back to indexDopamine Baseline, Impulsivity & Addiction | Dr. Anna Lempke & Dr.Andrew Huberman

Chapters
0:0 What is Dopamine?
1:1 Dopamine & Movement
1:48 We Are Always Releasing Dopamine
2:52 Changing our Brain's Tonic Dopamine Baseline
3:53 The Relationship of Dopamine & Temperament
5:6 Are Impulsive People More Prone to Addiction?
6:45 Impulsivity is Not Always Bad
7:38 Mental Illness Are Traits That Could be Adaptive Elsewhere
00:00:00.400 |
I and many listeners of this podcast are obsessed with dopamine and what is dopamine? How does it 00:00:10.840 |
work? We all hear that dopamine is this molecule associated with pleasure. I think the term 00:00:17.200 |
dopamine hits, like I'm getting a dopamine hit from this, from Instagram or from likes or from 00:00:22.740 |
praise or from whatever is now commonly heard. What is dopamine and what are maybe some things 00:00:32.380 |
about dopamine that most people don't know and probably that I don't know either? 00:00:36.700 |
So dopamine is a neurotransmitter and neurotransmitters are those molecules that 00:00:42.320 |
bridge the gap between two neurons. So they essentially allow one neuron, the presynaptic 00:00:48.580 |
neuron to communicate with the postsynaptic neuron. Dopamine is intimately associated with 00:00:56.260 |
the experience of reward, but also with movement, which I think is really interesting because 00:01:01.320 |
movement and reward are linked, right? If you think about, you know, early humans, you had 00:01:07.980 |
to move in order to go seek out the water or the meat or whatever it was. And even in the 00:01:15.360 |
most primitive organisms, dopamine is released when food is sensed in the environment. For 00:01:20.400 |
example, C. elegans, a very primitive worm. So dopamine is this really powerful, important 00:01:30.460 |
molecule in the brain that helps us experience pleasure. It's not the only neurotransmitter involved 00:01:38.380 |
in pleasure, but it's a really, really important one. And if you want to think about something that 00:01:44.240 |
most people don't know about dopamine, which I think is really interesting is that we are always 00:01:49.000 |
releasing dopamine at a kind of tonic baseline rate. And it's really the deviation from that baseline 00:01:56.580 |
rather than like hits of dopamine in a vacuum that make a difference. So when we experience pleasure, 00:02:02.440 |
our dopamine release goes above baseline. And likewise, dopamine can go below that tonic baseline. 00:02:09.400 |
And then we experience a kind of pain. Interesting. So is it fair to say that one's baseline levels of 00:02:17.140 |
dopamine, how frequently we are releasing dopamine in the absence of some, I don't know, drug or food or 00:02:24.420 |
experience just sitting, being, is that associated with how happy somebody is, their kind of baseline of 00:02:31.860 |
happiness or level of depression? There is evidence that shows that people who are depressed may indeed 00:02:39.600 |
have lower tonic levels of dopamine. So that's a really reasonable thought. And there's some evidence 00:02:48.000 |
to suggest that that may be true. The other thing that we know, and this is, you know, really kind of 00:02:54.680 |
what, what the book is about, is that if we expose ourselves chronically to substances or behaviors that 00:03:04.240 |
repeatedly release large amounts of dopamine in our brain's reward pathway, that we can change our tonic 00:03:12.260 |
baseline and actually lower it over time as our brain tries to compensate for all of that dopamine, 00:03:18.840 |
which is more really than we were designed to, to experience. 00:03:23.880 |
Interesting. And is it would, is it, um, the case that our baseline levels of dopamine are set by our genetics, 00:03:31.120 |
Well, I think, you know, if you think about sort of, you know, the early stages of development in infancy, 00:03:37.060 |
certainly that is true. You're kind of, you know, born with probably whatever is your baseline level, 00:03:42.900 |
but obviously your experiences can have a huge impact on where your, your dopamine level ultimately settles 00:03:51.780 |
out. So, um, if somebody's disposition is one of, um, constant excitement and anticipation or easily excited, 00:04:00.180 |
these are, I think about the kind of people where you say, "Hey, do you want to check out this new place for tacos?" 00:04:04.760 |
and they're like, "Yeah, that'd be great." And other people are, um, a little more cynical, harder to budge, 00:04:10.200 |
like my bulldog Costello, um, very, very stable, low levels of dopamine with big inflections in his case. 00:04:17.400 |
Um, is that, do you think that's a set in terms of, um, our parents and obviously nature and nurture interact, 00:04:27.480 |
but is that, is dopamine at the core of our temperament? 00:04:30.920 |
I don't really think we know the answer to that, but I will say that people are definitely born 00:04:39.480 |
with different temperaments and those temperaments do affect their ability to experience joy. Um, 00:04:48.280 |
and, and, you know, we've known that for a long time and we described that in many different ways. 00:04:52.920 |
One of the ways that we describe that in the modern era is to use psychiatric nomenclature, 00:04:57.800 |
like this person has a dysthymic temperament, or, you know, this person has chronic major depressive 00:05:03.320 |
disorder. Um, in terms of looking specifically at who's vulnerable to addiction, um, that's an 00:05:10.520 |
interesting sort of mixed bag because when you look at, uh, the research on risk factors for 00:05:16.280 |
addiction, so what kind of temperament, uh, of a person makes them more vulnerable to addiction, 00:05:21.240 |
you see, um, some interesting findings. First, you see that people who are more impulsive 00:05:26.520 |
are more vulnerable to addiction. So what is impulsivity? That means having difficulty, um, 00:05:31.720 |
putting space between the thought or desire to do something and actually doing it. And people who have 00:05:38.040 |
difficulty putting a space there who are, who have a thought to do something and just do it 00:05:42.600 |
impulsively are people who are more vulnerable to addiction. 00:05:46.520 |
Interesting. Could I, uh, in terms of impulsivity, is this something that relates literally to, 00:05:52.280 |
um, the startle reflex? Like I, for instance, as a, uh, lab director, I'm familiar with walking around 00:05:57.320 |
my lab. And, um, when I decide deciding I'm going to talk to my people, of course, when they knock on my 00:06:01.800 |
door, it's always like, wait, why am I being bothered right now? Even though I love to talk to them, 00:06:05.560 |
but I walk around my lab from time to time and some people I notice I'll say, um, 00:06:09.320 |
do you have a moment? And they'll slowly turn around and say, yeah, or no, in some cases. 00:06:15.240 |
Um, and other people will jump the moment I say their name, they actually have a, 00:06:20.200 |
uh, a kind of a heightened, um, startle reflex. Right. 00:06:23.320 |
Is that related to impulsivity or is what you're referring to, um, an attempt to, uh, 00:06:29.080 |
withhold behavior that's very deliberate under very deliberate conditions? 00:06:32.920 |
Yeah. So I don't think that that startle reflex is, is necessarily related to impulsivity. That, 00:06:38.120 |
that can be related to anxiety. So people who are high anxiety, people will tend to have more 00:06:43.240 |
of a startle reflex. Impulsivity is a little bit different. And by the way, impulsivity is not always 00:06:49.000 |
bad, right? Um, impulsivity is, is that thing where there's not a lot of self editing or worrying about 00:06:58.600 |
future consequences. You know, you have the idea to do something and you do it. And of course we can 00:07:04.760 |
imagine many scenarios where that's absolutely wonderful. Um, you know, there can be a sort of, 00:07:11.080 |
uh, let's say intimate, um, interactions between people where you wouldn't really want to be super 00:07:18.520 |
inhibited about it. Right. You would want to be disinhibited and, and impulsive. Um, there, I can also 00:07:23.880 |
like imagine like sort of, um, fight or flight scenarios, like battle scenarios, right. Where, where it would 00:07:30.440 |
really be good to be impulsive and just go, rah, you know, just where hesitation can cost you your life. 00:07:35.720 |
Yes, that's right. That's right. But you know, and I think this brings up a really, 00:07:40.120 |
something that I've come to believe after 25 years of practicing psychiatry is that what we now 00:07:48.840 |
conceptualize in our current ecosystem as mental illness are actually traits that in another ecosystem 00:07:59.160 |
might be very advantageous. They're just not advantageous right now because of the world that 00:08:05.240 |
we live in. And, and I think, you know, impulsivity is potentially one of those, right. 00:08:10.760 |
Cause we live in this world that's sort of like, you have to, um, constantly be thinking 00:08:16.760 |
sort of rationally about the consequences of X, Y, or Z. And it's such a sensory rich environment, 00:08:24.440 |
right. That we're being bombarded with all of these opportunities, these sensory opportunities, 00:08:30.280 |
and we have to constantly check ourselves. And so, so, so impulsivity is something that 00:08:34.840 |
right now, um, can be a difficult trait, but isn't in and of itself a bad thing. 00:08:41.880 |
Yeah. And it's, I, I'm beginning to realize it's a fine line between spontaneity and impulsivity.