first.
02:08:45.040 | It's a great question.
02:08:46.040 | We're also asking questions about,
02:08:48.220 | do you have to know your buddy mouse, right?
02:08:50.920 | Do you, is it, are you more likely to behave
02:08:53.740 | in a generous or compassionate way
02:08:56.400 | if you grew up with that mouse, you know,
02:08:59.260 | in the way our mice grow up in academic environments?
02:09:03.960 | And if it's a stranger, how will you behave?
02:09:07.300 | How will you behave if you had a fight
02:09:09.200 | with that mouse previously?
02:09:11.360 | And what if you had, and it also matters,
02:09:14.960 | did you win the fight or did you lose the fight, right?
02:09:18.760 | Intuitively, as we probably would all guess,
02:09:21.760 | I'm more likely to help somebody
02:09:23.520 | I defeated in a fight previously
02:09:26.840 | because I'm the, in the hierarchy, I'm the dominant one.
02:09:31.400 | I'm probably less likely if that person beat me up.
02:09:34.920 | So all these are great questions.
02:09:37.160 | I think we can study them.
02:09:39.480 | I also think there are ways we can study
02:09:41.720 | these kinds of interactions in human subjects.
02:09:45.260 | Not that I am gonna do that myself.
02:09:47.120 | - But someone at Stanford will.
02:09:48.220 | - Yeah, yeah.
02:09:49.080 | So I think there's also an opportunity,
02:09:51.120 | and I'm happy to discuss how neuromodulators,
02:09:53.920 | like in particular serotonin,
02:09:56.080 | but also perhaps dopamine and oxytocin
02:09:59.000 | may influence the brain, the circuitry
02:10:01.960 | and the brain mechanisms that are mediating
02:10:04.760 | what I term empathic behaviors.
02:10:07.680 | - Let's return to autism.
02:10:09.040 | - Right.
02:10:09.880 | - Does autism involve a lack of empathy?
02:10:14.760 | Does autism involve a restructuring of the reward system
02:10:18.900 | around social interactions?
02:10:21.260 | Maybe considering the second question first,
02:10:24.160 | I could imagine, for instance,
02:10:25.880 | that there are variations in brain wiring
02:10:28.480 | that would make it such that a kid
02:10:31.840 | who then becomes an adult gets a tremendous amount of reward
02:10:35.520 | from, I don't know, math, designing mugs,
02:10:40.240 | any number of activities,
02:10:43.040 | but that through some variation in brain wiring,
02:10:46.740 | social interaction, spending time with friends
02:10:49.640 | is just not as socially rewarding.
02:10:51.480 | It just doesn't feel good in the moment,
02:10:54.360 | doesn't necessarily feel bad,
02:10:56.240 | but it's not selected for.
02:10:59.040 | And is there any evidence that that's the case
02:11:01.840 | in children who are classified as autistic
02:11:06.120 | or having autism?
02:11:07.120 | - I wanna be clear.
02:11:10.000 | I am not a world expert on pathophysiology
02:11:14.080 | of individuals with autism spectrum disorder.
02:11:17.200 | I have read some of the literature.
02:11:18.920 | I do study mouse models
02:11:23.200 | of genetically-based autism spectrum disorder.
02:11:26.600 | So the answer is yes.
02:11:28.200 | There have been imaging studies.
02:11:31.120 | And again, so your audience,
02:11:34.320 | certain members of your audience, don't get mad.
02:11:37.100 | Remember our earlier conversation,
02:11:39.200 | we made the point that autism spectrum disorder
02:11:42.720 | is a highly heterogeneous set of behavioral symptoms
02:11:47.720 | with wide variation in how these symptoms manifest
02:11:53.160 | in each individual.
02:11:54.560 | So we cannot make blanket statements
02:11:58.040 | that individuals with autism spectrum disorder
02:12:03.040 | are this or that.
02:12:04.720 | But there are studies, both in human beings and mice,
02:12:08.600 | that suggest that the reinforcing component
02:12:12.400 | of a social interaction is much less or lacking
02:12:17.400 | in our models of autism spectrum disorder
02:12:22.280 | and certain individuals.
02:12:24.280 | An important point is, is that just genetically wired?
02:12:29.280 | Was that because in their early experiences,
02:12:34.620 | they weren't able to get the sensory stimuli
02:12:38.000 | that tell them this is a reinforcing social experience?
02:12:42.100 | Unknown, or at least those are topics
02:12:44.960 | that I think are worthy of investigation.
02:12:47.260 | Do individuals or mice with autism spectrum disorder
02:12:54.200 | lack or do not have the capacity
02:12:57.320 | or the same experience of empathy?
02:13:00.640 | Again, a very complex topic in question.
02:13:03.960 | And it's very likely for some individuals,
02:13:08.760 | the answer is yes, meaning they do lack
02:13:13.080 | some of the neuro mechanisms that allow them,
02:13:15.540 | but that probably doesn't apply to everybody.
02:13:18.120 | I can say in our mouse models of social mechanisms,
02:13:23.760 | of social interactions, and our mouse models
02:13:26.680 | of in quotes, empathy, in these are mice show deficits.
02:13:31.680 | And those deficits can be rescued, meaning improved upon,
02:13:40.140 | by manipulations of certain neuromodulatory systems,
02:13:44.440 | in this case, the serotonin system,
02:13:47.060 | by giving drugs, including a drug called MDMA or ecstasy,
02:13:53.020 | so I hope I'm answering your question.
02:13:56.280 | I think these are worthwhile subjects for investigation.
02:14:01.280 | I think there's a lot of value in studying them.
02:14:08.680 | - Let's go back to serotonin in the nucleus accumbens.
02:14:11.920 | We will get into this in a bit more detail
02:14:13.920 | when we discuss MDMA, but I've now spent a lot of time
02:14:16.600 | with a recent paper of yours that really parsed--
02:14:19.200 | - Really, which one, the MDMA paper?
02:14:21.200 | The Boris Heifetz one?
02:14:22.960 | - Yeah, that parsed the relative roles of dopamine
02:14:26.960 | in the nucleus accumbens versus serotonin
02:14:28.880 | in the nucleus accumbens.
02:14:29.720 | By the way, folks, by the time this episode comes out,
02:14:33.280 | an episode all about MDMA itself and its modes of action
02:14:36.820 | will have already aired, and you can find that,
02:14:39.600 | but even if you haven't heard that,
02:14:41.500 | MDMA is an amazing molecule
02:14:44.000 | because it profoundly increases dopamine,
02:14:47.400 | and that's why the word methamphetamine
02:14:50.320 | is actually in MDMA.
02:14:52.500 | Still a surprise to many people to hear that,
02:14:55.220 | but it also robustly increases serotonin transmission,
02:14:59.640 | and what I love about the paper from your lab
02:15:02.520 | that explored this is that, at least by my read of the data,
02:15:06.300 | it showed very convincingly that it's serotonin released
02:15:10.400 | in the nucleus accumbens that's responsible
02:15:12.160 | for the prosocial effects of MDMA,
02:15:14.340 | whereas oxytocin, this thing we talked about earlier
02:15:17.360 | that everyone assumes is the pair bonding molecule,
02:15:19.560 | the molecule of love, both in humans now,
02:15:22.640 | there's a study in humans,
02:15:23.600 | and in the mouse work that you've done,
02:15:25.760 | doesn't seem to play as prominent a role
02:15:28.240 | in the social enhancement that MDMA causes,
02:15:32.800 | and the reason I'm asking this in the context of autism
02:15:35.440 | is that, for a long time, there was excitement
02:15:38.800 | about the idea that oxytocin nasal sprays
02:15:41.280 | might make autistic kids more excited
02:15:43.720 | about social interactions,
02:15:45.580 | more tuned to social interactions.
02:15:48.040 | First question is, is there any evidence
02:15:50.340 | that increasing oxytocin in a child or adult with autism
02:15:54.200 | makes them somehow more social
02:15:55.960 | or desiring more social connections?
02:15:59.080 | I'm not aware of any.
02:16:00.320 | - I don't think the, I think it is a worthwhile,
02:16:05.320 | it has been studied, I don't think we can close the door
02:16:10.080 | on the potential therapeutic uses of oxytocin.
02:16:14.200 | From the people I know who are much more expert in this
02:16:17.720 | than I am, I think most of the clinical trials
02:16:21.040 | have been pretty disappointing,
02:16:23.020 | with a lot of hope that intranasal oxytocin
02:16:27.080 | would promote more positive prosocial experiences.
02:16:32.080 | I don't think the door is shut yet.
02:16:33.900 | There may be different ways of administering it.
02:16:38.900 | There may be ways of making a different type of oxytocin
02:16:43.760 | that might be beneficial.
02:16:45.880 | I have a colleague at Stanford
02:16:48.240 | who's actually looking at a related neuropeptide
02:16:50.980 | called vasopressin,
02:16:52.640 | and she's finding some potential benefit from that.
02:16:57.280 | And vasopressin and oxytocin
02:16:59.040 | are closely related to each other.
02:17:01.280 | They can even activate some of the same,
02:17:04.360 | what we call receptors in the brain.
02:17:06.680 | So I don't think the door is closed on the possibility
02:17:10.840 | of oxytocin or related therapeutic agents.
02:17:16.600 | Having some therapeutic potential.
02:17:19.440 | The evidence, as far as I'm aware, is not there yet.
02:17:22.760 | In terms of MDMA, again, complicated story.
02:17:27.760 | As you pointed out, MDMA,
02:17:32.320 | it's major molecular targets,
02:17:36.920 | don't wanna get too technical here,
02:17:39.160 | are the serotonin vacuum cleaner,
02:17:43.160 | the molecule that vacuums up serotonin,
02:17:47.600 | and the dopamine vacuum cleaner,
02:17:50.640 | the molecule that vacuums up and, excuse my language,
02:17:55.640 | sucks up dopamine when it's released.
02:17:59.240 | And because it's an amphetamine derivative,
02:18:04.180 | as you correctly pointed out,
02:18:06.680 | it not only prevents these proteins, we call them,
02:18:10.380 | these molecules, these vacuum cleaners,
02:18:12.900 | from vacuuming up the dopamine and serotonin
02:18:16.300 | when it's released, it actually causes it,
02:18:19.620 | I don't wanna use the term,
02:18:21.020 | the terminals to vomit out dopamine and serotonin.
02:18:23.840 | - That's what I say on the--
02:18:25.200 | - All right, is that, am I allowed to say that?
02:18:26.040 | - When I talk about synaptic release,
02:18:27.520 | I'm known for, in my solo episodes,
02:18:29.800 | when I talk about synaptic release,
02:18:31.240 | I'll say that they, bleh, they vomited out.
02:18:34.180 | - So what amphetamine derivative--
02:18:35.780 | - But you worked on synaptic transmission,
02:18:37.220 | so that's almost an insult to a biologist
02:18:38.960 | that works on synaptic transmission.
02:18:39.800 | - No, no, not at all.
02:18:40.620 | What MDMA does is it actually calls
02:18:41.780 | what's known as a reverse transport.
02:18:43.500 | It actually causes, it not only prevents
02:18:45.940 | the vacuum cleaners from sucking up
02:18:49.080 | the dopamine and serotonin,
02:18:50.840 | it causes it to spew out dopamine and serotonin.
02:18:53.960 | So imagine if your vacuum cleaner started,
02:18:56.620 | the pressure in your vacuum cleaner reversed
02:18:58.960 | and all the dirt you collected started being spewed out.
02:19:02.360 | Now the one difference for MDMA,
02:19:04.400 | and it's a fascinating topic I hope we have time
02:19:08.500 | to talk about, is why does MDMA qualitatively for most people
02:19:13.500 | give human subjects a different experience
02:19:19.120 | than cocaine or meth, or especially methamphetamine?
02:19:22.080 | - Presumably it's the fact that there's so much serotonin.
02:19:24.440 | - Exactly.
02:19:25.280 | And so if you actually get, and this is why,
02:19:29.000 | for your audiences, this is why hardcore molecular science
02:19:32.840 | can actually teach us something about
02:19:36.580 | complex human behavioral phenomena,
02:19:39.280 | such as social interactions and addiction.
02:19:43.780 | At least the hypothesis we propose,
02:19:46.080 | and others in the field, it's not just,
02:19:48.800 | science is not done in isolation.
02:19:51.040 | So I wanna give credit where credit is due.
02:19:53.060 | We did not define the following,
02:19:54.880 | that MDMA affects the serotonin system
02:19:59.880 | more than the dopamine system.
02:20:03.000 | So it's not 50/50.
02:20:05.640 | Maybe it's 70/30, 80/20,
02:20:08.500 | and that's because the molecule itself of MDMA,
02:20:12.480 | again, I'm trying not to use language,
02:20:15.100 | it binds to, it has a higher affinity.
02:20:18.280 | It likes to bind to and influence
02:20:20.820 | the serotonin vacuum cleaner
02:20:22.880 | more than the dopamine vacuum cleaner.
02:20:25.160 | It's still affecting both, but it's not 50/50.
02:20:28.460 | It's more, whatever, 70% serotonin, 30% dopamine.
02:20:34.980 | And then it does influence oxytocin in very complex ways,
02:20:39.980 | which is a further technical discussion.
02:20:44.860 | It was just a nice paper that came out
02:20:48.860 | that reported that serotonin release
02:20:51.560 | in a hypothalamic structure,
02:20:53.760 | which again, the hypothalamus,
02:20:55.540 | you can explain to your listeners.
02:20:58.160 | - Marble-ish size structure above the roof of your mouth,
02:21:00.960 | responsible for sex temperature control,
02:21:04.040 | feeding and satiety, and a bunch of other things critical.
02:21:07.040 | - And it's a home of neurons that produce oxytocin.
02:21:12.680 | So this paper reported that when serotonin is released
02:21:16.280 | in the hypothalamus, it activates
02:21:18.040 | and causes the release of oxytocin.
02:21:20.420 | That's in the hypothalamus.
02:21:21.620 | Our work in the reward circuitry suggested oxytocin,
02:21:26.620 | so that's serotonin upstream of oxytocin
02:21:30.060 | in the hypothalamus, where we were looking
02:21:33.100 | in the accumbens, it was the opposite.
02:21:35.500 | Oxytocin caused the release of serotonin.
02:21:38.740 | So the point to your listeners is,
02:21:40.480 | the brain's unfortunately complicated.
02:21:42.640 | - It's tractable.
02:21:43.840 | - It's tractable.
02:21:45.240 | We like to come up with general hypotheses and principles,
02:21:49.140 | but sometimes the devil's in the details
02:21:51.380 | and we really need to probe deeper.
02:21:53.620 | So back to your question about our previous paper
02:21:57.620 | and dopamine and serotonin.
02:21:59.620 | So what we proposed, which is far from nailed down,
02:22:04.620 | is that MDMA, because it is an amphetamine derivative,
02:22:10.440 | does influence dopamine release
02:22:12.840 | and the dopamine system.
02:22:15.900 | And some of my colleagues in the MDMA field,
02:22:20.060 | who I respect enormously, don't like me to say this,
02:22:23.260 | but I'm gonna say it anyhow.
02:22:24.980 | Remember earlier in the podcast,
02:22:29.340 | we talked about different substances
02:22:31.300 | having addictive liabilities.
02:22:33.580 | It doesn't mean a substance is automatically addictive,
02:22:36.640 | doesn't mean it's automatically not, it's a continuum.
02:22:40.300 | And I would argue that MDMA does have
02:22:44.700 | some addictive liability,
02:22:47.220 | because it is an amphetamine derivative.
02:22:49.460 | - It feels good.
02:22:50.300 | - And it feels good.
02:22:51.260 | And so there are individuals that especially,
02:22:55.260 | as your listeners may know,
02:22:56.860 | MDMA has gotten a lot of attention
02:22:59.700 | because it's in a therapeutic trial
02:23:01.560 | that looks very promising as an adjunct to psychotherapy
02:23:06.380 | for post-traumatic stress disorder.
02:23:08.500 | And the FDA, the part of our government
02:23:11.620 | that approves or disapproves the legal distribution
02:23:16.500 | of therapeutic drugs,
02:23:18.180 | may end up approving MDMA for certain uses.
02:23:21.880 | The point being is that if it gets approved,
02:23:26.620 | my personal feeling is it will have some addictive liability.
02:23:30.340 | It also has this very powerful,
02:23:35.100 | what you and I might term, Andrew, a prosocial effect.
02:23:38.840 | Some people even call it an empathogen.
02:23:42.860 | That's a little controversial,
02:23:44.340 | meaning it enhances your capacity for empathy,
02:23:48.120 | to experience the emotional state of another individual,
02:23:53.100 | to want to understand that person's experiences
02:23:56.720 | and emotional state.
02:23:58.060 | And what we've suggested is that the addictive liability
02:24:02.740 | is mostly, although not solely,
02:24:04.700 | being mediated by its actions on the dopamine system,
02:24:08.340 | whereas its positive, more prosocial effects,
02:24:11.920 | and perhaps its empathogenic effects,
02:24:14.980 | are more likely to be mediated by its interactions
02:24:18.980 | with the serotonin system in this reward circuitry.
02:24:23.340 | And we're actually doing a lot of work
02:24:25.840 | to test that hypothesis.
02:24:28.080 | We're actually testing MDMA
02:24:31.180 | in these behavioral models of empathy in mice.
02:24:35.380 | And it looks like our hypothesis is being supported.
02:24:39.920 | The other thing, just to drive your listeners crazy
02:24:44.020 | about, sorry, listeners, how complex the brain is,
02:24:47.860 | if you think--
02:24:48.700 | - Well, neither you nor I were consulted
02:24:50.580 | at the design phase, and so we don't have to apologize
02:24:53.220 | for the brain's complexity.
02:24:54.460 | - Because I don't, trust me, as a scientist,
02:24:56.960 | I wish I could keep things as simple as possible.
02:24:59.740 | That's what good science is.
02:25:01.480 | It turns out the serotonin is produced by neurons
02:25:05.780 | in another part of the brain,
02:25:07.780 | with this wonderful name called the dorsoraphane nucleus.
02:25:11.920 | And it turns out the serotonin neurons
02:25:15.140 | talk to the dopamine neurons.
02:25:18.260 | And influence the dopamine neurons.
02:25:20.820 | And so it's, again, the point we made earlier
02:25:24.880 | in your podcast, even though it's fun and useful,
02:25:28.700 | both for your listeners and as scientists,
02:25:32.020 | to think about these powerful chemical messengers
02:25:35.600 | in isolation, because that's how we can make progress
02:25:39.440 | scientifically, it's how your audience can understand
02:25:43.500 | some of the concepts that have been elucidated
02:25:46.660 | from brain research over the decades.
02:25:50.580 | But they don't work in isolation.
02:25:52.860 | They influence each other,
02:25:54.260 | they communicate with each other.
02:25:56.280 | We're actually doing studies showing that
02:25:59.260 | serotonin release in the accumbens
02:26:01.100 | actually modulates dopamine release,
02:26:03.220 | so it gets crazy complicated.
02:26:06.120 | But you can still develop simplistic hypotheses,
02:26:09.440 | like as I was saying about MDMA,
02:26:12.020 | where, you know, abuse, addictive liability,
02:26:14.800 | and some of its reinforcing qualities,
02:26:17.960 | which you just mentioned, MDMA,
02:26:19.600 | a lot of people find it fun to take it,
02:26:22.200 | is probably mostly being mediated via the dopamine system,
02:26:26.560 | and some of its social effects
02:26:28.560 | are being mediated by the serotonin system.
02:26:32.800 | We're actually doing studies to figure out
02:26:34.600 | whether the reinforcing component of a social experience
02:26:39.360 | requires that dopamine release, probably does.
02:26:42.860 | - That's what I'm most interested in, really,
02:26:44.620 | in the context of MDMA.
02:26:45.940 | And we should just mention,
02:26:46.920 | 'cause we do like to mention these caveats,
02:26:51.060 | yes, and I can say this 'cause I participated
02:26:53.340 | in a trial with MDMA, it is a very pleasant experience.
02:26:56.420 | It's certainly not for everybody.
02:26:57.820 | It still is a schedule one drug at this moment.
02:27:00.340 | - Absolutely.
02:27:01.260 | - So you can go to jail for possessing or selling it.
02:27:04.420 | In fact, there was a big bust recently in Canada
02:27:07.060 | and another one in Brussels.
02:27:08.760 | Large amounts of MDMA collected.
02:27:11.580 | Those people are probably gonna go to prison
02:27:13.340 | for a long period of time.
02:27:14.520 | So you don't want to take it or possess it.
02:27:17.860 | It's illegal.
02:27:19.160 | We're talking about clinical trials here,
02:27:20.460 | but also the fentanyl issue.
02:27:23.040 | There's a lot of fentanyl contamination.
02:27:24.880 | - And I was just gonna mention to your listeners-
02:27:25.720 | - So we'd be remiss if we didn't mention.
02:27:27.460 | A lot of people are dying thinking
02:27:28.920 | that they're taking one drug when they're taking another.
02:27:30.820 | So we are not encouraging the use of these,
02:27:33.260 | but I will say that the subjective experience
02:27:36.620 | of MDMA provided it's done
02:27:38.660 | in the appropriate clinical setting,
02:27:40.320 | it's actually MDMA, doesn't contain other things,
02:27:43.060 | dosed correctly, et cetera,
02:27:44.560 | is a pleasant one for sure.
02:27:47.940 | And my sense is that the dopamine release
02:27:52.180 | is reinforcing the experience that,
02:27:56.540 | the context that serotonin is providing
02:27:58.720 | with the social context.
02:28:00.240 | And the word context there becomes important
02:28:02.300 | when we think about back to the '90s
02:28:04.080 | when there were a lot of raves
02:28:05.420 | and people were also getting, I guess,
02:28:09.060 | positive feedback from the interactions they were having,
02:28:11.280 | dancing all night, partying with friends, et cetera.
02:28:13.460 | I mean, I think that returning to the issue of autism
02:28:17.060 | and the role of serotonin,
02:28:18.580 | so in autism, there seems to be less
02:28:22.860 | of a reinforcement pathway
02:28:25.220 | for certain kinds of social interactions
02:28:27.680 | in some individuals with autism.
02:28:29.980 | And I'm aware that there are some prescription treatments
02:28:33.700 | for autism that capitalize on the serotonergic system
02:28:36.620 | and dopamine system.
02:28:37.740 | So is it fentamine?
02:28:39.420 | To my knowledge, the only FDA approved
02:28:43.920 | pharmacologic therapeutic for individuals
02:28:47.900 | with autism spectrum disorder is actually,
02:28:51.380 | oh God, I'm just blanking.
02:28:53.320 | It's not a serotonergic drug.
02:28:55.960 | I have to look it up.
02:28:59.020 | I wanna say resperidone for agitation.
02:29:02.740 | There is no drug for,
02:29:08.300 | for lack of a better term, the social deficits.
02:29:12.700 | There's no FDA approved drug.
02:29:14.740 | If you look at the literature,
02:29:16.860 | psychiatrists and individuals with good intention
02:29:21.180 | have tested the utility of traditional serotonergic drugs
02:29:26.180 | like Prozac, SSRIs.
02:29:29.100 | There are drugs known as SNRIs,
02:29:31.580 | drugs that influence serotonin release
02:29:33.460 | and another neuromodulator that you know well,
02:29:35.940 | norepinephrine and at least well done clinical trials,
02:29:40.940 | which in my view as an academic are very important.
02:29:44.340 | None of them have showed efficacy.
02:29:46.820 | Having said that, there are several companies
02:29:49.620 | and full disclosure here,
02:29:51.780 | I am the founder of a small biotech
02:29:55.660 | called Map Light Therapeutics.
02:29:57.600 | And I'm not advertising for Map Light.
02:29:59.880 | I'm just doing a full disclosure.
02:30:02.060 | It was founded with Karl Deisseroth,
02:30:04.220 | who you've had on your podcast
02:30:06.500 | and an entrepreneur in San Francisco
02:30:09.580 | named Caroline Nickolich.
02:30:11.040 | And we have the phase two trial.
02:30:15.220 | Phase two trial means it's a safe drug.
02:30:18.340 | We've done all the safety work
02:30:22.380 | and it's a drug that targets a subtype
02:30:27.380 | of receptor for serotonin.
02:30:30.220 | Serotonin works on many different,
02:30:32.160 | I don't know, what word can I use other than receptor?
02:30:35.100 | - No, listeners of this podcast
02:30:37.340 | probably be familiar with receptors.
02:30:38.640 | It's sort of parking spots for molecules.
02:30:41.340 | Yes, the paper I was referencing earlier from your lab,
02:30:45.900 | it talked about serotonin 1B receptors
02:30:48.580 | being particularly important.
02:30:50.020 | - And so the point being is,
02:30:52.640 | I do have an interest in this
02:30:54.940 | on can you use the type of discoveries we've made in mice?
02:30:59.900 | Might it actually have any relevance to human beings,
02:31:04.900 | in particular those who,
02:31:07.940 | some of which have some sort of sociability deficits?
02:31:11.900 | Other companies are pursuing this too.
02:31:16.080 | So MDMA itself, there has been,
02:31:20.040 | I don't know if it's ongoing.
02:31:21.380 | There's a well-known organization.
02:31:23.280 | I don't know if you've ever had anybody from MAPS on this.
02:31:27.300 | The Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies.
02:31:31.140 | MAPS deserves a lot of credit for being a pioneer
02:31:34.660 | in saying, in particular with MDMA,
02:31:38.020 | promoting the idea that this drug deserves
02:31:42.300 | rigorous and ethical study.
02:31:45.880 | That's at least my view.
02:31:48.800 | And MAPS, which was founded by a individual
02:31:53.980 | named Rick Doblin, deserves enormous credit
02:31:57.260 | for their 30-year effort to make it allowed and legal
02:32:02.160 | to actually study MDMA.
02:32:03.580 | The point I'm making is I know MAPS and perhaps others
02:32:06.900 | have done some small trials studying MDMA
02:32:10.940 | in individuals, high-functioning individuals
02:32:15.140 | with some form of social anxiety.
02:32:18.520 | I'm saying this because this is public.
02:32:21.500 | There's another company called MindMed,
02:32:24.500 | which is one of the publicly traded psychedelic companies,
02:32:28.380 | and this is on their website.
02:32:30.280 | Full disclosure, I am on their scientific advisory board.
02:32:34.200 | They are gearing up to do a trial of a,
02:32:40.140 | I don't wanna get too technical, of a certain form of MDMA.
02:32:44.700 | There are two different types of MDMA.
02:32:47.020 | They have these horrible names called enantiomers.
02:32:51.940 | So the MDMA that is used for clinical trials at MAPS,
02:32:56.940 | MDMA is a molecule and it has mirror images of itself.
02:33:01.540 | And one has the name R-MDMA and one has the name S-MDMA.
02:33:06.540 | And they're called the enantiomers
02:33:09.660 | because they're mirror images of each other.
02:33:12.300 | And other labs over the years, not my lab,
02:33:15.580 | I deserve no credit for this, have done some studies
02:33:19.660 | to suggest that the S enantiomer
02:33:23.780 | is the one that has a higher interaction
02:33:27.460 | with the dopamine system.
02:33:29.260 | And the R enantiomer has a higher interaction
02:33:33.300 | with the serotonin system.
02:33:35.060 | - Interesting.
02:33:36.500 | - If you look at the literature on autism spectrum disorder
02:33:40.740 | in human subjects, there's a bunch of papers suggesting
02:33:47.380 | serotonergic systems are malfunctioning
02:33:52.180 | in individuals with autism spectrum disorder.
02:33:56.260 | And if you look at reviews I've written or any of my papers,
02:34:01.260 | we probably cite some of the reviews.
02:34:03.340 | - It's clear that serotonin is playing some role
02:34:06.020 | in social interactions, at least in mice
02:34:09.220 | and almost certainly in humans as well.
02:34:11.420 | It's hard to imagine based on data from everything
02:34:14.180 | from SSRIs to neurotoxic lesions of the human brain,
02:34:16.900 | et cetera, that it's not also playing
02:34:18.660 | at least a similar role in humans.
02:34:19.980 | - Right, and I fully agree with that.
02:34:22.380 | And as we were discussing, there's a modestly extensive
02:34:27.380 | clinical literature, meaning literature from human subjects
02:34:30.780 | suggesting that some aspects of brain systems
02:34:35.780 | that utilize serotonin as one of their signaling molecules,
02:34:41.180 | one of their neuromodulatory mechanisms
02:34:44.060 | may not be functioning in some populations
02:34:47.740 | of individuals with autism spectrum disorder.
02:34:50.260 | So based on that, based on my lab's work
02:34:53.500 | on the role of serotonin in modifying reward circuitry,
02:34:57.900 | its role in pro-social behaviors.
02:35:00.940 | And the biggest clue, which I think you would agree
02:35:04.860 | with, Andrew, is this drug, MDMA.
02:35:08.180 | I mean, this is why I am not a druggie myself.
02:35:11.500 | I am a child of the '60s and '70s.
02:35:14.180 | So I did, which means I'm 20 years older than you, Andrew,
02:35:17.540 | I did experiment like everybody of my generation
02:35:22.100 | with psychoactive substances in the '70s.
02:35:25.540 | So I don't wanna lie about my experiences.
02:35:29.020 | I also would say like many neuroscientists,
02:35:32.180 | my experiences with psychoactive substances
02:35:35.460 | stimulated my interest in neuroscience.
02:35:39.540 | How do these substances work?
02:35:41.660 | Why, when I was a young kid,
02:35:45.620 | the first time I got drunk on beer, why is that happening?
02:35:49.980 | But more seriously, I use drugs in my research
02:35:54.980 | as powerful probes of brain function
02:35:59.220 | with the advantage that,
02:36:01.180 | and now I'm talking scientist to scientist with you, Andrew,
02:36:05.540 | they have molecular targets that we can manipulate
02:36:09.740 | in rigorous ways.
02:36:11.260 | We can figure out where in the brain they act
02:36:14.580 | using the modern tools of neuroscience,
02:36:17.160 | which your audience may not know about.
02:36:20.260 | I'm saying this to you, conditional knockout mice,
02:36:23.700 | rescue experiments.
02:36:24.900 | We can do all those fancy stuff.
02:36:27.420 | And we can use drugs to study even things
02:36:31.140 | as complicated as empathy.
02:36:34.300 | And I really do believe that it's why I've been interested
02:36:37.820 | in MDMA for decades is there's a clue there.
02:36:41.780 | How does a drug that has molecular targets
02:36:46.020 | in the dopamine neuromodulatory system,
02:36:48.660 | in the serotonin neuromodulatory system
02:36:51.420 | have such a powerful effect,
02:36:54.620 | which is relatively specific on social interactions.
02:36:59.620 | It doesn't make you wanna go eat more donuts.
02:37:03.620 | It doesn't, I don't know, for me, there's a clue there.
02:37:08.620 | There's something really important
02:37:11.420 | from that phenomenological observation
02:37:14.840 | in the human experiences that we can learn from.
02:37:18.140 | - I completely agree about MDMA.
02:37:20.260 | And we've done a couple podcasts about psilocybin
02:37:23.620 | and by extension LSD,
02:37:25.380 | because even though there are differences there,
02:37:27.660 | psilocybin LSD, as far as we understand,
02:37:30.700 | largely work through activation of the serotonin 2A receptor,
02:37:35.060 | broadening of a brain network connectivity.
02:37:37.340 | So again, it's serotonin, serotonin, serotonin,
02:37:40.260 | but different receptors,
02:37:42.140 | very different subjective experience.
02:37:45.840 | And I guess perhaps the best way to describe it
02:37:48.100 | is that LSD and psilocybin are almost always considered
02:37:51.420 | mystical in their subjective effects,
02:37:54.140 | whereas MDMA can be an empathogen, an actogen.
02:37:58.740 | And so serotonin acting through different receptor systems
02:38:03.700 | impacting and creating
02:38:04.980 | very different subjective experiences.
02:38:06.540 | I also agree, I think MDMA is particularly interesting
02:38:09.380 | for the neuroscientist,
02:38:10.860 | perhaps also because at least to my knowledge,
02:38:14.120 | there is no substance in nature, no plant, no mushroom,
02:38:18.140 | no ergot, no mold that creates this increase
02:38:23.140 | in dopamine and serotonin simultaneously.
02:38:27.700 | MDMA is a synthesized molecule.
02:38:30.060 | And so it may be one of the, again,
02:38:32.460 | highlighting all the safety issues
02:38:33.880 | and things we talked about before,
02:38:34.900 | it may be one of the great, at least experimental probes
02:38:38.680 | of the brain that humans have developed.
02:38:40.900 | And it may be one of the great therapeutic probes
02:38:44.260 | that folks like MAPS are now doing such fantastic work on.
02:38:47.900 | So I'm very excited about what's happening
02:38:50.260 | with the research on MDMA.
02:38:51.500 | And I'm so glad that your laboratory
02:38:53.500 | has parsed some of the relative roles of serotonin
02:38:56.820 | in the receptors involved.
02:38:58.980 | Since we mentioned serotonin 2A for psilocybin and LSD,
02:39:02.040 | we'd be remiss if we didn't say that this wonderful paper
02:39:04.620 | that we will provide a link to in the show note captions,
02:39:06.500 | by the way, folks,
02:39:08.020 | that Rob Malenka here's lab,
02:39:10.500 | it's focused on the serotonin 1B receptor.
02:39:13.540 | So it even just differences in receptor subtypes
02:39:16.620 | leading to profoundly different subjective outcomes.
02:39:19.340 | I find that to be just one of the most important areas
02:39:24.040 | that one could even think about, let alone work on.
02:39:27.860 | - Thank you, I appreciate the compliment.
02:39:30.060 | I will also say, like everything we're finding,
02:39:33.520 | it's not all about only serotonin 1B,
02:39:36.020 | but that's, as you know, there are, again,
02:39:38.780 | pointing to the amazing and powerful complexity
02:39:43.780 | of the human brain or the mammalian brain.
02:39:47.460 | There are 16 different serotonin parking spots or receptors
02:39:53.300 | that are distributed in different brain areas
02:39:57.240 | in complex ways, and so that's daunting,
02:40:00.480 | but it also offers possibilities
02:40:04.340 | for developing very novel therapeutic agents
02:40:07.620 | that activate or inhibit these in complex ways,
02:40:12.620 | hopefully for therapeutic benefit.
02:40:14.740 | - So before we conclude, I'm very curious
02:40:17.980 | to get your opinion on what you see as the landscape
02:40:22.140 | of the work on psychedelics and MDMA,
02:40:26.040 | which isn't really a classic psychedelic,
02:40:27.860 | but all these drugs that, as you pointed out,
02:40:30.800 | during your youth were used recreationally
02:40:33.320 | and for mind exploration and expansion
02:40:35.300 | and are now being probed as potential therapeutics
02:40:39.100 | for various mental health challenges,
02:40:41.580 | as well as potentially expanding consciousness,
02:40:44.780 | empathy, and all of that.
02:40:46.540 | I mean, not getting into the details of the legal issues
02:40:50.420 | that have to be overcome, not even necessarily
02:40:52.820 | talking about the clinical trials
02:40:54.360 | or the people doing the work in different laboratories,
02:40:56.500 | but just, I have to imagine this must amuse,
02:41:01.260 | tickle, surprise you.
02:41:02.640 | I mean, how do you feel about what you're seeing now?
02:41:05.220 | Because it is a very exciting time for these compounds.
02:41:08.980 | - It tickles me and excites me
02:41:11.340 | with the appropriate caution.
02:41:14.380 | So I do think drugs are very powerful probes
02:41:18.380 | of brain function.
02:41:19.780 | I think this class of drug,
02:41:21.780 | which as you correctly pointed out,
02:41:23.300 | people use the term psychedelics scientifically
02:41:27.660 | when pursuing their understanding,
02:41:32.420 | their therapeutic potential, their mechanism of action,
02:41:35.940 | it's more useful to divide them up into different categories,
02:41:39.560 | the classic hallucinogens, which are LSD and psilocybin,
02:41:43.780 | the intact or empathogens, which is MDMA,
02:41:47.020 | which is really a qualitatively different drug.
02:41:50.340 | There are other substances,
02:41:51.740 | which we don't have time to talk about,
02:41:53.540 | like Ibogaine and ayahuasca,
02:41:55.180 | which are very complex, and peyote.
02:41:58.160 | But nevertheless, I am tickled and excited
02:42:00.980 | as a child of the '60s and '70s,
02:42:03.580 | but I am also not evangelical
02:42:08.460 | about their use and their therapeutic potential.
02:42:13.060 | So as you can imagine what I'm gonna say,
02:42:15.780 | I think they should be the subject of rigorous,
02:42:19.620 | sophisticated, and most importantly, ethical research.
02:42:24.620 | I think we could learn a lot about how the brain works
02:42:29.020 | and its amazing capabilities.
02:42:32.780 | I think we could, I think they may, notice I say may,
02:42:36.900 | have therapeutic potential,
02:42:39.340 | but I do not think they're gonna be miracle cures.
02:42:43.680 | And I do worry, as somebody who lived through
02:42:47.720 | the '60s and '70s and watched, because of the Leary,
02:42:52.240 | the history with Timothy Leary and his colleagues,
02:42:57.540 | and the political landscape of how
02:43:00.780 | they were being used and promoted,
02:43:03.860 | I am cautious that these substances
02:43:08.380 | need to be studied scientifically and rigorously.
02:43:12.700 | And I hope that's the case.
02:43:14.520 | And I wanna caution your audience
02:43:17.720 | that not everybody should take these substances.
02:43:20.560 | They are not miracle cures.
02:43:22.440 | And while they certainly may be of benefit
02:43:25.760 | to certain individuals who are suffering,
02:43:27.880 | and they certainly may provide unusual
02:43:32.880 | and in quotes, mystical experiences
02:43:35.680 | for certain individuals,
02:43:38.560 | I am very concerned that there are individuals out there
02:43:43.160 | that will gain access to these substances
02:43:46.320 | and have very bad experiences.
02:43:48.800 | Because anybody who grew up in the '60s and '70s
02:43:50.900 | knows all about bad trips.
02:43:53.500 | And truth be told, I have had a bad trip or two in the '70s.
02:43:58.500 | And I'm glad I did, because it made me,
02:44:02.740 | I have no idea what a suicidal depression feels like,
02:44:07.600 | where you are experiencing such a darkness,
02:44:11.000 | such a lack of hope that a rational decision
02:44:15.580 | is to end one's life.
02:44:18.120 | And I think the closest I ever came to that experience
02:44:21.360 | is a bad trip on LSD.
02:44:23.820 | And I do have concerns that if you look
02:44:26.940 | at the clinical trials that have been done,
02:44:29.520 | the well could be done, not the anecdotal.
02:44:32.680 | I went and saw some psychedelic therapist
02:44:35.640 | that a friend recommended.
02:44:37.400 | And it did wonders for me.
02:44:39.900 | But the well-controlled clinical trials
02:44:42.200 | that are being done by certain biotechs,
02:44:44.640 | some academic institutions, they have very strict
02:44:48.920 | what are known as inclusionary and exclusionary criteria
02:44:54.240 | about who is allowed to participate in the subject.
02:44:57.600 | And they rule out a lot of people.
02:45:00.440 | So I don't mean to be overly cautious,
02:45:02.840 | but I do worry that if some people
02:45:07.840 | take these substances and bad things happen,
02:45:11.360 | it will slow down the excitement that's currently happening.
02:45:15.600 | And it will make it more difficult
02:45:17.980 | for serious human subjects researchers,
02:45:21.700 | preclinical researchers, to study these substances
02:45:25.040 | in the way they deserve to be studied.
02:45:28.500 | So I hope that articulates my viewpoint.
02:45:33.500 | - I think it does.
02:45:36.140 | And thank you for that viewpoint.
02:45:37.620 | It's an important counterbalance
02:45:39.460 | on a lot of the excitement that we hear about these days.
02:45:42.060 | I think the state of Kentucky just recently decided
02:45:46.220 | to give $42 million from the opioid lawsuit settlement
02:45:51.000 | with Purdue Pharmaceuticals to the study of Ibogaine.
02:45:55.020 | So there's a lot happening here.
02:45:57.660 | Just to be clear, I think there's no problem with that.
02:46:00.860 | And I actually would support that
02:46:03.080 | as long as the studies of Ibogaine
02:46:07.420 | are done thoughtfully, carefully, and ethically.
02:46:12.180 | I see no problem with testing its efficacy
02:46:15.220 | in certain mental illnesses and addiction.
02:46:20.140 | And it's actually a topic I know a little bit about
02:46:23.500 | that we'll save that for another time.
02:46:25.860 | - Right, well, first off, I want to thank you
02:46:29.580 | for coming here and sharing your knowledge with all of us.
02:46:32.900 | For me, it's been a real thrill.
02:46:35.380 | And I also just want to thank you
02:46:37.660 | for the incredible amount of work
02:46:39.260 | that you've done over the years.
02:46:40.300 | I know it's still ongoing.
02:46:41.620 | You're by no means retiring.
02:46:43.860 | - Who knows, who knows?
02:46:44.820 | - I certainly hope not.
02:46:45.960 | But I'm sure the listeners have now a clear picture
02:46:50.380 | of the enormous number of contributions
02:46:52.500 | and areas you've worked everywhere from,
02:46:54.220 | as I mentioned earlier, neuroplasticity
02:46:56.420 | at the cellular level, molecular level, addiction,
02:46:59.780 | work relating to social cognition and social interactions,
02:47:02.800 | rather as it pertains to autism models
02:47:06.740 | and now psychedelics and empathy and on and on.
02:47:10.140 | And again, trained so many prominent scientists
02:47:12.300 | in our field and to take time out of your schedule
02:47:14.680 | to come sit here with us and share some of that knowledge
02:47:17.780 | and stimulate our thinking.
02:47:19.020 | And as you mentioned, raise still more questions
02:47:22.020 | that need to be resolved is a real privilege.
02:47:25.280 | So thank you ever so much.
02:47:26.980 | And indeed, as you just mentioned,
02:47:28.200 | we'd love to have you back again for another conversation.
02:47:30.380 | - All I can say is I want to thank you for having me.
02:47:34.240 | I was a little hesitant or nervous about coming here
02:47:39.080 | and now I want to come back.
02:47:40.860 | So that was a blast when I just did with you
02:47:45.460 | and I'd be happy to continue this conversation anytime.
02:47:49.180 | So thank you for your very sophisticated
02:47:51.860 | and thoughtful questions.
02:47:54.520 | - To be continued. - Yeah, to be continued.
02:47:57.120 | - Thank you for joining me for today's discussion,
02:47:59.260 | all about neuroplasticity, reward systems,
02:48:01.980 | social connection and empathy with Dr. Robert Malanka.
02:48:05.700 | If you're learning from and are enjoying this podcast,
02:48:08.120 | please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
02:48:09.980 | That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.
02:48:12.720 | In addition, please subscribe to the podcast
02:48:14.960 | on Spotify and Apple.
02:48:16.520 | And on both Spotify and Apple,
02:48:17.880 | you can leave us up to a five-star review.
02:48:20.340 | If you have questions for me or comments about the podcast
02:48:22.780 | or guests that you'd like me to consider hosting
02:48:24.800 | on the Huberman Lab Podcast,
02:48:26.420 | please put those in the comment section on YouTube.
02:48:28.820 | I do read all the comments.
02:48:30.820 | In addition, please check out the sponsors mentioned
02:48:33.100 | at the beginning and throughout today's episode.
02:48:35.180 | That's the best way to support this podcast.
02:48:37.700 | Not so much on today's episode,
02:48:39.060 | but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast,
02:48:42.020 | we discuss supplements.
02:48:43.360 | While supplements aren't necessary for everybody,
02:48:45.520 | many people derive tremendous benefit from them
02:48:47.520 | for things like enhancing sleep, hormone support
02:48:50.020 | and focus and much more.
02:48:51.780 | The Huberman Lab Podcast is partnered
02:48:53.340 | with Momentous Supplements.
02:48:54.460 | If you're interested in learning more about the supplements
02:48:56.300 | discussed on the Huberman Lab Podcast,
02:48:57.880 | please go to Live Momentous, spelled O-U-S.
02:49:00.380 | That's livemomentous.com/huberman.
02:49:03.160 | If you're not already following me on social media,
02:49:05.580 | I am Huberman Lab on all social media platforms.
02:49:08.220 | So that means Instagram, Twitter, Facebook, and LinkedIn.
02:49:11.060 | On all those platforms, I post content,
02:49:12.780 | some of which overlaps with the content
02:49:14.260 | of the Huberman Lab Podcast,
02:49:15.740 | but much of which is distinct from the content
02:49:17.900 | on the Huberman Lab Podcast.
02:49:19.660 | If you haven't already subscribed
02:49:20.740 | to the Huberman Lab Podcast, Neural Network Newsletter.
02:49:23.740 | The Neural Network Newsletter is a monthly newsletter.
02:49:26.200 | It is completely zero cost and it includes protocols
02:49:29.800 | or what we call toolkits that you can download.
02:49:31.960 | So for instance, toolkits for enhancing sleep,
02:49:34.180 | a toolkit for learning and neuroplasticity,
02:49:36.380 | toolkits for fitness and for much more.
02:49:39.040 | To sign up for the Neural Network Newsletter,
02:49:41.140 | simply go to HubermanLab.com,
02:49:43.180 | go to the menu and scroll down to newsletter.
02:49:45.360 | You sign up by providing your email,
02:49:47.000 | but I want to be clear
02:49:47.840 | that we do not share your email with anybody.
02:49:50.360 | Thank you once again for joining me for today's discussion
02:49:52.600 | with Dr. Robert Malenka.
02:49:54.240 | And last, but certainly not least,
02:49:56.440 | thank you for your interest in science.
02:49:58.120 | [upbeat music]
02:50:00.700 | (upbeat music)