back to indexHow to Focus to Change Your Brain
Chapters
0:0 Introduction
3:50 Plasticity: What Is it, & What Is It For?
6:30 Babies and Potato Bugs
8:0 Customizing Your Brain
8:50 Hard-Wired Versus Plastic Brains
10:25 Everything Changes At 25
12:29 Costello and Your Hearing
13:10 The New Neuron Myth
14:10 Anosmia: Losing Smell
15:13 Neuronal Birthdays Near Our Death Day
16:45 Circumstances for Brain Change
17:21 Brain Space
18:30 No Nose, Eyes, Or Ears
19:30 Enhanced Hearing and Touch In The Blind
20:20 Brain Maps of The Body Plan
21:0 The Kennard Principle (Margaret Kennard)
21:36 Maps of Meaning
23:0 Awareness Cues Brain Change
25:20 The Chemistry of Change
26:15 A Giant Lie In The Universe
27:10 Fathers of Neuroplasticity/Critical Periods
29:30 Competition Is The Route to Plasticity
32:30 Correcting The Errors of History
33:29 Adult Brain Change: Bumps and Beeps
36:25 What It Takes to Learn
38:15 Adrenalin and Alertness
40:18 The Acetylcholine Spotlight
42:26 The Chemical Trio For Massive Brain Change
44:10 Ways To Change Your Brain
46:16 Love, Hate, & Shame: all the same chemical
47:30 The Dopamine Trap
49:40 Nicotine for Focus
52:30 Sprinting
53:30 How to Focus
55:22 Adderall: Use & Abuse
56:40 Seeing Your Way To Mental Focus
62:59 Blinking
65:30 An Ear Toward Learning
66:14 The Best Listeners In The World
67:20 Agitation is Key
67:40 ADHD & ADD: Attention Deficit (Hyperactivity) Disorder
72:0 Ultra(dian) Focus
73:30 When Real Change Occurs
76:20 How Much Learning Is Enough?
76:50 Learning In (Optic) Flow/Mind Drift
78:16 Synthesis/Summary
85:15 Learning With Repetition, Forming Habits
00:00:02.280 |
where we discuss science and science-based tools 00:00:10.740 |
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology 00:00:17.140 |
from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. 00:00:21.840 |
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science 00:00:24.660 |
and science-related tools to the general public. 00:00:29.380 |
I want to thank the first sponsor of today's podcast. 00:00:34.920 |
Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform 00:00:37.840 |
that analyzes blood factors and DNA-related factors 00:00:41.780 |
that helps you develop a personalized health plan. 00:00:45.200 |
Many important factors related to our health and wellbeing 00:00:48.300 |
can only be measured by a blood sample and by a DNA sample. 00:00:52.740 |
I've been getting my blood work done for many years now, 00:01:10.260 |
there are all these numbers and all these levels 00:01:11.980 |
of different hormones and metabolic factors and so forth, 00:01:14.520 |
but one doesn't know what to do with that information. 00:01:17.420 |
Inside Tracker has a terrific dashboard platform 00:01:20.260 |
where you go online and it makes analyzing all that easy, 00:01:25.860 |
and straightforward directives in terms of exercise, 00:01:31.640 |
that can help guide your health and improve your health. 00:01:44.700 |
Today's episode is also brought to us by Headspace. 00:01:47.660 |
Headspace is a meditation app that makes meditating easy. 00:01:51.180 |
I've been meditating on and off for about three decades now, 00:01:58.220 |
I find it hard to stick with a meditation practice. 00:02:01.260 |
A few years ago, I started using the Headspace app, 00:02:05.000 |
I found that I was meditating really consistently. 00:02:09.640 |
are backed by quality scientific peer-reviewed studies. 00:02:12.840 |
Second of all, the meditations allow the meditation 00:02:19.080 |
I started first using them when I would travel 00:02:21.340 |
because on JetBlue flights, which was the airline I was using 00:02:24.520 |
the meditations are offered as an alternative 00:02:29.680 |
And I found that I would arrive feeling much more refreshed 00:02:31.880 |
than had I just sort of zoned out on the TV the whole time, 00:02:38.920 |
pretty much every day for a short meditation, 00:02:41.240 |
and I personally derive tremendous benefits from it. 00:02:49.440 |
and they'll give you one month of all the meditations 00:02:54.380 |
That's the best offer available right now from Headspace. 00:03:04.240 |
The third sponsor of today's podcast is Made For. 00:03:14.780 |
in order to bring about positive behavioral change 00:03:18.840 |
The company was founded by former Navy Seal Patrick Dossett, 00:03:26.560 |
of the Scientific Advisory Board at Made For, 00:03:28.660 |
and some of the other members of the advisory board 00:03:34.220 |
as well as psychiatrists from Harvard, UC Irvine, 00:03:37.720 |
and many other individuals who are serious about science 00:03:43.360 |
If you want to try Made For, you can go to getmadefor.com, 00:03:54.800 |
which is this incredible feature of our nervous systems 00:03:58.180 |
that allows it to change in response to experience. 00:04:03.800 |
one of the most important aspects of our biology. 00:04:15.800 |
and to essentially adapt to anything that life brings us 00:04:20.700 |
Neuroplasticity has a long and important history, 00:04:24.560 |
and we're not going to review all of it in detail, 00:04:32.360 |
as well as the different forms of neuroplasticity. 00:04:35.480 |
We're going to talk about how to access neuroplasticity 00:04:40.580 |
and depending on the specific types of changes 00:04:44.560 |
This is a topic for which there are lots of tools, 00:04:53.640 |
Most people are familiar with the word neuroplasticity. 00:05:01.560 |
So if I say neuroplasticity or neuroplasticity, 00:05:07.400 |
which is the brain and nervous system's ability 00:05:12.440 |
There are a lot of reasons why the nervous system 00:05:16.720 |
It could do it in response to some traumatic event. 00:05:20.860 |
It could, for instance, create a sense of fear 00:05:27.600 |
It could also occur when something positive happens, 00:05:35.640 |
or we see an incredible feat of performance in athleticism. 00:05:41.600 |
The word neuroplasticity means so many things 00:05:46.720 |
to just first put a little bit of organizational logic 00:05:56.800 |
you would find hundreds of thousands of references, 00:06:00.400 |
scientific references, as well as a lot of falsehoods 00:06:03.240 |
about what neuroplasticity is and how to access it. 00:06:10.040 |
that allow you to engage this incredible feature 00:06:15.040 |
which is that all of us were born with a nervous system 00:06:38.400 |
and we can see evidence of that in the fact that babies 00:06:42.980 |
like a kind of a little potato bug with limbs. 00:06:45.180 |
They can't really do much in terms of coordinated movement. 00:06:50.240 |
and they can't really do anything with precision. 00:06:53.480 |
And that's because we come into this world over-connected. 00:06:59.400 |
Those wires have names like axons and dendrites. 00:07:04.800 |
But those little parts and those wires and connections 00:07:11.040 |
that are all connected to one another in kind of a mess, 00:07:17.080 |
That's essentially what the young nervous system is like. 00:07:21.660 |
as we go from day one of life to 10 years old, 00:07:28.880 |
what happens is particular connections get reinforced 00:07:35.480 |
So that's the first important principle that I want everyone 00:07:38.000 |
to understand, which is that developmental plasticity, 00:07:41.960 |
the neuroplasticity that occurs from the time we're born 00:07:49.000 |
of removing connections that don't serve our goals well. 00:08:01.400 |
are really stamped down into our nervous system 00:08:08.840 |
and then our nervous system is forever changed 00:08:22.520 |
you were essentially a widely connected web of connections 00:08:33.020 |
what you were exposed to by your parents or other caretakers 00:08:36.220 |
through your social interactions, through your thoughts, 00:08:40.940 |
through the places you traveled or didn't travel, 00:08:47.900 |
Now, that's true for certain parts of your brain 00:08:51.500 |
that are involved in what we call representations 00:08:55.280 |
A lot of your brain is designed to represent the visual world 00:09:06.000 |
However, there are aspects of your nervous system 00:09:13.520 |
or changes in those circuits is very unlikely. 00:09:24.760 |
And thank goodness that those circuits were set up that way 00:09:27.640 |
because you want those circuits to be extremely reliable. 00:09:33.300 |
or whether or not you will continue breathing 00:09:34.920 |
or whether or not you'll be able to digest your food. 00:09:37.680 |
So many nervous system features like digestion 00:09:41.760 |
and breathing and heart rate are hard to change. 00:09:52.560 |
is that we can learn through almost passive experience. 00:10:13.180 |
are actually primed to learn and create new combinations. 00:10:26.080 |
plus or minus a year or two, everything changes. 00:10:32.320 |
in order to get changes in our nervous system, 00:10:34.780 |
we have to engage in a completely different set of processes 00:10:40.400 |
and for them, more importantly, to stick around. 00:10:43.680 |
And this is something that I think is vastly overlooked 00:10:46.120 |
in the popular culture discussion about neuroplasticity. 00:10:49.560 |
People always talk about fire together, wire together. 00:10:55.320 |
It is the statement of my colleague at Stanford, 00:11:11.360 |
And so we have these little memes and these little quotes 00:11:18.780 |
or there's a famous quote from the greatest neurobiologist 00:11:25.380 |
should somebody wish to change their nervous system, 00:11:27.680 |
they could be the sculptor of their nervous system 00:11:34.260 |
I mean, who wouldn't want to change their nervous system 00:11:43.660 |
But please understand that early in development, 00:11:46.280 |
your nervous system is connected very broadly 00:11:49.340 |
in ways that make it very hard to do anything well. 00:11:59.240 |
And the incredible strengthening of connections 00:12:04.020 |
or that allow us to do things like walk and talk 00:12:14.600 |
we have to engage in some very specific processes. 00:12:17.680 |
And those processes as we'll soon learn are gated, 00:12:21.160 |
meaning you can't just decide to change your brain. 00:12:23.280 |
You actually have to go through a series of steps 00:12:27.400 |
in ways that will allow you to change your brain. 00:12:30.280 |
I just want to acknowledge that Costello is snoring 00:12:34.480 |
Some of you seem very keen at picking up on his snoring. 00:12:45.640 |
probably relates to the sensitivity of your hearing. 00:12:47.740 |
We're actually going to talk about perfect pitch today 00:12:57.180 |
I want to talk about how the nervous system changes. 00:13:10.620 |
This idea, oh, if you go running or you exercise, 00:13:15.420 |
Well, I'm going to give you the bad news first, 00:13:28.000 |
The idea that new neurons could be added to the brain 00:13:31.220 |
is one that has a rich history in experimental science. 00:13:34.480 |
It's clear that in rodents and in some non-human primates, 00:13:56.040 |
And there is strong evidence that new neurons 00:13:58.620 |
can be added to those structures throughout the lifespan. 00:14:01.960 |
In humans, the evidence is a little bit more controversial. 00:14:11.320 |
the unfortunate experience of being hit on the head too hard, 00:14:15.420 |
the wires called axons from those olfactory neurons 00:14:21.540 |
because they have to pass through a bony plate 00:14:24.200 |
And the cribriform plate can shear those axons 00:14:33.460 |
unlike most all central nervous system neurons, 00:15:07.140 |
the memory center of the human brain, isn't clear. 00:15:09.700 |
Many years ago, Rusty Gage's lab at the Salk Institute 00:15:25.920 |
their brains were harvested, the brains were looked at, 00:15:39.480 |
in the popular press discussion around those papers 00:15:42.220 |
was that it was very few cells that were being added. 00:15:45.720 |
And a number of papers have come along over the years, 00:15:56.380 |
it's an infinitesimally small number of new neurons. 00:16:02.840 |
After we're born, we pretty much have the neurons 00:16:07.860 |
and we start to lose certain functions in our brain, 00:16:15.360 |
because there are other ways in which neural circuits 00:16:18.860 |
can create new connections and add new functions, 00:16:31.720 |
which of course are those things we call synapses, 00:16:34.500 |
between neurons, making those connections stronger 00:16:37.280 |
so they're more reliable, they're more likely to engage, 00:16:47.600 |
or removing the emotional load of a traumatic experience. 00:16:59.160 |
it's clear that we can change our nervous system, 00:17:01.580 |
that the nervous system is available for change, 00:17:04.560 |
that if we create the right set of circumstances 00:17:09.800 |
and if we create the right environmental circumstances 00:17:12.860 |
around us, our nervous system will shift into a mode 00:17:15.960 |
in which change isn't just possible, but it's probable. 00:17:21.720 |
the hallmark of the child nervous system is change. 00:17:26.880 |
The whole thing, everything from the chemicals 00:17:31.620 |
to the fact that there's a lot of space between the neurons, 00:17:39.240 |
and sample different connections very easily, 00:17:43.160 |
As we get older, the so-called extracellular space 00:17:46.520 |
is actually filled up by things called extracellular matrix 00:17:52.740 |
those cells are involved in a bunch of different processes, 00:17:59.600 |
and when that happens, it becomes much harder 00:18:05.080 |
One of the ways in which we can all get plasticity 00:18:19.120 |
and somewhat tragic examples of people, for instance, 00:18:22.300 |
who have genetic mutations where they are born 00:18:24.380 |
without a nose and without any olfactory structures 00:18:37.260 |
involved in other things like touch and hearing and sight. 00:18:50.960 |
The neurons there will start to respond to sounds 00:18:55.000 |
and actually there's one particularly tragic incident 00:19:03.660 |
we knew her visual cortex was no longer visual, 00:19:06.480 |
it was responsible for braille reading and for hearing, 00:19:14.160 |
so then she was blind, she couldn't braille read or hear, 00:19:30.840 |
have much better auditory acuity and touch acuity, 00:19:35.320 |
meaning they can sense things with their fingers 00:19:39.140 |
that typical sighted folks wouldn't be able to. 00:19:42.400 |
In fact, you will find a much greater incidence 00:19:52.400 |
and in particular this area we call the neocortex, 00:19:56.920 |
to be a map of our own individual experience. 00:20:00.020 |
So these, what I call experiments of impairment or loss 00:20:04.080 |
where somebody is blind from birth or deaf from birth, 00:20:11.920 |
where they have a stump instead of an entire limb 00:20:16.360 |
their brain will represent the body plan that they have, 00:20:23.060 |
But the beauty of the situation is that the real estate 00:20:29.000 |
the essence of it is to be a customized map of experience. 00:20:35.340 |
that if, let's say I were to be blind when I'm 50, 00:20:42.400 |
If I was blind at 50, I'll probably have less opportunity 00:20:55.120 |
it's just not the same brain I had when I was a baby. 00:21:05.140 |
which says, if you're going to have a brain injury, 00:21:10.000 |
And of course, better to not have a brain injury at all, 00:21:14.880 |
And this is based on a tremendous number of experiments 00:21:17.600 |
examining the amount of recovery and the rate of recovery 00:21:47.600 |
except for folks that are colorblind, of course. 00:21:50.680 |
And we also have a map of emotional experience, 00:22:03.320 |
"You know, I wasn't teaching the course, I was in the course." 00:22:08.440 |
that every time you speak, it really stresses me out." 00:22:16.120 |
And she said, "Yeah, your tone of voice reminds me 00:22:18.320 |
of somebody that I had a really terrible experience with." 00:22:21.760 |
I said, "Well, okay, well, I can't change my voice, 00:22:23.460 |
but I really appreciate that you acknowledge that 00:22:25.800 |
and it also will help explain why you seem to cringe 00:22:29.300 |
every time I speak," which I hadn't noticed until then, 00:22:31.600 |
but after that, I did notice she had a very immediate 00:22:36.200 |
perhaps some of you are having that right now. 00:22:38.360 |
But in any event, over the period of this two-week course, 00:22:42.480 |
she would come back every once in a while and say, 00:22:46.660 |
that your voice was really difficult for me to listen to, 00:22:49.720 |
it's actually becoming more tolerable to me." 00:22:51.640 |
And by the end, we actually became pretty good friends 00:22:54.720 |
And so what this says is that the recognition of something, 00:23:02.840 |
is actually the first step in neuroplasticity. 00:23:12.400 |
things like our breathing, our heart rate are obvious ones, 00:23:15.840 |
but other aspects are reflexive like our ability to walk. 00:23:19.400 |
If I get up out of this chair and walk out of the door, 00:23:21.560 |
I don't think about each step that I'm taking 00:23:23.360 |
and that's because I learned how to walk during development. 00:23:31.220 |
or some new piece of information that we want to learn 00:23:34.620 |
is something that we want to bring into our consciousness, 00:23:40.120 |
because it cues the brain and the rest of the nervous system 00:23:44.480 |
that when we engage in those reflexive actions going forward 00:23:54.340 |
we're going to talk about protocols for how to do this. 00:23:59.000 |
is recognizing that you want to change something. 00:24:16.620 |
We have to know what it is exactly that we want to change. 00:24:24.440 |
we at least have to know that we want to change something 00:24:28.520 |
In this case, I believe that she came and told me 00:24:31.080 |
that my voice was really awful for her to listen to, 00:24:33.640 |
not to make me feel bad or for any other reason, 00:24:36.720 |
except that she wanted it to not be the case. 00:24:48.640 |
or you want to change your nervous system in any way, 00:24:51.360 |
whether or not it's because of some impairment 00:24:53.460 |
or because of something that you want to acquire, 00:24:56.480 |
a cognitive skill, a motor skill, an emotional skill, 00:25:00.480 |
the first thing is recognizing what that thing is. 00:25:03.760 |
And that often can be the hardest thing to identify. 00:25:06.460 |
But the brain has these self-recognition mechanisms. 00:25:20.800 |
We're going to talk next about the neurochemicals 00:25:32.740 |
because this is in the direction of the change 00:25:42.980 |
or even just that we want to make some change, 00:25:48.080 |
that allow us the opportunity to make those changes. 00:25:51.500 |
Now there are specific protocols that science tells us 00:25:53.740 |
we have to follow if we want those changes to occur. 00:25:56.800 |
But that self-recognition is not a kind of murky concept. 00:26:11.780 |
feel or experience is worth paying attention to. 00:26:15.340 |
So we'll pause there and then I'm going to move forward. 00:26:22.240 |
is that every experience you have changes your brain. 00:26:27.300 |
They love to say your brain is going to be different 00:26:29.500 |
after this lecture or that your brain is going to be 00:26:31.220 |
different after today's class than it was two days ago. 00:26:50.620 |
in the period in which those chemicals are swimming around 00:26:54.020 |
to strengthen or weaken the connections of those neurons. 00:27:15.940 |
David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel started off at Johns Hopkins, 00:27:20.820 |
And in the '70s and '80s, they did a series of experiments 00:27:30.860 |
meaning they put the electrodes in the visual cortex, 00:27:35.900 |
and how the visual brain organizes all the features 00:27:38.540 |
of the visual world to give us these incredible things 00:27:50.020 |
when, for instance, a child comes into the world 00:27:53.660 |
The lens of their eye isn't clear, but it's opaque. 00:28:01.260 |
which is when the eyes either deviate outward or inward. 00:28:11.740 |
is they figured out that there was a critical period 00:28:20.080 |
the visual brain would completely rewire itself 00:28:24.520 |
basically to represent whatever bit of visual information 00:28:30.580 |
to kind of simulate a droopy eye or a deviating eye 00:28:35.700 |
And then what they found is that the visual brain 00:28:39.740 |
There was sort of a takeover of the visual brain 00:28:44.180 |
Many experiments in many different sensory systems 00:28:49.560 |
There are beautiful experiments, for instance, 00:28:58.420 |
if two fingers were taped together early in development, 00:29:06.700 |
so that the person couldn't actually distinguish 00:29:14.200 |
All of this is to say that David and Torsten's work, 00:29:32.220 |
the amount of activity for a given part of our body, 00:29:47.660 |
if we are to change our nervous system in adulthood, 00:29:51.040 |
we need to think about not just what we're trying to get, 00:30:02.100 |
but it actually turns out to be a great advantage. 00:30:04.540 |
One of the key experiments that David and Torsten did 00:30:08.980 |
was an experiment where they closed both eyes, 00:30:11.720 |
where they essentially removed all visual input 00:30:16.060 |
Now, this is slightly different than blindness 00:30:32.580 |
your brain is going to be completely different," 00:30:38.980 |
unless there's a selective shift in your attention 00:30:52.140 |
strengthening and weakening of particular connections. 00:30:57.040 |
which has nothing to do with emotional depression, 00:30:58.740 |
by the way, spike timing dependent plasticity. 00:31:07.320 |
please, you can go Google those and look them up. 00:31:12.520 |
I might even touch on them in some subsequent episodes. 00:31:19.580 |
we really need to bring an immense amount of attention 00:31:25.100 |
This is very much linked to the statement I made earlier 00:31:33.120 |
Well, David and Torrenston won their Nobel Prize, 00:31:38.080 |
because they also figured out how vision works. 00:31:40.860 |
'cause they're my scientific great-grandparents, 00:31:42.380 |
but I think everybody in the field of neuroscience agrees 00:31:51.640 |
absolutely deserved a Nobel Prize for their work 00:32:07.440 |
He's really into art and a number of other things. 00:32:12.520 |
It's also, he was a great Frisbee player, I discovered, 00:32:20.080 |
But anyway, Hubel and Wiesel did an amazing thing 00:32:34.960 |
that if you were to deprive the nervous system of an input, 00:32:41.320 |
and the rest of the visual cortex is taken over 00:32:51.280 |
for why a kid that has a lazy eye or a cataract, 00:32:59.080 |
why now we know that you want to get in there early 00:33:07.640 |
However, their idea that you had to do it early 00:33:12.720 |
to rescue the nervous system deficit later on, 00:33:22.700 |
by the name of Greg Reckenzone was in the laboratory 00:33:34.680 |
because the adult brain simply isn't plastic. 00:33:39.720 |
And they did a series of absolutely beautiful experiments. 00:33:59.200 |
Let's say you were a subject in one of their experiments. 00:34:01.940 |
You would come into the lab and you'd sit down at a table 00:34:05.060 |
and they would record from or image your brain 00:34:09.440 |
and look at the representation of your fingers, 00:34:21.480 |
Some of the bumps were spaced close together. 00:34:32.560 |
the bumps got closer together or further apart. 00:34:41.680 |
And these were not braille readers or anyone skilled 00:34:52.840 |
and they would signal when there was a change 00:34:56.240 |
As they did that, there was very rapid changes, 00:34:58.920 |
plasticity in the representation of the fingers. 00:35:17.840 |
But what it told us is that these maps of touch 00:35:28.120 |
They don't have any impairments that we're aware of. 00:35:34.840 |
And they did some beautiful control experiments 00:35:36.980 |
that are important for everyone to understand, 00:35:39.040 |
which is that sometimes they would bring people in 00:35:49.440 |
or there was a shift in the pitch of that tone, 00:35:53.240 |
So the subject thought they were doing something 00:35:55.800 |
And all that showed was that it wasn't just the mere action 00:36:01.400 |
They had to pay attention to the bumps themselves. 00:36:14.200 |
And this really spits in the face of this thing 00:36:17.860 |
that you hear so often, which is every experience 00:36:20.640 |
that you have is going to change the way your brain works. 00:36:24.220 |
The experiences that you pay super careful attention to 00:36:31.700 |
And it opens up plasticity to that specific experience. 00:36:38.640 |
And Merzenich and his graduate students and postdocs 00:36:45.440 |
is a very straightforward neurochemical answer. 00:36:51.120 |
for any of us to change our brain at any point 00:36:55.180 |
essentially for anything that we want to learn. 00:37:02.080 |
It could be building a greater range of emotion. 00:37:08.980 |
It could be learning new motor skill like dance or sport, 00:37:13.980 |
or it could be some combination of cognitive motor. 00:37:26.380 |
So we're going to talk about what that chemical is, 00:37:31.960 |
that chemical is the same chemical of stress. 00:37:35.700 |
This is not a discussion about stress per se. 00:37:40.420 |
we'll talk all about stress and tools to deal with stress. 00:37:42.940 |
It's something my lab works on quite extensively. 00:37:55.140 |
And the immediate question should be, well, why? 00:37:59.080 |
Well, the answer is that when we pay careful attention, 00:38:06.700 |
that are released from multiple sites in our brain 00:38:23.300 |
And the first neurochemical is epinephrine, also adrenaline. 00:38:33.980 |
but they are chemically identical substances. 00:38:36.760 |
Epinephrine is released from a region in the brainstem 00:38:42.960 |
Fancy name, you don't need to know it unless you want to. 00:38:46.460 |
Locus coeruleus sends out these little wires we call axons, 00:38:51.460 |
such that it hoses the entire brain, essentially, 00:38:58.340 |
Now, it's not always hosing the brain with epinephrine. 00:39:00.700 |
It's only when we are in high states of alertness 00:39:11.300 |
And that's because the way that epinephrine works 00:39:14.980 |
is to increase the likelihood that neurons will be active. 00:39:30.480 |
We know this is true also from the work of Hubel and Wiesel, 00:39:39.100 |
in subjects that were either awake or asleep. 00:39:44.620 |
but you cannot just simply listen to things in your sleep 00:39:50.220 |
Later, I'll talk about how you can do certain things 00:39:57.780 |
but that is not the same as just listening to some music 00:40:06.560 |
Epinephrine is released when we pay attention 00:40:12.060 |
But the most important thing for getting plasticity 00:40:14.540 |
is that there be epinephrine, which equates to alertness, 00:40:17.900 |
plus the release of this neuromodulator acetylcholine. 00:40:21.740 |
Now, acetylcholine is released from two sites in the brain. 00:40:28.320 |
and it's named different things in different animals, 00:40:30.260 |
but in humans, the most rich site of acetylcholine neurons 00:40:36.260 |
is the parabigeminal nucleus or the parabrachial region. 00:40:44.900 |
All you need to know is that you have an area 00:40:46.140 |
in your brainstem, and that area sends wires, 00:40:55.180 |
So we have this area of the brain called the thalamus, 00:40:58.660 |
with all sorts of sensory input all the time. 00:41:11.240 |
like if I really hone in on Costello's snoring, 00:41:18.640 |
is that acetylcholine is now amplifying the signal 00:41:22.980 |
of sounds that Costello is making with his snoring 00:41:33.800 |
So those of you with an engineering background 00:41:37.060 |
Those of you who do not have an engineering background, 00:41:39.620 |
All it means is that one particular shout in the crowd 00:41:43.000 |
comes through, Costello's snoring becomes more salient, 00:41:46.100 |
more apparent relative to everything else going on. 00:41:57.420 |
those two things alone are not enough to get plasticity. 00:42:02.240 |
and the third component is acetylcholine released 00:42:04.700 |
from an area of the forebrain called nucleus basalis. 00:42:15.740 |
or going to medical school, you should know that. 00:42:19.040 |
If you have acetylcholine released from the brainstem, 00:42:22.000 |
acetylcholine released from nucleus basalis and epinephrine, 00:42:31.740 |
as well as other members of the Mersennek lab, 00:42:34.420 |
Michael Kilgard and others, did these incredible experiments 00:42:38.240 |
where they stimulated the release of acetylcholine 00:42:40.860 |
from nucleus basalis, either with an electrode 00:42:43.620 |
or with some other methods that we'll talk about. 00:42:54.660 |
and then the basal forebrain source of acetylcholine, 00:43:02.960 |
doing or paying attention to, immediately in one trial 00:43:12.060 |
you essentially get rapid, massive learning in one shot. 00:43:16.160 |
And this has been shown again and again and again 00:43:20.780 |
also by a guy named Norm Weinberger from UC Irvine. 00:43:23.600 |
And it is now considered a fundamental principle 00:43:27.740 |
So while Hubel and Wiesel talked about critical periods 00:43:31.940 |
it's very clear from the work of Merzenich and Weinberg 00:43:34.300 |
and others that if you get these three things, 00:43:39.160 |
of epinephrine, acetylcholine from these two sources, 00:43:43.040 |
not only will the nervous system change, it has to change. 00:43:51.860 |
for people to understand if they want to change their brain. 00:43:59.420 |
but the way to use repetition to change your brain 00:44:03.540 |
So now let's talk about how we would translate 00:44:08.460 |
into some protocols that you can actually apply, 00:44:10.860 |
because I think that's what many of you are interested in. 00:44:12.700 |
And I'm willing to bet that most of you are not interested 00:44:15.360 |
in lowering electrodes into your nucleus basalis. 00:44:22.020 |
I described the various ways that people can monitor 00:44:34.440 |
can include some dos, do this, and some don'ts, 00:44:49.880 |
which is one can also combine behavioral practices 00:45:00.140 |
In fact, I'm not recommending you do anything in particular. 00:45:06.060 |
I'm not a physician, so I don't prescribe anything. 00:45:09.280 |
I'm a professor, so I profess a lot of things. 00:45:11.240 |
What you do with your health and your medical care 00:45:13.800 |
You're responsible for your health and wellbeing. 00:45:15.660 |
So I'm not going to tell you what to do or what to take. 00:45:18.480 |
I'm going to describe what the literature tells us 00:45:21.320 |
and suggests about ways to access plasticity. 00:45:26.640 |
Most people accomplish this through a cup of coffee 00:45:30.140 |
So I will say you should master your sleep schedule 00:45:33.860 |
and you should figure out how much sleep you need 00:45:36.220 |
in order to achieve alertness when you sit down to learn. 00:45:42.160 |
than probably you ever wanted to hear about sleep 00:45:47.880 |
and all of that is in episodes two, three, four, and five 00:45:53.720 |
if your sleep is not where you would like it to be. 00:45:57.400 |
Your ability to engage in deliberate focused alertness 00:46:00.460 |
is in direct proportion to how well you are sleeping 00:46:11.560 |
the question then is how do I access this alertness? 00:46:20.440 |
They will tell people that they're going to do something 00:46:26.320 |
Or they'll post a picture of themselves online 00:46:28.460 |
and they'll commit to learning a certain amount, 00:46:30.500 |
losing, excuse me, a certain amount of weight 00:46:40.300 |
They'll write checks to organizations that they hate 00:46:48.280 |
They'll decide that they're going to run a marathon 00:46:57.520 |
The truth is that from the standpoint of epinephrine 00:47:01.040 |
and getting alert and activated, it doesn't really matter. 00:47:03.860 |
Epinephrine is a chemical and your brain does not distinguish 00:47:06.560 |
between doing things out of love or hate, anger or fear. 00:47:18.320 |
if you're feeling not motivated to make these changes, 00:47:33.200 |
Also being motivated to not be completely afraid, 00:47:37.640 |
ashamed or humiliated for not following through on a goal 00:47:40.800 |
Just want to briefly mention one little aside there 00:47:44.800 |
He's a cardiologist who has a really interesting theory. 00:47:48.220 |
but I think it will resonate with a lot of people, 00:47:50.280 |
which is that you've all heard of this molecule dopamine 00:47:57.100 |
Well, we also want to be able to access dopamine 00:48:08.420 |
there's many, many instances where someone will come to him 00:48:18.900 |
And his theory is if you get so much dopamine 00:48:22.880 |
"Oh yeah, you're absolutely going to be able to do that," 00:48:29.000 |
So beware these positive reinforcements also. 00:48:32.440 |
Not saying people should flagellate themselves 00:48:34.200 |
to the point of victory in whatever they're pursuing, 00:48:44.480 |
And what is it that's driving me to accomplish this? 00:48:47.620 |
And come up with two or three things, fear-based perhaps, 00:48:51.240 |
love-based perhaps, or perhaps several of those 00:49:08.580 |
outside the laboratory and have trouble focusing, 00:49:13.960 |
to a particular location in space for a particular event. 00:49:36.860 |
There are some important neuroscience principles 00:49:41.760 |
I want to briefly talk about the pharmacology first 00:49:53.160 |
because acetylcholine binds to the nicotinic receptor. 00:49:56.660 |
There are two kinds of acetylcholine receptors, 00:50:05.080 |
These are not my kind of like bro science buddies. 00:50:17.360 |
He quit smoking because of fear of lung cancer. 00:50:26.100 |
This is somebody who's had a very long career. 00:50:37.800 |
But when I asked him, why are you doing this? 00:50:39.100 |
He said, well, increases my alertness and focus. 00:50:42.220 |
And also his theory, and I want to really underscore 00:50:45.080 |
that it's theory not scientifically supported yet 00:50:47.580 |
is that it offsets Parkinson's and Alzheimer's. 00:50:50.880 |
It is true that nucleus basalis is the primary site 00:50:56.880 |
in people that have dementia and Parkinson's. 00:50:59.320 |
And it's what leads to a lot of their inability 00:51:01.700 |
to focus their attention, not just deficits in plasticity. 00:51:10.240 |
I don't like it because I can't focus very well. 00:51:31.960 |
that can increase cholinergic transmission in the brain. 00:51:50.520 |
That's what's going to create a mark in your brain 00:51:55.960 |
Things that increase acetylcholine besides nicotine 00:52:08.420 |
I would encourage you to go to examine.com, the website, 00:52:15.000 |
as well as some of the dangers of these supplements 00:52:17.060 |
that are associated with cholinergic transmission. 00:52:22.000 |
if I didn't say that there are a lot of people out there 00:52:34.280 |
but I'm well aware that the fact that the sprinters 00:52:42.280 |
for the focus that allows them to hear the gun 00:52:49.040 |
Hearing that gun and being quickest on reaction time, 00:52:59.480 |
So your speed of reflexes is actually controlled 00:53:04.240 |
So lots to think about in terms of acetylcholine 00:53:06.760 |
in sport and mental acuity, not just plasticity. 00:53:15.440 |
or take supplements for increasing acetylcholine. 00:53:18.360 |
So what are some ways that you can increase acetylcholine? 00:53:33.880 |
or talking to someone and seeing their mouth move, 00:53:38.560 |
and coming, mm-hmm, mm-hmm, and none of it sinks in. 00:53:41.340 |
This can be very damaging for school, work performance 00:53:46.240 |
Costello, incidentally, never seems to pay attention 00:53:50.940 |
to anything I say while looking directly at me, 00:53:55.100 |
which is that the best way to get better at focusing 00:53:59.960 |
is to use the mechanisms of focus that you were born with. 00:54:04.420 |
And the key principle here is that mental focus 00:54:10.280 |
We are all familiar with the fact that our visual system 00:54:17.420 |
or we can be very laser focused on one location in space. 00:54:21.400 |
What's interesting and vitally important to understanding 00:54:31.820 |
as a way of increasing your mental focus abilities 00:54:41.780 |
And as I mentioned before, that alertness can come from 00:54:45.140 |
a sense of love, a sense of joy, a sense of fear, 00:54:49.380 |
There are pharmacologic ways to access alertness too. 00:54:58.340 |
you know reduces this molecule that makes us sleepy 00:55:02.720 |
I drink plenty of caffeine, I'm a heavy user of caffeine. 00:55:16.640 |
Now, many people are now also using Adderall. 00:55:21.960 |
Adderall chemically looks a lot like amphetamine. 00:55:27.900 |
It will increase epinephrine release from locus coeruleus, 00:55:43.060 |
However, it also has a high probability of abuse, 00:55:47.500 |
especially in those who are not prescribed it. 00:55:50.020 |
Adderall will not increase focus, it increases alertness. 00:55:56.460 |
And if those of you that are taking Adderall say, 00:56:01.500 |
that's probably because your autonomic nervous system 00:56:03.900 |
is just veering towards what we call parasympathetic. 00:56:08.340 |
and so it's bringing your levels of alertness up. 00:56:27.500 |
with a psychiatrist in the room at some point 00:56:33.460 |
The acetylcholine system and the focus that it brings 00:56:38.160 |
is available, as I mentioned, through pharmacology, 00:56:47.660 |
are going to be the ones that are going to allow you 00:56:49.740 |
to develop great depth and duration of focus. 00:56:53.780 |
So let's think about visual focus for a second. 00:56:56.540 |
When we focus on something visually, we have two options. 00:57:01.200 |
We can either look at a very small region of space 00:57:13.100 |
We can't look at everything at high resolution. 00:57:16.460 |
the pupil more or less relates to the fovea of the eye, 00:57:19.900 |
which is the area in which we have the most receptors, 00:57:22.060 |
the highest density of receptors that perceive light. 00:57:26.680 |
in the center of our visual field than in our periphery. 00:57:29.820 |
It's a simple experiment you can do right now. 00:57:31.300 |
If you're listening to this, you can still do it. 00:57:38.580 |
For me, it's five, still got all five fingers. 00:57:41.100 |
Amazingly enough, if I move my hand off to the side, 00:57:47.660 |
but as I move them back into the center of my visual field, 00:57:53.100 |
the number of pixels in the center of my visual field 00:57:57.340 |
When we focus our eyes, we do a couple things. 00:58:08.480 |
The other thing that happens is the lens of our eye moves 00:58:14.620 |
but is seeing a small cone of visual imagery. 00:58:18.380 |
If it, that was the dog bumping into the wall, forgive me. 00:58:22.580 |
That small cone of visual imagery or soda straw view 00:58:25.940 |
of the world has much higher acuity, higher resolution 00:58:32.760 |
Now you say, of course, this makes perfect sense, 00:58:34.860 |
but that's about visual attention, not mental attention. 00:58:48.040 |
the key is to learn how to focus better visually 00:58:58.500 |
Now there's a remarkable phenomenon in animals 00:59:05.740 |
are scanning the entire visual environment all the time. 00:59:09.300 |
Think you're grazing animals, your cows, your sheep, 00:59:13.080 |
But think about a bird picking up seeds on the beach 00:59:21.960 |
or if it's a small bird, about six inches off the ground, 00:59:27.840 |
that can quickly pick up these little seeds off the ground 00:59:36.280 |
and picking up items in front of you with high precision 00:59:44.300 |
and they don't smash their beak into the ground 00:59:54.340 |
or this awareness of what's in front of them? 01:00:04.260 |
Now, their eyes can't actually translocate in their head. 01:00:07.900 |
They're fixed in the skull, just like yours and mine are. 01:00:17.140 |
basically shortening or making the interpupillary distance, 01:00:24.020 |
Not only do we develop a smaller visual window 01:00:27.260 |
into the world, but we activate a set of neurons 01:00:33.980 |
of both norepinephrine, epinephrine, and acetylcholine. 01:00:38.100 |
Norepinephrine is kind of similar to epinephrine. 01:00:40.260 |
So in other words, when our eyes are relaxed in our head, 01:00:44.400 |
at our entire visual environment, moving our head around, 01:00:48.580 |
things moving past us, or we're sitting still, 01:00:51.200 |
we're looking broadly at our space, we're relaxed. 01:00:57.200 |
toward a particular visual target, our visual world shrinks, 01:01:05.080 |
to the release of acetylcholine and epinephrine 01:01:07.760 |
at the relevant sites in the brain for plasticity. 01:01:10.860 |
Now, what this means is that if you have a hard time 01:01:13.660 |
focusing your mind for sake of reading or for listening, 01:01:22.820 |
Now, this works best if you practice focusing 01:01:28.480 |
from the work that you intend to do for sake of plasticity. 01:01:36.180 |
on something related to, I don't know, science. 01:01:39.200 |
I'm reading a science paper and I'm having a hard time, 01:01:42.840 |
I might think that I'm only looking at the paper 01:01:45.140 |
that I'm reading, I'm only looking at my screen, 01:01:46.980 |
but actually my eyes are probably darting around a bit. 01:01:51.020 |
Or I'm gathering information from too many sources 01:01:55.860 |
Now, presumably, 'cause it's me, I've already had my coffee, 01:02:01.940 |
and I still experience these challenges in focusing. 01:02:05.120 |
Spending just 60 to 120 seconds focusing my visual attention 01:02:12.920 |
meaning just on my screen with nothing on it, 01:02:15.720 |
but bringing my eyes to that particular location 01:02:19.080 |
increases not just my visual acuity for that location, 01:02:25.500 |
in a bunch of other brain areas that are associated 01:02:28.840 |
with gathering information from this location. 01:02:33.340 |
So put simply, if you want to improve your ability to focus, 01:02:40.080 |
Now, if you wear contacts or you wear corrective lenses, 01:02:44.400 |
that's fine, you of course would wanna use those. 01:02:46.480 |
You don't wanna take those off and use a blurry image. 01:02:58.320 |
Many times on Instagram, and here I've been teased 01:03:06.220 |
which as you hear it, you'll probably just say duh. 01:03:08.600 |
As we get tired, the neurons in the brainstem 01:03:12.760 |
and that hold the eyelids open start to falter 01:03:22.000 |
But assuming that you're paying attention and you're alert, 01:03:29.480 |
And as you get tired, your eyelids start to close. 01:03:32.200 |
Blinks actually reset our perception of time and space. 01:03:36.520 |
This was shown in a beautiful paper in Current Biology. 01:03:38.680 |
I'll be sure to post the reference in the notes. 01:03:41.600 |
And blinking, of course, is necessary to lubricate the eyes. 01:03:45.200 |
People blink because their eyes might get dry. 01:03:51.240 |
and by focusing your eyes to a particular location 01:03:53.420 |
that's probably pretty creepy for you to experience 01:03:57.920 |
the more that you can maintain a kind of a cone 01:04:04.880 |
which is that I've worked very hard through blinking contests 01:04:09.280 |
with my 14 year old niece who still beats me every time 01:04:15.740 |
of learning to blink less and focus my visual attention 01:04:22.760 |
because I'm mainly learning things on a computer screen. 01:04:29.480 |
and we can discuss how you might translate that to sport. 01:04:37.560 |
to learning of movement practices and coordinated movements. 01:04:45.940 |
You can get that through mental tricks of motivation, 01:04:59.380 |
That increases, it actually will increase alertness. 01:05:02.300 |
Having a very full bladder will increase alertness, 01:05:04.140 |
although you don't want your alertness to be so high, 01:05:11.560 |
You don't want your alertness to go through the roof. 01:05:14.020 |
You need focus and visual focus is the primary way 01:05:18.140 |
in which we start to deploy these neurochemicals. 01:05:21.280 |
Now, you may ask, well, what about the experiment 01:05:29.800 |
Ah, if you look at people who are learning things 01:05:41.100 |
please don't ask them to look you directly in the eye 01:05:47.320 |
If you say, now listen to me and look me in the eye, 01:05:53.060 |
more than they're going to hear what you're saying. 01:06:00.620 |
And this is what low vision or no vision folks do. 01:06:03.200 |
They have tremendous capacity to focus their attention 01:06:08.120 |
Incidentally, does anyone know the two animals 01:06:17.120 |
Turns out it's the elephant that might not surprise you. 01:06:25.260 |
but now it explains why they're so hard to catch. 01:06:37.840 |
If you look at great piano players like Glenn Gould, 01:06:42.840 |
they oftentimes will turn their head to the side. 01:06:49.600 |
that like Stevie Wonder, that were blind, right? 01:06:54.360 |
because he had no reason to look at the keys, 01:06:58.760 |
or one side of their head to the keys on the piano. 01:07:02.160 |
people who are non-sighted have better pitch. 01:07:03.640 |
So we have these cones of attention that we can devote. 01:07:06.680 |
And for most people, vision is the primary way 01:07:09.320 |
to train up this focus ability and these cones of attention. 01:07:19.120 |
If you're feeling agitation and it's challenging to focus 01:07:23.200 |
and you're feeling like you're not doing it right, 01:07:31.840 |
I know it's a little eerie for people to watch, 01:07:36.080 |
that visual window for sake of controlling your focus, 01:07:43.440 |
because we know it engages things like nucleus basalis 01:07:50.480 |
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, ADHD, 01:07:56.040 |
Some people actually have clinically diagnosed ADD and ADHD. 01:08:01.900 |
with a good psychiatrist to try and figure out 01:08:04.120 |
the right pharmacology and/or behavioral practices for you. 01:08:13.800 |
because of the way that they move through their world. 01:08:16.840 |
They are looking at their phone a lot of the time. 01:08:19.400 |
It's actually very easy to anchor your attention 01:08:25.520 |
So it's very easy to limit your visual attention 01:08:30.280 |
It's one of the design features of the phone. 01:08:33.840 |
The other is that just as you've probably heard, 01:08:46.780 |
our attentional system will naturally gravitate 01:08:51.960 |
It's actually much harder to read words on a page 01:08:57.400 |
because we're used to seeing things spelled out for us 01:09:00.480 |
in YouTube videos or videos where things move 01:09:11.500 |
and things that are very dramatic and very intense, 01:09:14.680 |
the worse we're getting at attending to things 01:09:23.620 |
So much so that I think many people have asked me, 01:09:27.160 |
why aren't you providing intense visuals for us to look at? 01:09:31.540 |
are consuming this content through pure auditory, 01:09:35.500 |
and I want them to be able to digest all the material. 01:09:42.580 |
that dictate whether or not we become successful, 01:09:47.400 |
most of those involve the kind of boring practices 01:09:55.300 |
Boring because it's not as exciting in the moment perhaps 01:09:58.480 |
as watching a movie or something being spoonfed to us. 01:10:02.680 |
But the more attention that we can put to something, 01:10:05.820 |
and we feel like we're only getting little bits and pieces, 01:10:08.320 |
shards of the information as opposed to the entire thing, 01:10:13.360 |
in engaging this cholinergic system for plasticity 01:10:22.460 |
the entire thing can be great, it can be awesome, 01:10:28.180 |
if you're somebody who's interested in building your brain 01:10:37.320 |
one has to ask how much of my neurochemical resources 01:10:43.120 |
of letting something just kind of overwhelm me and excite me 01:10:50.360 |
I enjoy movie content and TV content all the time. 01:11:00.360 |
of these acetylcholine release mechanisms or epinephrine. 01:11:06.300 |
that we don't devote all our acetylcholine and epinephrine, 01:11:14.220 |
that are not going to enrich us and better us. 01:11:17.080 |
So that's a little bit of an editorial on my part, 01:11:25.120 |
The real question is, is the information rich for us 01:11:41.440 |
what's it doing for our physical wellbeing for that matter? 01:11:44.440 |
So I don't want to tell people what to do or not to do, 01:11:47.920 |
but think carefully about how often you're focusing 01:11:50.960 |
on something and how good you are or poor you are 01:11:56.760 |
So once you get this epinephrine, this alertness, 01:12:09.640 |
The typical learning bout should be about 90 minutes. 01:12:12.680 |
I think that learning bout will no doubt include 01:12:18.560 |
I think everyone should give themselves permission 01:12:20.820 |
to not be fully focused in the early part of that bout, 01:12:30.240 |
So that for me means eliminating distractions. 01:12:36.340 |
If I find myself reflexively getting up to get the phone, 01:12:39.540 |
I will take the phone and lock it in the car outside. 01:12:43.860 |
I am guilty of giving away the phone for a period of time 01:12:52.700 |
so I can't get to it till the end of the day. 01:12:59.520 |
But I encourage you to try experiencing what it is 01:13:05.400 |
where you feel the agitation that your attention is drifting 01:13:15.280 |
And the way to do that if you're sighted is with your eyes. 01:13:18.160 |
That as your attention drifts and you look away, 01:13:19.960 |
you want to try and literally maintain visual focus 01:13:26.180 |
but you can greatly increase your powers of focus 01:13:30.640 |
which is anchored in all the work of Merseneck, 01:13:37.080 |
but the real secret is that neural plasticity 01:13:41.720 |
doesn't occur during wakefulness, it occurs during sleep. 01:13:46.520 |
We now know that if you focus very hard on something 01:13:54.020 |
maybe you even do several bouts of that per day, 01:13:58.600 |
some people can only do one focus bout of learning. 01:14:01.920 |
That night and the following nights while you sleep, 01:14:04.760 |
the neural circuits that were highlighted, if you will, 01:14:07.720 |
with acetylcholine transmission will strengthen 01:14:12.720 |
which is wonderful because that's the essence of plasticity. 01:14:15.760 |
And what it means is that when you eventually wake up 01:14:19.680 |
you will have acquired the knowledge forever, 01:14:22.520 |
unless you go through some process to actively unlearn it. 01:14:25.120 |
And we will talk about unlearning in a later episode. 01:14:30.840 |
in order to reinforce the learning that occurs. 01:14:32.880 |
But let's say you get a really poor night of sleep 01:14:39.400 |
or the following night, that learning will occur. 01:14:45.560 |
It actually marks those synapses neurochemically 01:14:57.420 |
There's also a way in which you can bypass the need 01:15:03.360 |
by engaging in what I call non-sleep deep rest, 01:15:08.420 |
But I just want to discuss the science of this. 01:15:11.920 |
in Cell Reports last year that shows that if people did, 01:15:17.440 |
actually a quite difficult one where they had to remember 01:15:30.880 |
If immediately after, and it was immediately after 01:15:34.320 |
the learning, the actual performance of this task, 01:15:37.040 |
people took a 20-minute non-sleep deep rest protocol 01:15:45.020 |
so lying down, feet slightly elevated perhaps, 01:15:50.360 |
the rates of learning were significantly higher 01:15:52.840 |
for that information than to just had a good night's sleep 01:15:59.840 |
with these NSDR protocols or with brief naps, 01:16:04.200 |
So the key to plasticity in childhood is to be a child. 01:16:09.000 |
The key to plasticity in adulthood is to engage alertness, 01:16:13.280 |
engage focus, and then to engage non-sleep deep rest 01:16:18.160 |
and deep sleep while you're in your typical bout of sleep. 01:16:26.360 |
Well, I know people that train up these visual focus 01:16:41.520 |
A lot of people find that they can recover best 01:16:51.540 |
where you get into self-generated optic flow. 01:16:53.920 |
And that should make sense if you've ever heard me lecture 01:16:59.060 |
When we are in a mode of self-generated optic flow, 01:17:03.900 |
and things are just floating past us on our retina, 01:17:06.680 |
we're not really looking anywhere in particular. 01:17:08.260 |
So this is the opposite of a tight window of focus. 01:17:11.000 |
When we do that, there are areas of the brain 01:17:15.640 |
in releasing epinephrine and create alertness. 01:17:18.800 |
At the extremes, it creates fear, but certainly alertness. 01:17:25.920 |
So some people find it much more pleasurable and practical 01:17:38.480 |
where you're not really thinking about much of anything. 01:17:40.180 |
And so for those of you that listen to audio books 01:17:41.920 |
or podcasts while you run, you may want to consider 01:17:44.300 |
whether or not that's how you want to spend your time. 01:17:47.200 |
Now, I'd love it if you were listening to this podcast 01:17:49.240 |
while you run or cycle, but I'm much more interested 01:17:52.340 |
in you actually getting the benefits of neuroplasticity 01:17:55.280 |
than just listening to me for sake of listening to me. 01:18:04.720 |
after a period of very deliberate focused effort 01:18:25.560 |
but by no means has it covered all of the potential 01:18:29.240 |
for neuroplasticity and protocols for plasticity. 01:18:34.240 |
But today I want to make sure that these key elements 01:18:42.540 |
First of all, plasticity occurs throughout the lifespan. 01:18:49.600 |
mere exposure to a sensory event can create plasticity. 01:18:56.560 |
We're going to talk about unlearning the bad stuff, 01:18:58.680 |
traumas, et cetera, in a subsequent episode this month. 01:19:02.320 |
If you want to learn as an adult, you have to be alert. 01:19:10.120 |
but I think a lot of people don't think about 01:19:12.640 |
when in their 24 hour cycle they're most alert. 01:19:17.320 |
There are four episodes devoted to that 24 hour cycle 01:19:33.920 |
in learning specific things during that period of time. 01:19:43.000 |
That'd be a terrible time to get into passive observance 01:19:47.200 |
or just letting your time get soaked away by something. 01:19:53.120 |
That epinephrine released from your brainstem 01:19:56.100 |
is going to occur more readily at particular phases 01:20:10.200 |
the behavioral practices, maybe the pharmacologic practices 01:20:15.000 |
that will support heightened levels of alertness. 01:20:20.880 |
And attention is critical for creating that condition 01:20:25.260 |
where whatever it is that you are engaging in 01:20:31.400 |
that you won't have to spend so much attention on it 01:20:36.240 |
that things will eventually become reflexive. 01:20:41.440 |
the ability to suppress an emotional response 01:21:01.860 |
Learning how to engage the cholinergic system 01:21:12.960 |
But how long can you maintain visual focus on a target, 01:21:16.940 |
just on a piece of paper set a few feet away in the room 01:21:21.980 |
These are actually things that people do in communities 01:21:24.440 |
where high levels of visual focus are necessary. 01:21:27.560 |
Now, the other way to get high levels of visual focus 01:21:31.240 |
or to have a situation that's very, very bad. 01:21:40.840 |
is trying to harness the mechanisms of attention 01:21:45.560 |
You may want to do that with your auditory system, 01:21:48.900 |
either because you're low vision or no vision, 01:21:52.640 |
that relates more to sounds than to what you see. 01:21:55.920 |
they're trying to learn information, cognitive information, 01:21:58.160 |
or they're trying to learn how to hear the nuance 01:22:02.940 |
of their emotionally challenging events, et cetera. 01:22:05.820 |
And just remember, by the way, what I said earlier, 01:22:08.040 |
which is that if you really want somebody to listen to you 01:22:11.180 |
and really hear what you're saying and what's underlying it, 01:22:18.320 |
That's actually going to limit their ability to focus. 01:22:24.320 |
I, of course, have never been in this struggle. 01:22:32.940 |
But I know that one can get better at listening, 01:22:42.080 |
Now, of course, you can also combine protocols. 01:22:55.040 |
by drinking their coffee right before they do their learning. 01:22:58.240 |
But I would also encourage you to think about 01:23:06.780 |
see a clinician, but you should also ask yourself, 01:23:16.400 |
or some other activity that doesn't serve you well? 01:23:19.080 |
Or are you devoting that period to the opportunity to learn? 01:23:25.200 |
whether or not you're trying to focus too much 01:23:29.360 |
I know some very high-performing individuals, 01:23:33.320 |
very high-performing in a variety of contexts, 01:23:51.960 |
because we learn best in these 90-minute bouts 01:23:56.640 |
And I should repeat again that within that 90-minute cycle, 01:24:01.320 |
for the entire period of one 90-minute cycle. 01:24:06.820 |
How do you know when one of these 90-minute cycles 01:24:11.500 |
is the beginning of the first 90-minute cycle, 01:24:17.100 |
of these 90-minute cycles as you start to engage 01:24:19.360 |
in these learning practices, should you choose. 01:24:22.400 |
And then of course, getting some non-sleep deep rest 01:24:33.320 |
kind of mindlessly, it might seem in a chair, 01:24:49.880 |
plasticity is your natural right early in life, 01:24:57.840 |
you have to do some work in order to access it. 01:25:03.260 |
of Hubel and Wiesel and Merzenich and Weinberger and others 01:25:10.400 |
It points to the neurochemicals and the circuits. 01:25:19.700 |
that there's an entire other aspect of behavioral practices 01:25:26.220 |
that don't involve intense focus and emotionality, 01:25:32.600 |
So there's another entire category of plasticity 01:25:35.900 |
that involves doing what seemed like almost mundane things, 01:25:40.120 |
but doing them over and over again, repeatedly, 01:25:44.120 |
and incorporating the reward system that involves dopamine. 01:25:47.160 |
So today I talked about the kind of plasticity 01:25:50.740 |
You would get that extreme focus and alertness naturally 01:25:53.480 |
through a hard or difficult event that you didn't want. 01:26:05.360 |
You can get incredible plasticity of positive experiences 01:26:12.400 |
of things that you want by engaging this high focus regime 01:26:15.980 |
and then rest, non-sleep deep rest and sleep. 01:26:24.380 |
as well as when we explore movement-based practices 01:26:27.980 |
for enhancing plasticity and plasticity of movement itself. 01:26:35.860 |
kind of high emotionality or in the intensity 01:26:42.360 |
Those are more about repetition and reward and repeat. 01:26:47.760 |
And they are used for a distinctly different category 01:26:51.380 |
of behavioral change, more of which relate to habits 01:26:54.640 |
as opposed to learning of particular types of information 01:26:58.720 |
that allow us to perform physically, cognitively 01:27:09.680 |
Please put your questions in the comment section below 01:27:24.640 |
So please do put your questions in the comment section 01:27:27.340 |
and I will address them in the other episodes coming soon 01:27:38.660 |
and therefore there isn't an opportunity to leave comments 01:27:44.780 |
So if you have specific topics related to neuroplasticity 01:28:03.540 |
how you can help support the Huberman Lab Podcast. 01:28:06.720 |
Best way to do that is to subscribe on YouTube. 01:28:10.320 |
You might want to also hit the notification button 01:28:12.400 |
so that you don't miss any upcoming episodes. 01:28:27.400 |
In addition, it's always helpful if you recommend the podcast 01:28:32.280 |
who you think might benefit from the information. 01:28:42.120 |
I've talked a number of times about supplements. 01:28:44.680 |
I'm very pleased that we're partnering with Thorne, 01:28:49.840 |
because Thorne has the very high levels of stringency 01:28:56.160 |
about how much of given supplements are in the bottle, 01:29:05.320 |
If you want to check out Thorne and go to Thorne, 01:29:14.960 |
you'll get 20% off any supplements that you purchase. 01:29:18.440 |
I've also listed there a gallery of supplements that I take, 01:29:35.280 |
And as always, thank you for your interest in science.