back to indexThe Science of Hearing, Balance & Accelerated Learning
Chapters
0:0 Overview of Topics
2:20 Protocol: New Data for Rapid Learning
9:10 Introduction: Hearing & Balance
13:53 How We Perceive Sounds
21:56 Your Hearing Brain (Areas)
23:48 Localizing Sounds
28:0 Ear Movement: What It Means
33:0 Your Ears (Likely) Make Sounds: Role of Hormones, Sexual Orientation
35:30 Binaural Beats: Do They Work?
43:54 White Noise Can Enhance Learning & Dopamine
51:0 Headphones
55:51 White Noise During Development: Possibly Harmful
63:25 Remembering Information, The Cocktail Party Effect
72:55 How to Learn Information You Hear
78:10 Doppler
82:43 Tinnitus: What Has Been Found To Help?
90:40 Aging: How Big Are Your Ears?
95:0 Balance: Semi-Circular Canals
100:35 A Vestibular Experiment
103:15 Improve Your Sense of Balance
108:55 Accelerating Balance
111:55 Self-Generated Forward Motion
116:25 Dizzy versus Light-Headed
118:38 Motion Sickness Solution
121:23 Synthesis
00:00:02.280 |
where we discuss science and science-based tools 00:00:10.120 |
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology 00:00:15.080 |
Today, we're going to talk all about hearing and balance 00:00:18.700 |
and how you can use your ability to hear specific things 00:00:22.200 |
and your balance system in order to learn anything faster. 00:00:27.000 |
The auditory system, meaning the hearing system, 00:00:32.520 |
interact with all the other systems of the brain and body 00:00:41.020 |
remember that information longer and with more ease, 00:00:45.040 |
and you can also improve the way you can hear. 00:00:49.440 |
We're going to talk about tools for all of that. 00:00:51.840 |
This is one area of science where we understand a lot 00:00:56.260 |
about the cells and the mechanisms in the ear 00:01:00.840 |
so we're going to talk about that a little bit, 00:01:02.480 |
and then we're going to get directly into protocols, 00:01:09.380 |
in which the auditory and balance systems suffer. 00:01:15.920 |
unfortunately for people that suffer from it, 00:01:21.540 |
We're going to talk about some treatments that can work 00:01:24.400 |
in some circumstances and some of the more recent 00:01:43.520 |
Chances are your ears are making noises right now, 00:01:48.440 |
and yet those can have an influence on other people 00:01:56.240 |
You're going to learn a lot about how your biology 00:01:59.080 |
and brain and ears and the so-called inner ear 00:02:03.080 |
you're going to learn a lot about how all those work. 00:02:07.420 |
I'll even tell you what type of music to listen to, 00:02:10.960 |
you can leverage that in order to learn faster. 00:02:15.580 |
of hearing and balance and tools that leverage hearing 00:02:20.360 |
I want to provide some information about another way 00:02:27.800 |
This is a paper that was published in Cell Reports, 00:02:32.400 |
It's a peer-reviewed paper from a really excellent group 00:02:45.300 |
the vital thing to do is to get lots of repetitions. 00:02:55.180 |
for learning faster, 80/20 rule and all that. 00:03:13.020 |
within the learning episode is very important. 00:03:16.520 |
I want to be really clear what I'm referring to here. 00:03:21.860 |
when you're trying to learn something, it's beneficial, 00:03:30.100 |
or you simply do nothing after a period of learning, 00:03:37.060 |
your ability to learn and remember that information. 00:03:43.120 |
that say that you actually should be injecting rest 00:03:48.540 |
Now, I'm not talking about going to sleep while learning. 00:03:54.340 |
The study involved having people learn sequences of numbers 00:04:03.180 |
I'm not a musician, but I think I'll get this correct. 00:04:06.500 |
They asked people to practice a sequence of keys, 00:04:17.000 |
and they would practice that either continually 00:04:23.300 |
They would play G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G, 00:04:28.420 |
and then they would take a 10-second pause, a rest. 00:04:31.780 |
They would just take a space or a period of time 00:04:37.080 |
Then they would go back to G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G. 00:04:48.300 |
or to inject or insert these periods of 10 seconds idle time 00:05:00.620 |
and they are not touching the keys on the keyboard. 00:05:04.260 |
What they found was that the rates of learning, 00:05:07.460 |
the skill acquisition and the retention of the skills 00:05:11.980 |
when they injected these short periods of rest, 00:05:19.380 |
when I say significantly faster, were much, much faster. 00:05:28.460 |
Why would it be that injecting these 10-second rest periods 00:05:32.740 |
What they called them was micro-offline gains 00:05:35.300 |
because they're sort of taking their brain offline 00:05:38.420 |
Well, it turns out the brain isn't going offline at all. 00:05:47.220 |
that's involved in processing sensory information. 00:05:50.340 |
Well, it turns out that during these brief periods of rest, 00:05:55.600 |
the hippocampus and the cortex are active in ways 00:05:59.940 |
such that you get a 20-times repeat of the GDFEG. 00:06:08.200 |
So basically, the rehearsal continues while you rest, 00:06:14.700 |
So if you were normally getting just, let's just say, 00:06:17.620 |
five repetitions of GDFEG, GDFEG, GDFEG per 10 seconds, 00:06:25.080 |
In the rest periods, you've practiced it 100 times. 00:06:29.100 |
We know this because they were doing brain imaging, 00:06:31.100 |
functional imaging of these people with brain scanners 00:06:50.920 |
and it was actually first proposed by Ebington in 1885. 00:06:58.820 |
for a huge number of different what they call domains, 00:07:01.340 |
in the cognitive domain, so for learning languages, 00:07:04.160 |
in the physical domain, so for learning skills 00:07:13.660 |
If you want to learn all about the spacing effect 00:07:17.040 |
and the categories of learning that it can impact, 00:07:31.040 |
the behavioral learning and the improvement of skill 00:07:39.480 |
I should mention that the paper that I'm referring to, 00:07:41.960 |
the more recent paper that injects these 10-second 00:07:49.160 |
is the work of the laboratory of Leonard Cohen, 00:07:56.320 |
a wonderful poet and musician, but not a neuroscientist. 00:07:59.760 |
Again, the paper was published in Cell Reports, 00:08:01.420 |
and we will provide a link to the full paper as well. 00:08:04.320 |
So the takeaway is if you're trying to learn something, 00:08:09.640 |
but one way that you can get 20 times the number of reps in 00:08:12.960 |
is by injecting these little 10-second periods 00:08:19.580 |
you really don't want to attend to anything else 00:08:25.600 |
and then get right back into generating repetitions. 00:08:31.400 |
and other journals have been publishing recently 00:08:32.960 |
to be fascinating because they're really helping us 00:08:41.760 |
that the brain is willing to generate repetitions for us, 00:08:44.400 |
provided that we give it the rest that it needs. 00:08:47.680 |
So inject rest throughout the learning period, 00:08:49.760 |
and if you can, based on the scientific data, 00:08:56.820 |
where you're not doing anything after a period of learning. 00:09:07.000 |
Before we begin talking about hearing and balance, 00:09:09.540 |
I just want to mention that this podcast is separate 00:09:11.740 |
from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. 00:09:16.500 |
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science 00:09:19.360 |
and science-related tools to the general public. 00:09:24.040 |
I want to thank the sponsors of today's podcast 00:09:27.080 |
and make it clear that we only work with sponsors 00:09:31.320 |
and that we think you will benefit from as well. 00:09:36.840 |
Roca makes sunglasses and eyeglasses that, in my opinion, 00:09:42.660 |
The company was founded by two All-American swimmers 00:09:44.900 |
from Stanford, and everything about their eyeglasses 00:09:47.860 |
and sunglasses were created with performance in mind. 00:09:52.420 |
These eyeglasses and sunglasses have a number of features 00:09:58.080 |
The optical clarity of the lenses is spectacular. 00:10:01.560 |
And for the sunglasses, they have this really great feature, 00:10:07.640 |
or as across the day, the amount of sunshine might change, 00:10:11.320 |
you always experience the world as clear and bright. 00:10:14.360 |
And that can only come from really understanding 00:10:22.640 |
You don't need to know how those things work, 00:10:30.020 |
They also stay on your face even if you get sweaty. 00:10:34.740 |
so running and biking, et cetera, or indoors. 00:10:37.280 |
One thing that I really like about Roca eyeglasses 00:10:39.520 |
and sunglasses is that the aesthetic is terrific. 00:10:42.340 |
Even though they were designed for performance, 00:10:48.300 |
that look kind of ridiculous, kind of space age, 00:10:50.920 |
Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses, you could wear anywhere. 00:10:56.180 |
and they have a huge number of different styles 00:11:04.260 |
and enter the code Huberman to save 20% off your first order. 00:11:12.620 |
Today's podcast is also brought to us by Inside Tracker. 00:11:15.960 |
Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform 00:11:24.620 |
I've long been a believer in getting regular blood work done 00:11:27.740 |
for the simple reason that many of the factors 00:11:30.660 |
that impact our immediate and long-term health 00:11:33.180 |
can only be analyzed from a quality blood test. 00:11:43.760 |
how our brain is functioning, how our endocrine system, 00:11:49.900 |
One of the issues with a lot of companies and programs 00:11:52.180 |
that involve getting blood and DNA tests, however, 00:11:56.020 |
you don't know what to do with that information. 00:11:57.620 |
With Inside Tracker, they make all of that very easy. 00:12:00.160 |
First of all, they can send someone to your home 00:12:07.480 |
and of course you get all the numbers and levels 00:12:09.280 |
of hormone factors, metabolic factors, et cetera, 00:12:12.460 |
but the dashboard in Inside Tracker provides directives 00:12:15.800 |
so that if you want to bring those numbers up 00:12:18.840 |
or if you want to keep them in the same range, 00:12:20.500 |
it points to specific regimens related to nutrition, 00:12:28.180 |
to best suit your health goals and health status. 00:12:35.980 |
and you'll get 25% off any of Inside Tracker's plans. 00:12:41.260 |
Today's podcast is also brought to us by Headspace. 00:12:51.900 |
that meditation is beneficial for our brain and for our body. 00:12:56.340 |
One of the challenges, however, is maintaining 00:13:01.960 |
but I found it very hard to keep that practice going. 00:13:05.040 |
Then I discovered the Headspace meditation app, 00:13:07.300 |
and what I found was that because they have meditations 00:13:09.900 |
that are very short, as well as some that are longer 00:13:15.620 |
Sometimes I do a short five-minute meditation. 00:13:19.700 |
I try and meditate at least 20 minutes per day, 00:13:22.740 |
but sometimes, some weeks, I only do it five times a week, 00:13:35.600 |
And if you do that, you'll get a free one-month trial, 00:13:46.200 |
and you want to get access to all their meditations for free, 00:13:55.980 |
Okay, well, if you can hear me, that's amazing, 00:14:01.980 |
is causing little tiny changes in the airwaves 00:14:06.700 |
and that your ears and whatever's contained in those ears 00:14:20.140 |
and yet we understand a lot about how that process works. 00:14:23.700 |
So I'm going to teach it to you now in simple terms 00:14:34.660 |
but more often they're called pinna, the pinna, P-I-N-N-A, 00:14:42.980 |
this outer part that is made of cartilage and stuff, 00:14:54.980 |
'cause it turns out that your ears change size 00:14:57.160 |
across the lifespan and that how big your ears are, 00:15:01.020 |
or rather how fast your ears are changing size 00:15:05.120 |
is a pretty good indication of how fast you're aging. 00:15:09.460 |
but I want to talk about these things that we call ears 00:15:19.540 |
is such that it amplifies high-frequency sounds, 00:15:30.080 |
that's a low-frequency sound or high-frequency sound, okay? 00:15:34.760 |
So we have low-frequency sounds and high-frequency sounds 00:15:38.860 |
Now those sound waves get captured by our ears 00:15:51.960 |
in the way that air is moving toward your ear 00:16:27.300 |
those three parts are called malleus, incus, and stapes. 00:16:30.980 |
It's like, but basically you can just think about it 00:16:33.720 |
So you've got this eardrum and then a hammer. 00:16:36.280 |
And then that hammer has to hammer on something. 00:16:47.040 |
depending on where somebody lives in the country. 00:16:50.020 |
So typically in the Midwest, on the East Coast, 00:16:57.280 |
So this snail-shaped structure in your inner ear 00:17:00.220 |
is where sound gets converted into electrical signals 00:17:10.000 |
because that little hammer is really, really cool. 00:17:12.200 |
What it means is that sound waves come in through your ears. 00:17:20.080 |
It's like a membrane, or it can move back and forth. 00:17:34.000 |
Now, the cochlea at one end is more rigid than the other. 00:17:44.300 |
And that turns out to be very important for decoding 00:17:59.000 |
And that's because within that little coiled thing 00:18:05.460 |
you have all these tiny little, what are called hair cells. 00:18:11.120 |
but they're not at all related to the hairs on your head 00:18:23.980 |
that a particular sound is in our environment. 00:18:28.620 |
it means that particular sound is not in our environment. 00:18:33.160 |
So just to give you the mental picture of this, 00:18:36.860 |
because there's stuff out there making noises, 00:18:45.940 |
That information is getting funneled into your ears 00:18:48.940 |
because your pinna's are shaped in a particular way. 00:18:54.140 |
and the hammer bangs on this little snail-shaped thing. 00:18:57.740 |
And because that snail-shaped thing at one end 00:19:05.140 |
It can separate out high-frequency and low-frequency sounds. 00:19:09.660 |
And the fact that this thing in your inner ear 00:19:28.000 |
And that's where the hair cells, if they move, 00:19:35.940 |
it's very flexible and it's more like a bass drum. 00:19:38.980 |
So basically what happens is sound waves come into your ears 00:19:42.420 |
and then at one end of this thing that we call the cochlea, 00:19:59.040 |
like a cymbal, tss, tss, tss, tss, tss, tss, tss. 00:20:05.060 |
we have other frequencies, medium frequencies. 00:20:18.100 |
whether or not it's music or voices or crying or screaming 00:20:28.020 |
all of that is being broken down into its component parts. 00:20:32.340 |
And then your brain is making sense of what it means. 00:20:37.940 |
like the pinna of your ears and this little hammer 00:20:40.700 |
and the cochlea, that's all purely mechanical. 00:20:45.440 |
It's just breaking things down into high frequencies, 00:20:56.180 |
And if those ripples are very close together, 00:21:01.940 |
If those ripples are further apart, it's low frequency. 00:21:05.380 |
And obviously, medium frequency is in between. 00:21:16.660 |
and how if you take a prism and put it in front of light, 00:21:20.240 |
it will split that light into its different wavelengths, 00:21:22.980 |
its different colors, red, green, blue, et cetera, right? 00:21:26.260 |
So like the Pink Floyd, "Dark Side of the Moon" album, 00:21:29.780 |
I think has a prism and it's converting white light 00:21:34.900 |
all the wavelengths that are contained in white light. 00:21:42.980 |
and it splits up those sounds into different frequencies. 00:21:52.620 |
and puts it back together and makes sense of it. 00:21:55.100 |
So those hair cells in each of your two cochlea, 00:21:58.160 |
'cause you have two ears, you also have two cochlea, 00:22:04.860 |
that convey their patterns of activity into the brain. 00:22:19.100 |
And because some of you have asked for more names 00:22:40.100 |
So it means a bunch of neurons, so a clump of cells. 00:22:48.540 |
what are called the cochlear nuclei in the brainstem. 00:22:53.980 |
Then up to a structure that has a really cool name 00:22:57.220 |
because you have one on each side of your brain. 00:23:01.900 |
and show you the superior olives in your brain 00:23:04.340 |
or anyone else's brain, they look like little olives. 00:23:24.540 |
to what's called the medial geniculate nucleus, 00:23:32.900 |
but you should know that there are a lot of stations 00:23:39.100 |
before it gets up to our conscious detection. 00:23:45.880 |
which is that more important than knowing what you're hearing 00:23:52.900 |
It's vital to our survival that if something, for instance, 00:23:59.080 |
that we know if it's coming to our right side, 00:24:04.260 |
if a car is coming at us from our left or from our right, 00:24:10.460 |
but our auditory and our visual system collaborate 00:24:13.020 |
to help us find and locate the position of things in space. 00:24:19.500 |
If you hear somebody talking off to your right, 00:24:22.380 |
you tend to turn to your right, not to your left. 00:24:24.780 |
If you see somebody's mouth moving in front of you, 00:24:32.220 |
Disruptions in this auditory hearing and visual matching 00:24:41.260 |
which we'll talk about in a few minutes in more depth. 00:25:01.060 |
is that one of these stations deep in your brainstem 00:25:09.480 |
through a process that's called interaural time differences. 00:25:13.980 |
but really the way you know where things are coming from, 00:25:17.980 |
what direction a car or a bus or a person is coming from 00:25:21.280 |
is because the sound lands in one ear before the other. 00:25:29.180 |
that calculate the difference in time of arrival 00:25:31.860 |
for those sound waves in your right versus your left ear. 00:25:37.240 |
you assume that thing is making noise right in front of you. 00:25:43.740 |
And if the sound arrives first to your left ear, 00:25:47.820 |
that the thing is coming toward your left ear. 00:25:50.940 |
So it's a very simple and kind of a mechanical system 00:26:00.240 |
a sound coming from above is going to land on your right ear 00:26:04.660 |
A sound from below is going to land on your right ear 00:26:21.300 |
The shape of your ears actually modifies the sound 00:26:25.460 |
depending on whether or not it's coming straight at you 00:26:34.340 |
you are taking information about the outside world 00:26:37.140 |
and determining where that information is coming from. 00:26:40.740 |
Now, this all happens very, very fast in a subconscious, 00:26:55.720 |
If you've ever seen those cute little fennec fox things, 00:27:08.200 |
And so when people lean in with their hand like this, 00:27:17.840 |
Oh yeah, and if I do it on the left side, I can do this side. 00:27:47.560 |
as well as these differences in the frequencies 00:27:52.880 |
or I should say the differences in the frequencies 00:27:57.280 |
depending on whether or not the thing is above you 00:28:06.120 |
that these two things on the side of our head 00:28:10.720 |
than hanging earrings on or for other aesthetic purposes 00:28:21.260 |
for allowing us to capture sound waves from our environment. 00:28:35.520 |
humans are not terrifically good at moving their ears. 00:28:38.900 |
Other animals can move their ears even independently. 00:28:42.240 |
So Costello is pretty good at raising his ears, 00:29:02.080 |
will put one ear down to a very particular angle 00:29:34.440 |
The typical distances that people can move it 00:29:37.680 |
is usually no more than two or three millimeters. 00:29:57.780 |
but if you move your eyes all the way to your left, 00:30:01.380 |
which I'm doing now, or all the way to my right, 00:30:04.020 |
you might feel a little bit of a contraction of the muscles. 00:30:16.580 |
but it's mostly by like cramping down my face on one side, 00:30:20.040 |
and I certainly can't raise my right eyebrow. 00:30:47.180 |
Typically, men can do this more than women can, 00:30:50.480 |
although plenty of women can move their ears as well. 00:30:53.080 |
Now, if you think that is all a little strange 00:31:01.400 |
is a system of the brain, but also of the body, 00:31:05.480 |
of the musculature for localizing things in space, 00:31:11.220 |
that one of the things that we share very closely 00:31:14.640 |
with other primates, with non-human primates, 00:31:22.520 |
their ears are remarkably similar to our ears, 00:31:25.780 |
or rather our ears are remarkably similar to their ears. 00:31:29.900 |
The eyes of certain monkeys, like macaque monkeys, 00:31:40.080 |
it has this unbelievably human element to it, 00:31:44.300 |
but the ears of these primates is very similar to our ears, 00:31:56.440 |
and some of the things that ear movements correlate with 00:32:01.540 |
there's a nice paper, actually, a scientific paper. 00:32:13.480 |
some of the sex differences in ear movement control, 00:32:23.380 |
who are interested in this topic could parse fairly easily, 00:32:27.020 |
and there's some very interesting underlying biology 00:32:34.320 |
or ancient carryover of a system for moving our ears. 00:32:43.000 |
next I want to talk about a different feature 00:32:45.780 |
of your hearing and ears that's even stranger, 00:32:49.900 |
but that has some really interesting implications 00:32:55.660 |
and I'm guessing that you've not heard of this. 00:33:05.160 |
and autoacoustic emissions, as the name suggests, 00:33:24.580 |
but what I do mean is that 70% of people's ears 00:33:35.380 |
Sometimes they can be detected by other people in the room 00:33:45.140 |
females who report themselves as heterosexual 00:33:48.620 |
have a higher frequency, not frequency of sound, 00:33:52.660 |
but a higher frequency of autoacoustic emissions 00:33:56.840 |
than do men who report themselves as heterosexual. 00:34:00.420 |
Women who report themselves as homosexual or bisexual 00:34:04.220 |
make fewer autoacoustic emissions than heterosexual women. 00:34:09.540 |
These are data that come from Dennis McFadden's lab 00:34:23.540 |
He was studying hearing, he's a auditory scientist, 00:34:29.060 |
and they were detecting these autoacoustic emissions, 00:34:31.380 |
and they started to notice the group differences 00:34:36.260 |
So they started asking people about their sex 00:34:41.300 |
and these differences fell out of the data, as we say. 00:34:45.580 |
And it's interesting because autoacoustic emissions 00:34:54.860 |
first of all, a lot of us are making noises with our ears, 00:34:59.420 |
and that exposure to certain combinations of hormones 00:35:09.700 |
meaning the cochlea and the pinna and all sorts of things, 00:35:15.620 |
We did do an episode on hormones and sexual development, 00:35:19.160 |
which gets much deeper into the other effects 00:35:21.940 |
that hormones have on the developing brain and body. 00:35:31.920 |
ways to leverage your hearing system, your auditory system, 00:35:37.860 |
not just auditory information, but anything faster. 00:35:41.900 |
I get a lot of questions about so-called binaural beats. 00:35:48.220 |
involve playing one frequency of sound to one ear 00:35:52.480 |
and a different frequency of sound to the other ear. 00:35:55.180 |
So it might be doon, doon, doon, doon to your right ear, 00:35:59.200 |
and it might be doon, doon, doon, doon, doon, doon, 00:36:09.300 |
and because the pathways that bring information 00:36:12.260 |
from the ears into the brain eventually cross over, 00:36:21.500 |
and come up with a sort of intermediate frequency. 00:36:25.020 |
And the rationale is that those intermediate frequencies 00:36:28.340 |
place the brain into a state that is better for learning. 00:36:44.720 |
we have to be alert and focused in order to learn. 00:36:54.760 |
Can binaural beats allow us to relax more if we're anxious? 00:37:02.360 |
as they drill into your teeth and give root canals 00:37:18.500 |
or at least have been proposed to increase creativity. 00:37:20.620 |
So what are the scientific data say about binaural beats? 00:37:23.320 |
There are a number of different apps out there 00:37:28.500 |
I think you can also even just find these on YouTube 00:37:31.420 |
and on the internet, but typically it's an app 00:37:33.380 |
and you'll program in a particular outcome that you want, 00:37:42.420 |
So believe it or not, the science on binaural beats 00:37:45.040 |
is actually quite extensive and very precise. 00:37:48.500 |
So sound waves are measured typically in hertz or kilohertz. 00:37:54.780 |
with thinking about things in hertz or kilohertz, 00:37:56.900 |
but again, just remember those waves on a pond, 00:38:06.420 |
And if they're far apart, then they are low frequency. 00:38:10.840 |
what you're essentially hearing is higher frequency, right? 00:38:19.500 |
And so you may have heard of these things as delta waves 00:38:23.220 |
or theta waves or alpha waves or beta waves, et cetera. 00:38:26.680 |
Delta waves would be big slow waves, so low frequency. 00:38:33.660 |
from peer-reviewed studies that are not sponsored 00:38:38.800 |
that tell us that delta waves like one to four hertz, 00:38:42.780 |
so very low frequency sounds, think Costello's snoring, 00:38:46.300 |
can help in the transition to sleep and for staying asleep. 00:38:51.300 |
And that theta rhythms, which are more like four to eight hertz 00:38:55.020 |
can bring the brain into a state of subtle sleep 00:39:00.020 |
or meditation, so deeply relaxed, but not fully asleep. 00:39:04.340 |
And then you can sort of ascend the staircase of findings 00:39:23.420 |
are great for bringing the brain into focus states 00:39:27.300 |
for sustained thought or for incorporating new information 00:39:31.660 |
and especially gamma waves, the highest frequency, 00:39:34.100 |
the most frequent ripples of sound, so to speak, 00:39:37.680 |
32 to 100 hertz for learning and problem solving. 00:39:41.600 |
Now, all of this matches, or I should say maps onto 00:39:45.920 |
what I've said before about learning really nicely, 00:39:48.540 |
which is that you need to be in a highly alert state 00:39:57.600 |
or the brain is telling itself, okay, I need to learn this. 00:39:59.860 |
This is why stress and unfortunate circumstances 00:40:06.080 |
Here, we're talking about the use of binaural beats 00:40:17.720 |
fundamentally important about the binaural beats. 00:40:20.780 |
They are yet another way of bringing the brain 00:40:23.380 |
into states of deep relaxation through low-frequency sound 00:40:35.780 |
and I'll review a little bit of the data in detail. 00:40:50.820 |
So there are a lot of studies that allowed us to arrive, 00:40:57.740 |
on these parameters of slow, low-frequency waves 00:41:06.840 |
There's very good evidence for anxiety reduction 00:41:14.260 |
And what's interesting is the anxiety reduction 00:41:18.300 |
seems to be most effective when the binaural beats 00:41:25.280 |
so those slow, big waves like sleep, theta, and alpha states. 00:41:32.300 |
although I will probably link more to the list 00:41:43.860 |
There's three studies in peer-reviewed journals 00:42:03.700 |
But the real boost from binaural beats appears to be 00:42:16.100 |
Believe it or not, there are people who would rather 00:42:22.680 |
without Novocain, and that's because they sometimes 00:42:25.420 |
have a syringe phobia or something of that sort, 00:42:28.340 |
or they just don't like being numb from the Novocain, 00:42:30.120 |
or maybe there's an underlying medical reason. 00:42:35.300 |
even if they have Novocain in there or a root canal. 00:42:37.700 |
And so it seems that binaural beats can be effective 00:42:40.500 |
in that environment, and you don't have to go 00:42:45.260 |
Binaural beats are a either relatively inexpensive thing 00:42:51.380 |
to access, most of the apps are pretty inexpensive. 00:42:57.880 |
I confess I did use binaural beats a few years ago. 00:43:00.140 |
I kind of shifted over to other what I call NSDR, 00:43:04.020 |
non-sleep deep breath protocols in favor of those. 00:43:13.440 |
I think part of the reason for that relates to the ability 00:43:17.580 |
to channel our focus when we have some background noise. 00:43:21.380 |
And this is something I also get asked about a lot. 00:43:24.140 |
Is it better to listen to music and have background noise 00:43:27.140 |
when studying, or is it better to have complete silence? 00:43:29.820 |
Well, there's actually a quite good literature 00:43:32.380 |
on this as well, but not so much as it relates 00:43:35.200 |
to binaural beats, but rather whether or not people 00:43:37.900 |
are listening to music, so-called white noise, 00:43:41.500 |
brown noise, believe it or not, there's white noise 00:43:43.580 |
and there's brown noise, there's even pink noise, 00:43:46.080 |
and how that impacts brain states that allow us 00:43:55.100 |
And I want to be very clear that white noise has been shown 00:44:02.480 |
in certain individuals, in particular in adults. 00:44:06.460 |
But white noise actually can have a detrimental effect 00:44:09.460 |
on auditory learning and maybe even the development 00:44:12.040 |
of the auditory system in very young children, 00:44:16.940 |
So first I'd like to talk about the beneficial effects 00:44:22.280 |
There are some really excellent studies on this. 00:44:25.460 |
The first one that I'd like to just highlight is one 00:44:29.340 |
that's entitled Low-Intensity White Noise Improves 00:44:32.200 |
Performance in Auditory Working Memory Task, an FMRI study. 00:44:35.980 |
This is a study that explored whether or not learning 00:44:41.180 |
could be enhanced by playing white noise in the background. 00:44:45.100 |
But the strength of the study is that they looked 00:44:50.620 |
and the activation of the neural circuitry in these people 00:44:54.380 |
And what it essentially illustrates is that white noise, 00:44:58.220 |
provided that white noise is of low enough intensity, 00:45:04.880 |
Not imperceptible, so not so quiet that you can't hear it, 00:45:10.740 |
It actually could enhance learning to a significant degree. 00:45:13.740 |
And this has been shown now for a huge number 00:45:21.940 |
as in a somewhat obscure journal, at least obscure to me, 00:45:26.240 |
on cognitive performance and brain activity patterns. 00:45:32.660 |
They basically were evaluated for mental workload 00:45:36.400 |
and attention under different levels of noise exposure, 00:45:43.480 |
And the reason I like this study is that they looked 00:45:45.740 |
at different levels of noise and types of noise, 00:45:47.940 |
and they varied a number of different things, 00:45:50.440 |
as opposed to just doing a kind of two-condition, 00:45:52.780 |
either white noise or no white noise type thing. 00:46:07.380 |
Now, that's all great, but it really doesn't get 00:46:12.900 |
And as a neuroscientist, what I really want to see is not 00:46:15.380 |
just that something has an effect, that's always nice. 00:46:17.660 |
It's always nice to see in a nice peer-reviewed study 00:46:20.320 |
without any kind of commercial biases that there's an effect. 00:46:37.660 |
in which you could develop better tools and protocols. 00:46:40.660 |
So I was very relieved to find, or I should say excited 00:46:50.620 |
White noise improves learning by modulating activity 00:46:57.380 |
and the right superior temporal sulcus, okay? 00:47:13.660 |
meaning it's a chemical that's released in our brain 00:47:18.020 |
that modulates, meaning controls the likelihood 00:47:30.500 |
Motivation is associated with all sorts of different things, 00:47:37.220 |
is that white noise can really enhance the activity 00:47:42.900 |
of neurons in what's called the substantia nigra VTA. 00:47:45.720 |
The substantia nigra VTA is a very rich source of dopamine, 00:47:50.220 |
and that's because it's very chock-a-block full 00:48:03.740 |
under the microscope, if you take a slice of brain 00:48:06.100 |
or you look at a brain without even staining it 00:48:10.460 |
it's two very dark regions at the kind of bottom 00:48:13.980 |
of the brain, and the reason it's called substantia nigra, 00:48:17.500 |
nigra meaning dark, is because the dopamine neurons 00:48:21.220 |
actually make something that makes those neurons dark. 00:48:25.740 |
And so you've got these two regions down there 00:48:28.300 |
that contain dopamine and can release dopamine 00:48:42.500 |
You actually have to trigger dopamine release from them. 00:48:49.080 |
or the fact that that thing gave you a lot of pleasure 00:48:51.540 |
in the past, or you're highly motivated by fear or desire. 00:48:55.160 |
But what's so interesting to me is that it appears 00:49:02.020 |
the what we call the basal, the baseline levels of dopamine 00:49:09.280 |
So now we're starting to get a more full picture 00:49:15.660 |
can increase learning, and that's in part, I believe, 00:49:20.420 |
through the release of dopamine from substantia nigra. 00:49:24.700 |
So I'm not trying to shift you away from binaural beats, 00:49:30.820 |
that turning on white noise at a low level, not too loud, 00:49:36.140 |
but not too loud can allow you to learn better 00:49:42.660 |
So how loud or how soft should that white noise be 00:49:48.020 |
Well, in these studies, it seemed that white noise 00:49:58.740 |
but not so loud that they felt it was intrusive 00:50:05.740 |
So that's going to differ from person to person 00:50:07.640 |
because people have different levels of auditory sensitivity. 00:50:11.820 |
going to depend on a number of different factors. 00:50:28.960 |
So what you're going to have to do is adjust that white noise 00:50:35.080 |
but rather it's enhancing your ability to focus. 00:50:51.480 |
And really blasting any noise, frankly, is not good, 00:50:58.200 |
meaning it's especially bad, if you have headphones in. 00:51:01.440 |
I do want to mention something about headphones 00:51:03.460 |
before I talk about white noise in the developmental context 00:51:12.460 |
it has this incredible effect of making the sounds 00:51:30.120 |
as opposed from the environment that you're in 00:51:34.940 |
or maybe you're listening to this podcast with headphones, 00:51:37.240 |
that's very different than when you're listening 00:51:39.100 |
to something out in the room and there are other sounds, 00:51:42.600 |
especially if you use these noise cancellation headphones. 00:51:52.000 |
motor skill learning while you're exercising, 00:51:54.620 |
my suggestion would be that if you're using headphones 00:52:00.280 |
This is an effect on the midbrain dopamine neurons. 00:52:03.380 |
That's a background effect of raising the baseline 00:52:06.680 |
The way that dopamine neurons fires, they're always firing. 00:52:10.680 |
When something exciting happens, they fire a lot. 00:52:19.560 |
your overall levels of attention and motivation, 00:52:32.060 |
This isn't like turning on your favorite song. 00:52:35.800 |
This is about getting that baseline up just a bit, okay? 00:52:39.820 |
So I recommend turning the volume up just a bit 00:52:49.240 |
so your attention might drift to that for a moment. 00:52:52.840 |
If you can disappear into the work, so to speak, 00:52:55.280 |
if your attention can disappear into the work, 00:53:08.520 |
And this is coming from somebody who really likes loud music. 00:53:12.200 |
I grew up with kind of a loud, fast rules mentality. 00:53:14.640 |
And if you don't know what loud, fast rules means, 00:53:22.460 |
to listen to music loud, but especially with headphones, 00:53:25.500 |
you can trigger, excuse me, hearing loss quite rapidly. 00:53:34.120 |
our central nervous system neurons, they do not regenerate. 00:53:45.200 |
to blow out your hearing for good, to eliminate your hearing, 00:53:54.860 |
So loud environments can cause hearing loss over time. 00:53:57.380 |
So if you work at a construction site clanging really loud, 00:54:00.300 |
or if you work the soundboard in a club or something, 00:54:09.980 |
Nowadays, some of the earplugs are very low profile, 00:54:13.840 |
So that's kind of nice, so you're not like the, 00:54:15.940 |
like when I was younger, you didn't want to be the dork 00:54:19.360 |
but turns out those dorks were smarter than everybody else 00:54:21.780 |
because they're not the ones who are craning their neck 00:54:26.420 |
at the age of 30 or so because they blew out their hearing. 00:54:35.340 |
or you expose yourselves to a loud environment, 00:54:49.140 |
loud environments plus very high-frequency intense sound, 00:54:59.860 |
that normally would be below the threshold of injury, 00:55:15.160 |
and you feel like you want to crank it up all the way, 00:55:25.640 |
the longer you'll be able to hear that music or that thing. 00:55:30.120 |
So again, I'm not the hearing cop, that's not my job, 00:55:41.860 |
The old argument that it helps you not have to hear 00:55:45.480 |
or listen to people that you don't want to listen to, 00:55:50.380 |
So what about white noise and hearing loss in development? 00:56:05.500 |
to physical and mental health and family health. 00:56:27.060 |
or their speech patterns are going to be disrupted 00:56:32.900 |
but there are data published in the journal Science, 00:56:37.140 |
in Science being one of the three apex journals, 00:56:39.580 |
Science, Nature, Cell, the most stringent journals, 00:56:42.360 |
data published in the journal Science some years ago, 00:56:46.180 |
actually by a scientist who I know quite well, 00:56:48.300 |
his name is Edward Chang, he's a medical doctor now, 00:56:51.900 |
he's actually the chair of neurosurgery at UCSF, 00:56:55.100 |
and he runs a laboratory where they study auditory learning, 00:57:00.440 |
He and his mentor at the time, Mike Mursinek, 00:57:04.740 |
published a paper showing that if young animals, 00:57:13.100 |
so shh, the very type of noise that I'm saying 00:57:21.940 |
early 20s and older, could benefit from listening to that 00:57:25.060 |
at a low level in the background for sake of learning, 00:57:28.500 |
well, they exposed very young animals to this white noise, 00:57:32.040 |
it actually disrupted the maps of the auditory world 00:57:39.240 |
but I want to take a moment and talk about them 00:57:40.640 |
and explain this effect and what it might mean for you 00:57:47.620 |
So auditory information goes up into our cortex, 00:57:52.280 |
into essentially the outside portion of our brain 00:57:55.740 |
that's responsible for all of our higher level cognition, 00:57:59.520 |
our planning, our decision-making, et cetera, 00:58:10.720 |
and at one end, it responds to high frequencies, 00:58:12.680 |
and the other end, it responds to low frequencies? 00:58:15.520 |
Sort of like a piano, the keys sound different 00:58:20.960 |
and it's organized in a very systematic way, right? 00:58:23.680 |
It's not all intermixed high frequencies and low frequencies, 00:58:30.700 |
Your visual system is in what's called a retinotopic map, 00:58:33.640 |
so neighboring points in space off to my right, 00:58:38.900 |
are mapped to neighboring points in space in my brain, 00:58:45.420 |
is mapped to a different location in my brain, 00:58:47.160 |
but it's systematic, it's regular, it's not random, 00:58:49.800 |
it's not salt and pepper, it goes from high to low 00:58:58.460 |
where frequency, high frequency to low frequency, 00:59:06.340 |
Now, our experience of life from the time we're a baby 00:59:09.520 |
until the time that we die is not systematic, 00:59:12.640 |
we don't hear low frequencies at one part of the room 00:59:16.040 |
and high frequencies at another part of the room 00:59:17.660 |
and another part of the day, they're all intermixed, 00:59:20.400 |
but if you remember, the cochlea separates them out 00:59:26.740 |
the cochlea separates out the different frequencies, 00:59:29.980 |
and the developing brain takes those separated out 00:59:37.000 |
between itself, meaning the child, and the outside world. 00:59:41.860 |
White noise essentially contains no tonotopic information, 00:59:48.440 |
the frequencies are all intermixed, it's just noise, 00:59:55.400 |
now I'm getting technical, but it has what's called 00:59:57.380 |
a certain envelope, meaning it has some low frequencies 01:00:01.440 |
I can make my voice higher, although I'm not very good 01:00:14.020 |
it's like all the colors of the rainbow spread out together, 01:00:17.400 |
which is actually what you get when you get white light, 01:00:23.400 |
So one of the reasons why hearing a lot of white noise 01:00:31.600 |
can be detrimental to the development of the auditory system 01:00:34.360 |
is that these tonotopic maps don't form normally, 01:00:39.040 |
Now, the reason I'm raising this is that many people I know, 01:00:43.400 |
in particular friends who have small children, 01:00:45.800 |
they say, I want to use a white noise machine while I sleep, 01:00:51.980 |
but is it okay for my baby to use a white noise machine? 01:00:56.160 |
And I consulted with various people, scientists about this, 01:01:04.360 |
and is hearing music and is hearing the dog bark, 01:01:08.320 |
However, every single person that I consulted with said, 01:01:12.780 |
but you know, there's neuroplasticity during sleep, 01:01:17.640 |
and I don't know that you'd want to expose a child 01:01:26.320 |
It might not destroy it, it might not eliminate it, 01:01:38.720 |
in the appropriate order and everything in between, 01:01:54.600 |
to make the choice of either using white noise 01:02:00.260 |
it's got a little bit more of a certain frequency, 01:02:06.000 |
kind of if you are in a position to make choices 01:02:13.860 |
especially very young child's sleeping environment, 01:02:16.400 |
white noise might be something to consider avoiding, 01:02:21.200 |
but it's something to perhaps consider avoiding. 01:02:27.300 |
but if you talk to any auditory physiologists 01:02:34.280 |
I'm fairly certain that they would have opinions about that. 01:02:37.200 |
Now, whether or not their opinions agree with mine 01:02:41.840 |
is a separate matter, I don't know 'cause I don't know them, 01:02:44.960 |
but it's something that I felt was important enough 01:02:47.280 |
to cue you to, especially since I've highlighted, 01:02:51.440 |
excuse me, the opposite effect is true in adulthood. 01:03:07.300 |
The idea is that it's playing at a low enough volume 01:03:11.080 |
and that it's supporting learning by bringing your brain 01:03:15.200 |
and especially this heightened state of dopamine, 01:03:24.680 |
So now I want to talk about auditory learning 01:03:35.920 |
There are a lot of reasons to want to do this. 01:03:42.960 |
Not everything is necessarily written down for us. 01:03:47.080 |
It's also good to get better at listening or so I'm told. 01:03:52.080 |
So there's a phenomenon called the cocktail party effect. 01:03:55.960 |
Now, even if you've never been to a cocktail party, 01:04:02.900 |
The cocktail party effect is where you are in an environment 01:04:08.200 |
coming from many different sources, many different things. 01:04:14.060 |
that contains people having various conversations, 01:04:22.880 |
meaning you need to hear certain people and not others. 01:04:25.640 |
The reason it's called the cocktail party effect 01:04:27.920 |
is that you and meaning your brain are exquisitely good 01:04:37.640 |
a narrow band of attention with which you can extract 01:04:52.380 |
from a loud gathering, maybe a stadium, a sports event, 01:04:55.960 |
or a cocktail party for that matter and feel just exhausted 01:04:59.200 |
is because if you were listening to conversations there 01:05:03.400 |
while watching the game and people moving past you 01:05:05.720 |
and hearing all this noise, clinking of glasses, et cetera, 01:05:12.360 |
and the brain uses up a lot of energy just at rest, 01:05:19.940 |
when you are paying strong attention to something, 01:05:25.480 |
Even if you're ketogenic, it's burning up energy. 01:05:28.640 |
So the cocktail party effect has been studied extensively 01:05:36.580 |
how one accomplishes this feat of attending to certain sounds 01:05:45.380 |
So there are a couple of ways that we do this. 01:05:46.880 |
First of all, much as with our visual system, 01:05:51.880 |
we can expand or contract our visual field of view. 01:05:59.560 |
see the entire scene that we are in by dilating our gaze, 01:06:03.320 |
talked a lot about this on this podcast and elsewhere. 01:06:05.840 |
We can, for instance, keep our head and eyes stationary 01:06:11.920 |
so you can see the walls and ceiling and floor, 01:06:16.720 |
It's what you would accomplish without having to try at all 01:06:23.520 |
I can bring my focus to a particular location, 01:06:26.720 |
what we call a vergence point, directly in front of me. 01:06:28.560 |
Now I'm pointing at the camera directly in front of me. 01:06:32.320 |
We can expand and contract our visual field of view. 01:06:43.080 |
You can try this next time you are in an environment 01:06:46.400 |
that's rich with noise, meaning lots of different sounds. 01:06:49.440 |
You can just tune out all the noise to a background chatter. 01:06:59.280 |
and you get the background kind of chatter of noise. 01:07:02.840 |
And you'll find that it's actually very relaxing 01:07:05.040 |
in comparison to trying to listen to somebody 01:07:06.840 |
at a cocktail party and you're shouting back and forth. 01:07:08.560 |
Now, if you're very, very interested in that person 01:07:10.560 |
or getting to know them better, or what they're telling you, 01:07:17.420 |
but nonetheless it requires energy and effort and attention. 01:07:29.600 |
although the underlying mechanisms are complex. 01:07:45.220 |
to accomplish this extraction of particular sounds. 01:07:48.040 |
We can really hear one person or a small number of people 01:07:54.840 |
because we pay attention to the onset of words, 01:08:03.000 |
Now, the way to visualize this is if the background noise 01:08:08.920 |
it's literally just sound waves coming every frequency, 01:08:11.320 |
low frequency, high frequency, glasses clinking together. 01:08:17.080 |
There's the crack of the ball if somebody actually manages 01:08:32.280 |
where we are listening, although we don't realize it, 01:08:34.400 |
we are listening for the onset and the offset of those words. 01:08:39.060 |
Now, this is powerful for a couple of reasons. 01:08:41.660 |
First of all, it's a call to arms, so to speak, 01:08:53.840 |
So if you are somebody, you're coming home from work, 01:08:56.020 |
you've had a very long day and you're trying to make out 01:09:00.180 |
a particular conversation on background noise, 01:09:03.420 |
you might consider just not having that conversation, 01:09:06.380 |
just letting your auditory landscape be very broad, 01:09:11.880 |
If you're trying to learn how to extract sound information, 01:09:17.040 |
it could be notes of music, it could be scales of music, 01:09:24.780 |
Maybe somebody is telling you what you need to say 01:09:30.260 |
that you need to learn for a particular topic 01:09:34.060 |
Deliberately paying attention both to the onset 01:09:37.740 |
and to the offset of those words can be beneficial 01:09:41.320 |
because it is exactly the way that the auditory system 01:09:50.460 |
that I think we all experience is you go to a party 01:09:57.360 |
and they'd say, "Hi, I'm Jeff," for instance. 01:10:00.660 |
And then a minute later, I can't remember the guy's name. 01:10:03.920 |
Now, is it because I don't care what his name is? 01:10:10.260 |
It's not that my mind was necessarily someplace else, 01:10:12.740 |
it's that the signal to noise, as we say, wasn't high enough. 01:10:17.500 |
Somehow the way he said it or the way it landed on my ears, 01:10:31.620 |
The noise was too high or the signal was too low 01:10:37.460 |
remember, listen to the onset of what they say 01:10:40.900 |
So it would be paying attention to the j in Jeff 01:10:43.940 |
and it would be paying attention to the th in F, in Jeff. 01:10:49.540 |
And chances are you'll be able to remember that name. 01:10:53.140 |
Now, I don't know if people who are super learners of names 01:11:01.020 |
I don't think they're going to give me access 01:11:03.500 |
But it's a very interesting way to take the natural biology 01:11:08.820 |
and apply it to scenarios where you're trying to remember 01:11:11.700 |
either people's names or specific information. 01:11:14.540 |
Now, I do acknowledge that trying to learn every word 01:11:17.580 |
in a sentence by paying attention to its onset and offset 01:11:24.780 |
So this would be more for specific attention. 01:11:27.340 |
Like you're asking directions in a city and somebody says, 01:11:29.520 |
okay, you say you're lost and they say, okay, 01:11:35.860 |
and then you're going to see a landmark on your right 01:11:37.820 |
and then you're going to go in the third door on your left. 01:11:42.820 |
That's a lot of information, at least for me, okay? 01:11:45.420 |
So the way you would want to listen to that is 01:11:51.240 |
You're going to go left and you're just going to program. 01:11:56.220 |
you're going to think the L at the front of left and the T. 01:12:02.140 |
And then, so you're coding in specific words. 01:12:07.780 |
these naturally occurring attention mechanisms 01:12:12.500 |
It's a little bit of data that for auditory encoding, 01:12:26.660 |
that deliberately encoding auditory information this way, 01:12:29.980 |
meaning trying to learn auditory information this way, 01:12:32.740 |
can be beneficial or can accelerate learning. 01:12:35.580 |
And some of these features of what I'm describing here 01:12:39.000 |
map onto some of the work of Mike Merzenich and others 01:12:43.580 |
that have been designed to try and overcome things 01:12:52.180 |
But more importantly, and perhaps more powerful, 01:12:59.020 |
that was done with his then graduate student, 01:13:12.660 |
in the adult brain, something that's very challenging to do, 01:13:15.980 |
and that the auditory system is one of the main ways 01:13:19.460 |
in which we can access neuroplasticity more broadly. 01:13:24.120 |
and describe the work of Recanzone and Merzenich 01:13:26.340 |
because it's absolutely fantastic and fascinating. 01:13:43.760 |
so essentially high-pitched sounds or low-pitched sounds. 01:13:52.300 |
to a bunch of stuff does not allow the brain to change 01:13:59.940 |
We've all experienced the phenomenon of having someone talk 01:14:03.540 |
and we see their mouth moving and we're like, 01:14:04.900 |
yeah, this is really important, this is really important, 01:14:17.720 |
You wonder whether or not you have trouble with learning 01:14:28.480 |
for particular cues within speech or within sounds, 01:14:32.780 |
that not only can you learn those things more quickly, 01:14:35.740 |
but that you can remap these tonotopic maps in the cortex 01:14:43.140 |
You actually get changes in the neural architecture, 01:15:03.180 |
in the auditory system actually was some of the first 01:15:06.780 |
that really opened up everybody's eyes and ears 01:15:10.700 |
to the idea that the brain can change in adulthood. 01:15:14.060 |
So here's how this sort of process would work 01:15:33.620 |
or certain scales or certain keys on the piano 01:15:36.380 |
and to only focus on those for certain learning bouts. 01:15:40.220 |
So I'll give an example that's sort of real time for me, 01:15:45.980 |
I know generally what I want to say when I arrive here. 01:16:05.200 |
because it wouldn't match my normal patterns of speech 01:16:07.500 |
and you'd probably think I was sounding rather robotic 01:16:11.840 |
So one way that we can remember information is 01:16:15.780 |
as we write out, for instance, something that we want to say, 01:16:28.780 |
we can decide just to highlight particular words 01:16:34.380 |
Now, of course, we're listening to all the information, 01:16:42.500 |
his former student or former postdoc, I don't know which, 01:16:46.620 |
Michael Kilgard, who's now got his own lab down in Texas, 01:16:49.540 |
or others, have shown that the queuing of attention 01:16:57.020 |
the way in which it increases our level of attention overall 01:17:00.420 |
allows us to capture more of the information overall. 01:17:04.100 |
And so I don't want this to be abstract at all. 01:17:12.080 |
All the information's coming in through your ears. 01:17:14.380 |
What you're trying to extract is particular things 01:17:20.740 |
So maybe you decide, if you're listening to me, 01:17:22.580 |
that you're only going to listen to the word tools 01:17:27.220 |
to when my voice kind of goes above background. 01:17:29.820 |
You get to decide what you decide to listen to or not. 01:17:44.020 |
What I'm talking about is exporting certain elements 01:17:46.720 |
of the mechanisms of the cocktail party effect, 01:17:49.360 |
paying attention to the onset and offset of words 01:17:51.820 |
or particular notes within music or particular scales, 01:18:00.200 |
or particular sentences of words or particular phrases. 01:18:13.420 |
that you've all experienced before, which is called Doppler. 01:18:17.900 |
So the Doppler effect is the way that we experience sound 01:18:24.300 |
when the thing that's making that sound is moving. 01:18:30.540 |
is to translate the sound into the visual world once again. 01:18:44.420 |
what you'll notice is that the ripples of water 01:18:49.360 |
are fairly regularly spaced in all directions. 01:18:52.060 |
And that's because that duck or goose is stationary. 01:18:55.540 |
but it's not moving forward or backward or to the side. 01:18:58.960 |
Now, if that duck or goose were to swim forward 01:19:02.100 |
by paddling its little webbed feet under the surface, 01:19:05.900 |
you would immediately notice that the ripples of water 01:19:09.080 |
that are close to and in front of that duck or goose 01:19:12.460 |
would be closer together than the ones that trailed it, 01:19:16.780 |
And that is essentially what happens with sound as well. 01:19:25.740 |
at higher frequency, the ripples are closer together, 01:19:29.440 |
and sounds that are further away at lower frequency, 01:19:50.400 |
I don't know what ambulance or police or what, 01:19:59.920 |
that we make out the direction that things are arriving from 01:20:11.680 |
at discerning what direction things are arriving from 01:20:15.280 |
and the direction that they are going to pass us in. 01:20:19.300 |
And the Doppler effect has probably saved your life 01:20:25.940 |
or you're driving your car and you pull to the side 01:20:29.260 |
so that the ambulance or fire truck can go by 01:20:32.120 |
because you heard that siren off in the distance. 01:20:41.840 |
but also you don't hear any other sirens in the distance. 01:20:44.940 |
Now, some animals such as bats are exquisitely good 01:20:49.000 |
at navigating their environments according to sound. 01:20:57.680 |
but they just rely more heavily on their auditory system. 01:21:07.880 |
Now, they don't simply listen to whether or not 01:21:11.120 |
things are coming at them or moving away from them 01:21:18.920 |
what they do is they generate their own sounds. 01:21:22.600 |
So a bat, as it flies around is sending out clicks. 01:21:38.320 |
Now, whether or not they're conscious of it, I don't know. 01:21:41.020 |
And if I did ask them, I don't think they could answer. 01:21:43.340 |
they couldn't answer in a language that I could understand. 01:21:53.360 |
And then depending on the frequency of sound waves 01:21:57.200 |
they know if they're getting closer to an object 01:22:01.120 |
So if they send out sounds at a frequency of, 01:22:03.860 |
this was much slower than it would actually occur, 01:22:06.660 |
but let's say one every half second [mumbles] 01:22:13.760 |
then they know they're getting closer, right? 01:22:20.240 |
they know that there's nothing in front of them. 01:22:22.280 |
So the bat is essentially navigating its world 01:22:29.380 |
that bounce back onto them from the various objects, 01:22:44.440 |
Now I'd like to talk about ringing in the ears. 01:22:48.000 |
This is something that I get asked about a lot. 01:22:55.540 |
because many people suffer from ringing in their ears 01:23:05.460 |
So it could be a sampling bias, I don't know, 01:23:20.640 |
So the ringing of the ears that one experiences 01:23:24.120 |
is called tinnitus, not tinnitus, but tinnitus. 01:23:36.200 |
It can vary across the lifespan or even time of day. 01:23:40.520 |
So it's very subject to kind of background effects 01:23:45.800 |
So I think we all know that we should do our best 01:23:53.760 |
of the Huberman Lab Podcast were all about sleep 01:23:57.020 |
We all know that we should try and limit our stress, 01:24:12.400 |
just can't explain the presence of the tinnitus. 01:24:15.000 |
Tinnitus can be caused by disruption to these hair cells 01:24:20.560 |
that we talked about earlier or damage to the hair cells. 01:24:34.680 |
environments where there's a lot of background noise, 01:24:36.920 |
and then you add another really loud auditory stimulus. 01:24:40.960 |
This also can happen at different times, I should mention. 01:24:45.280 |
or you listen to loud music with your headphones, 01:24:56.560 |
currently we don't have the technology to put them back, 01:24:59.920 |
including some excellent groups at Stanford and elsewhere too, 01:25:05.360 |
to replenish those hair cells and restore hearing. 01:25:21.800 |
which is typically what we discuss on this podcast 01:25:29.620 |
for which there are quality peer-reviewed data 01:25:32.560 |
where there does not appear to be any overt commercial bias, 01:25:36.880 |
meaning that nothing's reported in the papers 01:25:41.120 |
And those are melatonin, ginkgo bilboa, zinc, and magnesium. 01:25:49.080 |
I'm personally not a fan of melatonin as a sleep aid, 01:25:55.440 |
First one entitled "The Effects of Melatonin on Tinnitus" 01:26:02.680 |
Second one, "Treatment of Sensual and Sensory Neural Tinnitus 01:26:14.660 |
which is an interesting article, double-blinded study, 01:26:20.060 |
Each one of these studies has anywhere from 30 01:26:30.960 |
Typically, it's listed as sex, not gender in studies, 01:26:34.040 |
so it should say both sexes, but nonetheless. 01:26:44.400 |
I didn't see any studies of people younger than 30. 01:26:58.600 |
which is sort of typical of many supplements for melatonin, 01:27:02.280 |
still much higher than one would manufacture endogenously 01:27:40.920 |
And of course, as always, I'm not a physician. 01:27:44.420 |
I'm a scientist, so I don't prescribe anything. 01:27:48.640 |
You have to decide if melatonin is right for you, 01:27:53.100 |
And certainly, I say that both to protect myself 01:28:05.240 |
but it does seem that it can have some effects 01:28:13.120 |
It's been prescribed for or recommended for many, 01:28:21.840 |
again, double-blinded studies lasting one to six months, 01:28:24.920 |
any one that has an impressive number of subjects, 01:28:27.940 |
978 subjects ranging from age 18 all the way up to 65, 01:28:32.940 |
so on and so forth, that show not huge effects of ginkgo, 01:28:38.080 |
but as they quote, "Limited evidence suggests 01:28:43.220 |
that if tinnitus is a side effect of something else, 01:28:49.440 |
so age-related tinnitus might be helped by ginkgo boa boa. 01:28:54.440 |
I won't go through all the details of the zinc studies, 01:28:58.960 |
but it seems that zinc supplementation at higher levels 01:29:05.280 |
do appear to be able to reduce subjective symptoms 01:29:13.860 |
so I could only find one double-blinded study. 01:29:16.800 |
It lasted anywhere from one to six months, 41 subjects, 01:29:19.560 |
both genders listed out again here, 45 to 64, 01:29:23.400 |
and they saw a decrease in the severity of tinnitus symptoms 01:29:27.280 |
with 50 milligrams of elemental zinc supplementation. 01:29:31.280 |
And then last but not least is the magnesium study. 01:29:42.920 |
taking 532 milligrams of elemental magnesium. 01:29:52.280 |
but it was associated with a lessening of symptoms 01:30:13.120 |
I do realize that tinnitus is extremely disruptive. 01:30:18.880 |
because I don't from a place of experience, that is, 01:30:22.660 |
but for those of you that don't, including myself, 01:30:28.320 |
and the ringing in one's ears can be very disruptive 01:30:33.000 |
and explains why people with tinnitus reach out so often 01:30:38.360 |
and I hope this information was useful to you. 01:30:40.840 |
I'd like to now talk about balance and our sense of balance, 01:30:50.960 |
as well as by our brain and elements of our spinal cord. 01:30:55.540 |
But before I do that, I want to ask you another question, 01:31:07.240 |
It turns out that the ears grow our entire life. 01:31:10.520 |
In the early stage of our life, they grow more slowly, 01:31:19.120 |
You may have noticed, if you have family members 01:31:30.760 |
is that the ears of some of these individuals 01:31:33.460 |
get very, very big relative to their previous ear sizes. 01:31:41.440 |
can actually be measured according to ear size. 01:31:49.140 |
there is a formula in the scientific literature. 01:31:56.560 |
so the distance around your ear, ears, plural, 01:32:01.320 |
presumably you have two, most people do, in millimeters, 01:32:07.460 |
of your ears in millimeters, how would you do this? 01:32:11.040 |
Maybe you take a string and you put it around your ear, 01:32:15.420 |
That's probably going to be easier than marching around 01:32:23.520 |
On the outside, don't go in on the divot or anything. 01:32:26.380 |
You're just going around as if you were going to trace 01:32:28.020 |
the closest fitting oval, assuming your ears are oval, 01:32:32.320 |
closest fitting oval that matches your ear circumference. 01:32:36.000 |
Take that number in millimeters, subtract from it. 01:32:53.520 |
Then take that number in millimeters, subtract 88.1, 01:32:58.180 |
and then whatever value that is, multiply it times 1.96, 01:33:07.360 |
Now, why in the world would this be accurate? 01:33:10.700 |
Well, as we age, there are changes in a number 01:33:15.760 |
One of those pathways is the pathways related 01:33:27.000 |
My nose seems to be growing a lot, but then again, 01:33:29.380 |
I did sports where I would get my nose broken, 01:33:32.460 |
So I always point out, you don't get a nose like mine 01:33:34.180 |
doing yoga, but nonetheless, my nose is still growing 01:33:39.500 |
and I suspect as I get older, if I have the good fortune 01:33:47.300 |
A comparison between chronological age and biological age 01:33:50.180 |
is something that's of really deep interest these days, 01:33:52.340 |
and the work of David Sinclair at Harvard Medical School 01:34:01.980 |
how the epigenome and the genome can give us some insight 01:34:05.580 |
into our biological age and how that compares 01:34:13.840 |
and we know where we are relative to that now. 01:34:16.920 |
But you can start to make a little chart, if you like, 01:34:23.860 |
Your rates of ear growth actually correlate pretty well 01:34:32.440 |
So it's not something that we think about too often, 01:34:48.020 |
is in these two little goodies on the sides of our heads, 01:34:51.740 |
So even though it's a little bit of a bizarre metric, 01:34:54.860 |
it makes perfect sense in the biological context. 01:35:04.960 |
in the episode about hearing is that all the goodies 01:35:08.880 |
that are going to allow you to do that are in your ears. 01:35:17.680 |
So as you recall from the beginning of this episode, 01:35:19.940 |
you have two cochleas that are one on each side of your head 01:35:24.940 |
and that's a little spiral snail-shaped thing 01:35:27.780 |
that converts sound waves into electrical signals 01:35:34.680 |
you have what are called semicircular canals. 01:35:37.420 |
The semicircular canals can be best visualized 01:35:41.420 |
as thinking about three hula hoops with marbles in them. 01:35:48.320 |
and it's not filled with marbles all the way around, 01:35:51.380 |
it's just got some marbles down there at the base, okay? 01:35:54.740 |
So if you were to move that hula hoop around, 01:36:01.960 |
and each one of those hula hoops has these marbles 01:36:05.820 |
One of those hula hoops is positioned vertically 01:36:15.520 |
It's like this if you're watching on a video, 01:36:29.740 |
if you're standing upright now, if you're seated, okay? 01:36:42.860 |
Well, those marbles within each one of those hula hoops 01:36:45.260 |
can move around, but they'll only move around 01:36:56.300 |
Your head can move up and down like I'm nodding right now. 01:37:01.620 |
It's pitching forward or pitching back, okay? 01:37:12.920 |
You pilots will be very familiar with this, yaw. 01:37:18.260 |
And then there's roll, tilting the head from side to side 01:37:37.640 |
So pitch, yaw, and roll are the movements of the head 01:37:41.920 |
in each of the three major planes of motion, as we say. 01:37:45.300 |
And each one of those causes those marbles to move 01:37:49.700 |
in one or two of the various hula hoops, okay? 01:37:54.460 |
So if I move my head up and down, when I nod, 01:37:57.220 |
one of those hula hoops, literally right now, 01:38:03.180 |
These are little, kind of like little stones, basically, 01:38:27.020 |
but they're basically rolling past these little hair cells 01:38:42.480 |
there's a physical movement of these little stones 01:38:52.720 |
those hair cells, send information off to the brain. 01:39:12.260 |
Any animal that has a jaw has this so-called balance system, 01:39:22.460 |
is that it works together with the visual system. 01:39:30.000 |
and I swing my head over to the left to see what it is. 01:39:39.940 |
First of all, when I quickly move my head to the side, 01:39:43.900 |
those little stones, as I'm referring to them, 01:39:56.880 |
that my head just moved to the side like this. 01:40:00.640 |
or not that it tilted, but it just moved to the side. 01:40:03.940 |
But also visual information slid past my field of view. 01:40:30.820 |
and certainly don't do this if you're driving. 01:40:35.460 |
and you're going to move your head to the left very slowly, 01:40:42.060 |
So you're going to move it very, very slowly. 01:41:05.740 |
is that it's very uncomfortable to do it slowly, 01:41:14.940 |
The reason is when you move your head very slowly, 01:41:18.780 |
those little stones at the base of that hula hoop, 01:41:24.000 |
So you're actually not generating this signal to the brain 01:41:29.200 |
And what you'll notice is that your eyes have to go boom, 01:41:35.300 |
Whereas if you move your head really quickly, 01:41:41.860 |
So moving your body slowly is actually very disruptive 01:41:48.940 |
And it's very disruptive to your visual system. 01:41:52.280 |
Now, if you've ever had the misfortune of being on a boat 01:41:54.580 |
and you're going through big oscillations on the boat, 01:41:59.660 |
this can actually make certain people seasick 01:42:12.980 |
but there are ways that you can deal with this. 01:42:17.460 |
because what it means is it's a purely physical system 01:42:19.460 |
of these little stones rolling around in there 01:42:24.680 |
Okay, so you can do this also just by looking up. 01:42:32.300 |
You're doing this eyes open and you look down. 01:42:49.080 |
although it works best if you're moving your head 01:43:00.920 |
just to illustrate to you that normally these mechanisms 01:43:05.000 |
in your inner ear tell your eyes where to go, 01:43:09.000 |
but as well, your eyes tell your balance system, 01:43:15.420 |
So I'd like you to do a different experiment. 01:43:18.000 |
I'm not going to do it right now, but basically stand up. 01:43:25.080 |
and you're going to look forward about 10, 12 feet. 01:43:28.000 |
Pick a point on a wall or you can, anywhere that you like. 01:43:33.680 |
Just tell them you're listening to "Cuberman Lab Podcast" 01:43:38.900 |
Anyway, if you don't want to do it, don't do it, 01:43:46.640 |
and just look off into the distance about 10, 12 feet. 01:43:50.340 |
If you can do that, if you can stand on one leg, 01:44:02.480 |
It is very hard to balance with your eyes closed. 01:44:06.480 |
You might think, well, and if you think about that, 01:44:24.080 |
tell your balance system, your vestibular system, 01:44:34.800 |
by learning to balance with your eyes closed. 01:44:43.000 |
maybe off to just the distance that your thumb would be 01:44:45.240 |
if you were to reach your arm out in front of you, 01:44:46.700 |
although you don't necessarily have to put your thumb 01:44:49.400 |
So maybe just about two feet in front of you. 01:44:52.680 |
you're going to step your vision out a further distance, 01:45:00.560 |
And then you're going to march it back to you. 01:45:14.140 |
the point at which you direct your visual focus, 01:45:16.880 |
sends robust information about the relationship 01:45:20.480 |
between your visual world and your balance system. 01:45:25.460 |
not just these hula hoops, these semicircular canals, 01:45:31.640 |
it actually means mini brain in the back of your brain, 01:45:50.100 |
the activation of particular postural muscles, 01:45:57.220 |
it's about being able to adjust those postural muscles, 01:46:05.620 |
as you experience changes in your visual world. 01:46:08.900 |
And one of the most robust ways that you can engage changes 01:46:12.180 |
in your visual world is through your own movement. 01:46:15.180 |
And so most people are not trying to balance in place. 01:46:22.260 |
Most of what we think about when we think about balance 01:46:28.100 |
and when we're lunging in one particular direction 01:46:36.700 |
or any number of different things, gymnastics. 01:46:43.340 |
but you can't do it just with the visual system. 01:46:46.460 |
So what I'm recommending is if you're interested 01:46:54.440 |
Understand that they are both driving eye movements 01:47:03.060 |
And then even just two or three minutes a day 01:47:06.500 |
or every once in a while, even three times a week, 01:47:10.020 |
maybe five minutes, maybe 10 minutes, you pick. 01:47:14.820 |
you have to combine changes in your visual environment 01:47:37.040 |
in order to give an enhanced sense of balance. 01:47:39.020 |
And there's a really nice paper that was published in 2015 01:47:42.620 |
called "Effects of Balance Training on Balance Performance." 01:47:47.940 |
It's a systematic review and a meta-analysis. 01:47:50.980 |
A meta-analysis is when you combine a lot of literature 01:48:02.080 |
There are some papers out there, for instance, 01:48:06.760 |
and the role of vision in the elite athletes. 01:48:09.360 |
This is essentially the paper that I've extracted 01:48:11.540 |
most of the information that I just gave you from. 01:48:14.540 |
And that paper, and there are some others as well, 01:48:20.820 |
The core components are move your vision around 01:48:23.900 |
while staying static still, but in a balanced position, 01:48:30.620 |
if you're somebody who can do more complicated movements. 01:48:37.160 |
So standing on one leg as opposed to both, right? 01:48:45.420 |
meaning one limb asymmetrically being activated 01:48:49.780 |
And then the other way to encourage or to cultivate 01:48:57.960 |
actually involves movement itself, acceleration. 01:49:01.420 |
So that's what we're going to talk about now. 01:49:03.420 |
So up until now, I've been talking about balance 01:49:13.160 |
most people who want to enhance their sense of balance 01:49:21.520 |
meaning moving through lots of different planes of movement, 01:49:24.180 |
maybe even sometimes while squatting down low 01:49:28.140 |
or making trajectories that are different angles. 01:49:31.360 |
For that, we need to consider that the vestibular system 01:49:42.620 |
and where the eyes are and where you're looking, 01:49:44.700 |
but it also cares about what direction you're moving 01:49:54.860 |
is to start to bring together your visual system, 01:50:05.060 |
So if I move forward in space rigidly upright, 01:50:14.600 |
One of the best ways to cultivate a better sense of balance, 01:50:19.160 |
literally within the sense organs and the neurons 01:50:23.000 |
and the biology of the brain is to get into modes 01:50:27.960 |
where we are accelerating forward, typically it's forward, 01:50:46.780 |
lean into the turn so that your head is actually tilted 01:50:55.740 |
we aren't really exercising the vestibular system 01:51:00.420 |
This is why, you know, you see people in the gym on these, 01:51:04.660 |
Bonshi balls, the one that the guys roll in the park, right? 01:51:07.460 |
Bonshi balls where they're balancing back and forth, 01:51:09.480 |
that will work the small stabilizing muscles. 01:51:11.900 |
But what I'm talking about is getting into modes 01:51:13.820 |
where you actually tilt the body and the head 01:51:19.420 |
What I mean is with respect to earth's gravitational pull. 01:51:24.060 |
Now, the cerebellum is a very interesting structure 01:51:35.940 |
It's a fascinating structure deserving of an entire episode 01:51:44.960 |
meaning the neurons in the cell cerebellum get inputs, 01:51:50.480 |
The outputs of the cerebellum are strongly linked 01:51:53.400 |
to areas of the brain that release neuromodulators 01:52:01.320 |
And this is an early emerging subfield within neuroscience, 01:52:05.020 |
but a lot of what are called the non-motor outputs 01:52:11.200 |
not just on our ability to learn how to balance better, 01:52:23.900 |
We've talked about what that means in previous episodes. 01:52:26.560 |
So at least 150 minutes a week of endurance work, 01:52:34.880 |
you want to do that in order to maintain healthy strength 01:52:39.440 |
If you're doing that, but you're only doing things 01:52:44.720 |
riding the Peloton, or even if you're outside running 01:52:49.440 |
but you're never actually getting tilted, right? 01:52:58.560 |
and getting the most out of your nervous system. 01:53:03.200 |
Activation of the cerebellum in this way of being tilted 01:53:07.840 |
or the head being tilted and the body being tilted 01:53:09.880 |
while in acceleration, typically forward acceleration, 01:53:22.840 |
it can also enhance our ability to learn information 01:53:31.360 |
And that's because the cerebellum has these outputs 01:53:34.840 |
that release these neuromodulators like serotonin 01:53:37.560 |
and dopamine, and they make us feel really good. 01:53:41.440 |
I think this is one of the reasons why growing up, 01:53:44.160 |
I had some friends, some of whom might've been 01:53:50.240 |
for essentially everything except they would wake up 01:53:56.320 |
They would drive, they would get up so early to go surf. 01:54:00.080 |
It's not just surfers, and some surfers, by the way, 01:54:06.040 |
But I knew people that couldn't be motivated to do anything, 01:54:08.940 |
but they were highly driven to get into these experiences 01:54:16.580 |
Likewise with snowboarding or skiing or cycling. 01:54:21.120 |
Those modes of exercise seem to have an outsized effect 01:54:25.800 |
both on our wellbeing and our ability to translate 01:54:30.200 |
the vestibular balance that we achieve in those endeavors 01:54:33.640 |
to our ability to balance while doing other things. 01:54:37.100 |
So, and I don't mean psychological balance necessarily, 01:54:41.540 |
So for those of you that don't think of yourselves 01:54:43.440 |
as very coordinated or with very good balance, 01:54:49.000 |
forward movement or lateral movement while getting tilted, 01:54:51.720 |
even if you have to do it slowly, could be beneficial. 01:54:54.220 |
I do believe, and the scientific literature points 01:54:58.720 |
for cultivating better sense of physical balance. 01:55:08.800 |
of acceleration while tilted or while getting the head 01:55:12.340 |
into different orientations relative to gravity. 01:55:16.200 |
Well, that's the explanation for roller coasters. 01:55:24.840 |
And one of the reasons they love roller coasters 01:55:26.720 |
is because of the way that when you get the body, 01:55:33.980 |
and you're going upside down and tilted to the side 01:55:36.480 |
as the tracks go from side to side and tilt, et cetera, 01:55:39.220 |
you're getting activation of these deeper brain nuclei 01:55:47.200 |
In fact, some people get a long arc, a long duration 01:55:50.360 |
kind of buzz from having gone through those experiences. 01:55:54.760 |
are probably getting nauseous just hearing about that. 01:55:57.960 |
So I encourage people to get into modes of acceleration 01:56:04.660 |
It's an immensely powerful way to build up your skills 01:56:10.120 |
And it's also, for most people, very, very pleasing. 01:56:14.040 |
It feels really good because of the chemical relationship 01:56:16.620 |
between forward acceleration and head tilt and body tilt. 01:56:33.900 |
if you're feeling quote unquote dizzy or lightheaded is, 01:56:39.620 |
Now, we're not going to diagnose anything here 01:56:43.420 |
This is essentially me shouting into a tunnel 01:56:48.240 |
But if ever you feel that your world is spinning 01:56:51.860 |
but that you can focus on your thumb, for instance, 01:57:05.140 |
or that you feel like you need to get down onto the ground 01:57:07.960 |
because you feel lightheaded, that's being lightheaded. 01:57:12.060 |
And oftentimes with language, we don't distinguish 01:57:24.820 |
that it could happen for those of you that suffer from it 01:57:35.820 |
It could also be that you are low in electrolytes. 01:57:40.680 |
but we will talk about it more in a future episode. 01:57:43.280 |
Many people have too little sodium in their system, salt, 01:58:02.160 |
to their water remedied the problem entirely. 01:58:04.320 |
I don't think it's going to remedy every issue 01:58:08.940 |
but just the addition of salt in this particular case 01:58:14.600 |
Many people who think that they have low blood sugar 01:58:16.440 |
actually are lightheaded because of low electrolytes. 01:58:19.640 |
And because of the way that salt carries water 01:58:22.920 |
into the system and creates changes in blood volume, et cetera 01:58:26.800 |
low sodium can often be a source of lightheadedness 01:58:30.480 |
as can low blood sugar and of course other things as well. 01:58:36.020 |
we were all taught that you need to pick a point 01:58:44.280 |
It is true that if you are down in the cabin of a boat 01:58:51.140 |
and all you can see are things up close to you 01:59:02.660 |
Focusing on things close to you can be problematic. 01:59:09.120 |
And in that case, the advice to go up on deck 01:59:12.380 |
and get fresh air and to look off into the horizon, 01:59:17.600 |
But focusing your eyes on a particular location 01:59:26.260 |
where you're trying to move your head very slowly 01:59:33.920 |
So really what you want to do is allow your visual system 01:59:40.220 |
This is why sitting in the back of an Uber or a taxi 01:59:47.720 |
Sometimes the cabs, particularly in New York City, 01:59:51.000 |
They have a lot of stuff blocking your field of view. 01:59:55.000 |
where you can see out to the front windshield, 02:00:01.800 |
and you're watching that television and the cab is moving. 02:00:06.300 |
and sometimes you're taking corners, you're braking, 02:00:09.000 |
so then your vestibular system has to adjust to that. 02:00:16.960 |
actually, I'm starting to feel a little nauseous 02:00:19.200 |
I promise I'm not going to finish this episode 02:00:23.860 |
But what can happen is that you're uncoupling 02:00:39.240 |
you also get what's called proprioceptive feedback. 02:00:41.480 |
Your body is sending signals also to the vestibular system 02:00:48.060 |
you're just getting jolted around as the person is driving. 02:00:51.120 |
And if you're looking at your phone, it's even worse. 02:00:54.660 |
between you and the two front seats, that's even worse. 02:00:58.660 |
So this is why staring out the front windshield is great, 02:01:07.560 |
of having people get sick in their cars or Ubers. 02:01:20.820 |
Once again, we've covered a tremendous amount of information. 02:01:25.940 |
how you make sense of the sounds in your environment, 02:01:35.080 |
like low-level white noise and even binaural beats, 02:01:38.100 |
which can be used to enhance certain brain states, 02:01:40.740 |
certain rhythms within the brain and even dopamine release 02:01:59.560 |
And you can use those to enhance your learning as well, 02:02:02.780 |
as well as just to enhance your sense of balance. 02:02:10.100 |
In addition, please leave us any comments or feedback 02:02:13.580 |
or suggestions for future episode content on YouTube 02:02:17.840 |
If you haven't already subscribed on Apple and Spotify, 02:02:26.620 |
At Apple, you can also leave us comments and feedback. 02:02:29.720 |
During this episode, I mentioned some supplements. 02:02:33.220 |
because Thorne has the very highest levels of stringency 02:02:35.820 |
with respect to the quality of their ingredients 02:02:38.260 |
and accuracy about the amounts of those ingredients 02:02:42.960 |
If you'd like to see the products that I take from Thorne, 02:03:00.220 |
or 20% off any of the supplements that Thorne makes. 02:03:03.560 |
For those of you that might want to support us 02:03:13.840 |
In addition, if you'd like to support the podcast, 02:03:19.380 |
That is absolutely the best way to support us. 02:03:22.760 |
I'd like to thank you for your time and attention 02:03:29.120 |
And of course, thank you for your interest in science.