back to indexTime Perception & Entrainment by Dopamine, Serotonin & Hormones | Huberman Lab Podcast #46
Chapters
0:0 Introducing Time Perception, Note on Fasting & Supplements
5:12 Sponsors: ROKA, Athletic Greens, InsideTracker
9:25 Entrainment, Circannual Entrainment, Melatonin
13:20 Seasonal Oscillations in Testosterone & Estrogen, Tool 1
16:6 Circadian Timing, Tools 1, 2, 3 (for Circadian Entrainment)
21:13 Tool 4: Timing Physical Activity; Tool 5: Timing Eating Window
23:0 When Circadian Entrainment is Disrupted, Time Perception Suffers
25:0 Tool 6: Ultradian (90min) Cycles & Focus
31:42 Our Sense of the Passage of Time: Present, Prospective, Retrospective
34:40 Dopamine (& Nor/epinephrine) Lead to Time Overestimation; Frame Rate
37:18 Serotonin & Time Underestimation; Decreased Frame Rate
39:10 Dopamine vs. Serotonin Across the Day; Tool 7: When to Do Rigid vs. Creative Work
42:38 Example of Tool 7
43:38 How Sleep Deprivation Degrades Performance
44:38 Trauma, “Over-clocking” & Memories; Adjusting Rates of Experience
50:4 Why Trauma Involves Dopamine & Epinephrine, Arousal
51:3 Dopamine, Spontaneous Blinking & Time Perception; Tool 8
53:38 Deliberate Cold Exposure, Dopamine, Tool 9: Adjusting Frame Rate in Discomfort
56:30 Fun “Feels Fast” BUT Is Remembered as Slow; Boring Stuff “Feels Slow,” Recall As Fast
60:54 Retrospective Time, Context Variation & Enhanced Bonding with Places & People
63:0 Dopamine Release Resets the Start of Each Time Bin on Our Experience
67:40 Habits & Time Perception; Tool 10 (Setting Functional Units of Each Day)
71:58 Synthesis & Book Suggestion (Your Brain Is a Time Machine by D. Buonomano)
72:27 Supporting the HLP: Subscribe, Instagram, Patreon, Thorne Supplements
00:00:02.280 |
where we discuss science and science-based tools 00:00:10.140 |
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology 00:00:18.040 |
Our perception of time is perhaps the most important factor 00:00:24.040 |
That is, whether or not we think we are being successful, 00:00:33.680 |
And the reason for that is that our perception of time 00:00:37.040 |
is directly linked to the neurochemical states 00:00:39.840 |
that control mood, stress, happiness, excitement, 00:00:50.640 |
as successful or unsuccessful, it frames our present, 00:00:54.640 |
whether or not we think we are on track or off track, 00:00:59.840 |
whether or not we think we have a bright future, 00:01:10.260 |
and we are going to talk about tools and protocols 00:01:13.200 |
that you can use that can enhance your ability 00:01:21.920 |
We can control the speed at which we experience life. 00:01:29.880 |
and we can do that in a very direct and dynamic way. 00:01:34.440 |
once you understand how time perception works. 00:01:37.520 |
I think you're going to come away from today's episode 00:01:41.800 |
and certainly with many tools that you can try 00:01:44.400 |
in your daily life, whether or not that's work, sport, 00:01:49.400 |
Before we begin our discussion about time perception, 00:01:52.400 |
I'd like to answer some questions that I received 00:02:00.080 |
this information should still be of use to you. 00:02:05.000 |
for a particular period of time in each 24-hour cycle 00:02:15.940 |
but regardless, that feeding window is supposed to fall 00:02:18.040 |
at more or less the same period within each 24-hour day. 00:02:20.800 |
This has a number of positive effects on gene expression 00:02:36.800 |
In any event, one of the major questions I got 00:02:39.460 |
after that episode was do supplements break a fast? 00:02:44.000 |
And during that episode, I talked about what breaks a fast 00:02:50.560 |
something you ingest, whether it be liquid or food, 00:02:56.460 |
how much it increases that resting blood glucose, 00:03:08.200 |
and whether or not supplements a particular break of fast, 00:03:10.920 |
many of the questions were about Athletic Greens. 00:03:13.560 |
Athletic Greens is a sponsor of this podcast. 00:03:16.480 |
It is also a terrific supplement that I had been taking 00:03:19.040 |
for more than a decade before this podcast launched, 00:03:28.920 |
Well, that will somewhat depend on whether or not 00:03:31.040 |
your resting blood glucose tends to run high or low, 00:03:34.240 |
but for most people, including me, because I've measured it, 00:03:37.120 |
ingesting Athletic Greens does not break a fast, 00:03:43.800 |
So without knowing your resting blood glucose levels 00:03:46.560 |
on an individual basis, there's no way I can say for sure 00:03:49.040 |
that it doesn't break a fast, but chances are it does not, 00:03:51.880 |
because it doesn't contain much carbohydrate or sugar, 00:04:13.880 |
mainly essential fatty acids, in particular EPA and DHA, 00:04:17.180 |
those don't tend to raise blood glucose very much. 00:04:23.000 |
a continuous glucose monitor, my resting blood glucose, 00:04:29.640 |
Chances are it won't do that for most people as well. 00:04:49.280 |
it's not going to raise blood glucose very much. 00:04:51.500 |
Now, of course, protein can raise blood glucose 00:04:53.480 |
and fat can too as well, although to a lesser extent. 00:04:59.000 |
but at least by the logic that I just spelled out, 00:05:02.100 |
athletic greens, fish oil, and most forms of supplements, 00:05:05.780 |
provided they don't have any sugar or protein content, 00:05:13.520 |
that this podcast is separate from my teaching 00:05:19.800 |
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science 00:05:22.400 |
and science related tools to the general public. 00:05:26.040 |
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. 00:05:35.640 |
I've spent a lifetime working on the visual system. 00:05:38.040 |
And one of the key things about our visual system 00:05:40.040 |
is that it's designed so that when you move into areas 00:05:45.640 |
you can still see things with crystal clarity. 00:05:52.640 |
and putting them back on depending on the overall 00:05:55.160 |
so-called ambient environment that you're in. 00:06:00.540 |
And their eyeglasses also have superb clarity 00:06:03.840 |
regardless of overall ambient lighting, as we say. 00:06:06.520 |
In other words, you see everything very clearly 00:06:10.460 |
They also come in a number of different styles. 00:06:13.560 |
So unlike a lot of performance glasses out there 00:06:19.040 |
you can wear them to school or work or in social engagements, 00:06:29.280 |
and enter the code Huberman to save 20% off your first order. 00:06:33.260 |
Today's episode is also brought to us by Athletic Greens. 00:06:36.180 |
Athletic Greens is an all-in-one vitamin mineral 00:06:40.120 |
I started using Athletic Greens way back in 2012, 00:06:43.140 |
and so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. 00:06:58.060 |
they should take, if I were going to recommend 00:07:00.260 |
just one supplement, it would be Athletic Greens 00:07:02.340 |
because of the enormous number of biological factors 00:07:28.480 |
which make it really easy to mix up Athletic Greens 00:07:32.220 |
and they'll give you a year supply of vitamin D3K2. 00:07:35.760 |
Vitamin D3 and K2 have been shown to be really important 00:07:43.580 |
including blood lipid profiles and a number of other things. 00:07:49.400 |
to get the Athletic Greens, the five free travel packs, 00:07:54.600 |
Today's podcast is also brought to us by Inside Tracker. 00:07:57.360 |
Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform 00:08:05.840 |
I've long been a believer in getting regular blood work done 00:08:08.300 |
for the simple reason that many of the factors 00:08:10.800 |
that impact your immediate and long-term health 00:08:12.620 |
can only be analyzed from a quality blood test. 00:08:15.280 |
And it's for that reason that I get my blood work done 00:08:26.780 |
is heading in the direction that I'd like it to go 00:08:30.900 |
The other thing about Inside Tracker is they have DNA tests 00:08:33.780 |
which can tell you about the specific makeup of your genes 00:08:36.720 |
that can impact your particular nutrition, lifestyle, 00:08:41.100 |
and supplementation regimes that can also help you 00:08:57.320 |
the dashboard that they use, walks you through your data 00:09:00.380 |
and points to specific things related to nutrition. 00:09:03.380 |
Maybe you should be eating more of certain things, 00:09:12.000 |
in order to bring the numbers into the ranges 00:09:13.960 |
that are right for your immediate and long-term health. 00:09:20.620 |
to get 25% off any of Inside Tracker's plans. 00:09:28.020 |
And the most fundamental aspect of time perception 00:09:32.700 |
Entrainment is the way in which your internal processes, 00:09:48.660 |
for our entire life are so-called circannual rhythms. 00:09:53.720 |
We have neurons, nerve cells in our eye, in our brain, 00:09:58.400 |
and in our body that are marking off the passage of time 00:10:04.380 |
literally a calendar system in your brain and body. 00:10:07.160 |
And the way this works is beautifully simple. 00:10:37.060 |
In fact, if you wake up in the middle of the night, 00:10:42.180 |
and you flip on a bright light in the bathroom, 00:10:44.580 |
your melatonin levels crash down to almost zero 00:10:49.500 |
Light is a very powerful modulator of melatonin, 00:10:54.940 |
Throughout the year, depending on where you live, 00:11:09.580 |
the amount of melatonin in your brain and body 00:11:15.240 |
and it's released for shorter amounts of time, okay? 00:11:26.600 |
in your brain and body tends to be much longer. 00:11:48.880 |
that averages the amount of light that you're seeing, 00:11:51.260 |
both from artificial sources and from sunlight, 00:11:58.640 |
that for a given, say, eight-hour day in the spring, 00:12:03.180 |
because spring in the Northern Hemisphere or elsewhere, 00:12:13.400 |
and that signal is conveyed to all the systems 00:12:18.860 |
but most people feel like they have more energy 00:12:22.860 |
Conversely, when you have an eight-hour day in the winter, 00:12:31.660 |
is getting progressively greater and greater, 00:12:36.580 |
so melatonin is increasing from day to day to day. 00:12:40.480 |
Every cell and system of your body pays attention to this, 00:12:48.580 |
and a slightly lower mood in the winter months. 00:12:51.480 |
Now, there are exceptions to this, of course, 00:13:03.340 |
is entrained, is matched to some external event. 00:13:07.180 |
In this case, the event is the rotation of the Earth 00:13:16.040 |
to things that are happening in your external environment. 00:13:23.080 |
in "Cell Report," "Cell Press Journal," excellent journal, 00:13:35.820 |
they tend to make more testosterone and estrogen 00:13:43.740 |
or have romantic interactions with their existing partners, 00:13:47.140 |
even aggression, although not violent aggression, 00:14:00.820 |
but it had never really been cleanly demonstrated, 00:14:02.940 |
and what they showed was that it's actually the skin 00:14:06.660 |
that's taking information about the amount of light 00:14:12.700 |
Light exposure to the skin turns out about two hours a day. 00:14:17.500 |
This was sunlight in this case to the upper body. 00:14:20.280 |
They were wearing clothes, but their arms were exposed. 00:14:22.980 |
Their upper back and neck and face were exposed. 00:14:29.840 |
significant increases in testosterone and estrogen. 00:14:32.700 |
Now, you could probably export a tool from that if you liked. 00:14:35.580 |
That's not really what this podcast is about, 00:14:37.260 |
but it's very clear that because the skin is acting 00:14:46.120 |
that getting light on the skin, not just to the eyes, 00:15:05.600 |
Obviously, prioritize skin health and avoiding skin cancer. 00:15:09.960 |
Sunscreen is kind of a controversial topic nowadays. 00:15:12.640 |
Maybe the topic for another podcast episode at some point. 00:15:16.340 |
But nonetheless, what the Perik et al study shows, 00:15:23.480 |
we are matched to the external light-dark cycle. 00:15:26.400 |
And as the day length changes, our hormones change. 00:15:29.600 |
And we can override that with exposure to bright lights. 00:15:35.360 |
That's not a practice I particularly myself engage in, 00:15:44.240 |
The point is that our perception of time is both conscious, 00:15:52.040 |
and there are these slower, what we call oscillatory, 00:15:56.280 |
slower oscillatory events related to day length 00:16:06.400 |
The next level of time or bin of time, as we say, 00:16:23.560 |
that we all contain and that none of us can escape from. 00:16:35.600 |
meaning they release chemicals into our brain and body 00:16:48.220 |
Not surprisingly, there are periods of every 24-hour cycle 00:16:52.240 |
when we are very active and we tend to be alert 00:16:57.040 |
Now, I've talked a lot about circadian rhythms 00:17:01.080 |
and so I don't want to repeat too much of that information 00:17:03.280 |
in detail, but I'm just going to give a summary 00:17:15.140 |
It oscillates, it goes up and down once every 24 hours 00:17:19.800 |
Every cell of our body has a 24-hour oscillation 00:17:34.520 |
Every cell in our body has this beautiful 24-hour timer 00:17:42.100 |
And the important thing to understand about a given gene 00:17:45.600 |
in this context is that that gene is inhibited, 00:17:49.220 |
meaning it's reduced by a particular protein, 00:17:52.720 |
by a little biological molecule in that cell. 00:17:55.800 |
So the gene gets expressed when there's very little 00:18:01.320 |
DNA then becomes RNA, RNA is translated into a protein, 00:18:06.320 |
and that protein goes way, way up and the gene shuts down. 00:18:12.400 |
and its levels eventually drop low, low, low, low, low 00:18:23.560 |
So it's a little built-in timer in each and every one 00:18:29.240 |
they go by names like per, for period, be, mal, clock, 00:18:36.700 |
So every cell in our body has a 24-hour cycle 00:18:44.820 |
And the processes that are happening in every cell 00:18:51.520 |
to the outside light-dark cycle because morning sunlight, 00:19:00.640 |
in the middle of the night make sure that the changes, 00:19:05.160 |
these oscillations that are occurring within the cells 00:19:11.560 |
And I want to go into all the details of how that happens, 00:19:13.640 |
but there's some very simple tools that one can use 00:19:21.580 |
And I cannot emphasize enough how important it is 00:19:41.200 |
They decrease physical and mental performance. 00:19:44.740 |
You want your cells to be linked to the circadian cycle 00:19:49.440 |
And the circadian cycle outside you mainly consists 00:19:52.180 |
of when there's sunlight and when there is not. 00:19:54.920 |
And that's why the simple protocols to fall out 00:19:58.120 |
of this whole discussion about circadian entrainment 00:20:01.680 |
View 10 to 30 minutes of bright light, ideally sunlight, 00:20:06.400 |
assuming that you're waking early in the day, 00:20:10.840 |
Do that again in the afternoon or around evening, 00:20:14.000 |
10 to 30 minutes, depending on how bright it is outside. 00:20:19.100 |
or if you want to be awake and you wake up early 00:20:29.600 |
coming in through your eyes throughout the day. 00:20:32.960 |
you want as little bright light coming in through your eyes. 00:20:36.320 |
I've said this over and over and over again on this podcast. 00:20:53.140 |
set the fundamental layer of your time perception. 00:20:58.140 |
One of the best ways to disrupt your perception of time 00:21:23.360 |
try and exercise at a fairly consistent time of day. 00:21:25.980 |
Probably better to exercise than to not exercise, 00:21:31.360 |
But light, activity, and we'll talk about the third 00:21:36.920 |
that you entrain your internal perception of time 00:21:45.520 |
and therefore the exposure to sunlight or not. 00:21:48.680 |
So in addition to the sunlight viewing in the morning 00:21:52.840 |
and throughout the day and avoiding bright light at night 00:21:56.740 |
trying to get your activity, your exercise at fairly regular 00:22:00.080 |
within plus or minus two hours from each day to the next 00:22:13.200 |
The data mainly point to the fact that you need to eat 00:22:15.680 |
within more or less the same time window each day, 00:22:22.720 |
So you don't necessarily have to eat lunch at noon 00:22:28.300 |
in order to keep your circadian entrainment aligned or sharp. 00:22:33.300 |
You could, for instance, have a small snack at noon 00:22:35.520 |
and then eat at two and then have dinner at six 00:22:38.760 |
It doesn't so much matter when the exact meals fall, 00:22:44.080 |
within a consistent period or phase of each 24 hour cycle. 00:22:54.320 |
I mean, this is after all an episode about time perception. 00:22:57.160 |
It's not an episode about circadian rhythms and entrainment. 00:23:00.340 |
Well, there's a classic study by Ashoff done in 1985 00:23:10.080 |
where they didn't have clocks and they didn't have windows 00:23:14.020 |
And they were sometimes even in constant dark 00:23:28.160 |
how long they were in these isolated environments. 00:23:38.140 |
They generally underestimated how long they had been 00:23:41.140 |
in this very odd environment with no clocks or watches 00:23:48.300 |
In addition, they found that their perception 00:23:51.740 |
of shorter time intervals was also really disrupted. 00:23:55.380 |
So if they asked them to measure off two minutes, 00:24:00.740 |
People come within five to 15 seconds at most. 00:24:05.060 |
If you kind of had to sit there and just wait, 00:24:07.460 |
you have a pretty good idea of when two minutes is up, 00:24:13.100 |
or circadian entrainment, I should say, was disrupted, 00:24:16.060 |
their perception of time measurement on shorter timescales 00:24:20.780 |
of minutes or even seconds was greatly disrupted. 00:24:30.260 |
how you contend with challenges of different kinds. 00:24:33.060 |
You want your circadian entrainment to be pretty locked in 00:24:37.900 |
or pretty entrained to the outside light dark cycle 00:24:40.860 |
so that your perception of time on shorter time intervals 00:24:45.060 |
can be precise because the ability to perceive time 00:24:53.340 |
of the most fundamental ways that predicts how well 00:25:04.220 |
and organs of our body to the 365 day journey 00:25:08.340 |
that the earth takes around the sun each year. 00:25:13.820 |
the way that the 24 hour genetic and protein clocks 00:25:18.200 |
of each and every one of our cells is matched 00:25:23.700 |
and the exposure or lack of exposure to the sun 00:25:34.780 |
Ultradian rhythms are rhythms of about 90 minutes or so. 00:25:45.420 |
whether or not you sleep six hours or four hours 00:25:54.260 |
Early in the night, you tend to have more slow wave sleep. 00:25:56.260 |
Later in the night, you tend to have more REM sleep. 00:26:23.780 |
in which the brain can enter a state of focus 00:26:27.100 |
and alertness and do hard work and focus, focus, focus. 00:26:42.380 |
to the business literature to the pop psychology literature 00:26:48.020 |
by saying if you're going to do something hard 00:27:00.700 |
One should never expect that you're going to drop 00:27:02.540 |
immediately into a state of high focus at the beginning 00:27:07.620 |
We all struggle to varying degrees to achieve focus 00:27:10.860 |
and motivation and drive within those 90 minute cycles. 00:27:13.580 |
But it is true, meaning there is ample literature 00:27:17.500 |
to support the idea that after about 90 minutes, 00:27:21.720 |
we tend to go into a state of less ability to focus. 00:27:36.020 |
and then not so well at 100 minutes or 120 minutes, 00:27:49.140 |
for particular periods of time, 90 minutes or so. 00:27:55.500 |
the amount of those chemicals that can be released 00:27:58.600 |
which is why your ability to focus becomes diminished. 00:28:02.760 |
If one would like to explore more about the kind of backbone 00:28:09.880 |
This was originally called the basic rest activity cycle. 00:28:13.960 |
This was proposed many years ago by Nathaniel Kleitman. 00:28:17.620 |
It was established to be true within sleep states, 00:28:25.440 |
also control our ability to focus and perform work 00:28:29.000 |
in wakeful states, and it turns out that they do. 00:28:31.000 |
Now there's a lot of literature to support that. 00:28:34.840 |
how do you know when the 90-minute cycle begins? 00:28:37.160 |
In other words, let's say you wake up at 8 a.m. 00:28:40.200 |
and you just finished a 90-minute sleep cycle. 00:28:42.340 |
Does that mean that your next 90-minute cycle 00:28:48.740 |
The interesting thing about these basic rest activity cycles, 00:28:53.040 |
is that you can initiate them whenever you want. 00:28:57.820 |
which is a hardwired, unerring signal of 24 hours. 00:29:02.820 |
The ultradian rhythms that occur during sleep 00:29:08.820 |
You don't get the option of making your sleep cycles 00:29:24.240 |
Now the work begins, and this 90-minute cycle 00:29:32.000 |
I do a 90-minute cycle where I limit all distraction 00:29:40.540 |
on kind of an optimal workday, at least for me, 00:29:44.520 |
just to give an example of how this might work. 00:29:48.860 |
that these ultradian rhythms are ones that you set. 00:29:52.040 |
So you decide I'm going to work for 90 minutes. 00:30:00.720 |
you're going to see a diminishment in performance. 00:30:14.080 |
the things that give us narrow focus, motivation, and drive, 00:30:23.040 |
those circuits are far less willing to engage, 00:30:30.640 |
Some people like to do multiple 90-minute cycles 00:30:37.220 |
then go right into another 90-minute cycle then. 00:30:42.360 |
to acetylcholine and dopamine and norepinephrine, 00:31:01.960 |
and I can probably do another one in the afternoon. 00:31:04.240 |
This is not the kind of work that's like checking email 00:31:09.800 |
and it's working on hard problems of various kinds, 00:31:24.240 |
that could do three or four, but that's exceedingly rare. 00:31:31.480 |
and afterwards you're going to feel pretty taxed. 00:31:39.020 |
but we haven't really talked about time perception per se. 00:31:44.900 |
slow oscillatory ways in which we are entrained 00:31:50.800 |
and these ultradian cycles that we can impose on our work 00:31:55.360 |
and that we can leverage toward more focus if we like. 00:31:59.080 |
But what about the actual perception of time? 00:32:02.020 |
What actually controls how fast or how slowly 00:32:08.260 |
There are basically three forms of time perception 00:32:12.800 |
One is our perception of the passage of time in the present, 00:32:15.720 |
how quickly or slowly things seem to be happening for us. 00:32:19.480 |
This is kind of like an interval timer, ticking off time, 00:32:24.360 |
It's either fine slicing like that or tick, tick, tick. 00:32:31.440 |
I'll discuss the basis of those interval timers. 00:32:33.720 |
We also engage in what's called prospective timing, 00:32:40.220 |
That might sound a little bit like what I just described, 00:32:44.240 |
For instance, if I told you to start measuring off 00:32:58.360 |
and you couldn't use any clocks or watches or your phone 00:33:00.760 |
or anything like that, you would have to set the tick marks. 00:33:14.060 |
which is how you measure off time in the past. 00:33:17.680 |
So if I say, "Last week, I know you went to the park. 00:33:29.300 |
You probably then go, "Okay, well, I remember. 00:33:30.880 |
I went to dinner at seven and we had lunch right around two." 00:33:38.560 |
and get a sense of their relative positioning within time. 00:33:45.280 |
and then a prospective time measurement into the future. 00:33:48.280 |
The beauty of time perception in the human nervous system 00:33:54.660 |
is that it boils down to a couple of simple molecules 00:33:59.140 |
that govern whether or not we are fine slicing time 00:34:03.160 |
or whether or not we are batching time in larger bins. 00:34:06.360 |
Those molecules go by names that maybe you've heard, 00:34:15.240 |
they change the way that other neural circuits work. 00:34:21.200 |
Serotonin is released from a different site in the brain 00:34:25.880 |
and has a different effect on time perception. 00:34:36.320 |
I want to focus on a little bit of literature 00:34:38.940 |
that now has been done, fortunately, in animals and humans, 00:34:41.860 |
and which essentially shows that the more dopamine 00:34:49.420 |
the more we tend to overestimate the amount of time 00:34:55.040 |
The more dopamine that is released into our brain, 00:34:58.440 |
the more we tend to overestimate how much time has passed. 00:35:02.020 |
These experiments are very straightforward, excuse me, 00:35:04.960 |
and they're very objective, which is really nice, 00:35:07.000 |
which is you can give people or an animal a drug 00:35:15.240 |
without any measurement device, like a watch or a clock, 00:35:23.520 |
people tend to think that the minute is up before a minute. 00:35:32.160 |
So they've overestimated how much time has passed, okay? 00:35:45.400 |
and its role is very similar to that of dopamine. 00:35:47.960 |
And that's because norepinephrine and dopamine 00:35:51.000 |
As some of you may recall that they are actually manufactured 00:36:00.360 |
Biochemically, there's a cascade in which dopamine 00:36:03.960 |
can be made into norepinephrine and epinephrine, 00:36:16.800 |
that it causes fine slicing of your time bins. 00:36:21.360 |
So fine slicing of time bins is like increasing 00:36:27.220 |
Slow motion is achieved in movies and elsewhere 00:36:34.400 |
So if you take a movie at 30 frames per second and watch it, 00:36:39.200 |
it will appear to have a certain speed, right? 00:36:41.160 |
'Cause those are just snapshots, 30 frames per second. 00:36:47.220 |
at 4,000 frames per second, you are fine slicing 00:36:53.780 |
it's going to look like it moved slower, okay? 00:36:56.980 |
Whatever, so the kind of jump shot in basketball 00:36:59.540 |
that's done slowly, any slow motion is the consequence 00:37:04.780 |
So dopamine and norepinephrine increase frame rate. 00:37:12.420 |
to overestimate the amount of time that's passed. 00:37:15.520 |
Conversely, the neuromodulator serotonin causes people 00:37:21.100 |
to underestimate the amount of time that's passed. 00:37:26.220 |
They actually have done these experiments using in humans 00:37:32.620 |
which increases serotonin among other things, 00:37:34.820 |
including the cannabinoid receptor activation. 00:37:44.980 |
they tend to underestimate how much time has passed. 00:37:51.060 |
or tell you when five minutes, for instance, has passed, 00:37:54.380 |
just to use five minutes as an example this time, 00:37:56.500 |
and generally they will miss the five-minute mark. 00:37:59.460 |
They will think, they'll let six minutes pass 00:38:03.380 |
when they've underestimated how much time has passed. 00:38:06.140 |
And that's because serotonin and some of the related 00:38:09.740 |
molecules in the brain tend to lead to slower frame rates. 00:38:14.740 |
They take the frame rate from, in the example I used before, 00:38:17.880 |
from 4,000 frames per second down to say 20 frames per second 00:38:24.320 |
It's interesting in terms of how pharmacology can be used 00:38:28.400 |
to adjust time perception, but it's also interesting 00:38:34.260 |
There's some emerging evidence that throughout 00:38:39.900 |
in the amount of dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin 00:38:42.380 |
that are present in the brain and bloodstream and body, 00:38:45.680 |
depending on time of day within the circadian cycle. 00:38:53.700 |
During sleep, there are definitely variations 00:38:55.620 |
in things like dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin. 00:38:57.700 |
I talked about that in the episodes on sleep. 00:39:00.060 |
Here I'm just talking about the role of these molecules 00:39:12.940 |
dopamine and norepinephrine are elevated in the brain, 00:39:15.580 |
body, and bloodstream much more than is serotonin. 00:39:21.340 |
and in particular towards evening and nighttime, 00:39:27.800 |
What that means based on what we just discussed 00:39:30.440 |
about the role of dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin 00:39:36.220 |
is that our perception of the passage of time 00:39:38.940 |
will be very different in the early part of the day 00:39:43.360 |
And there's starting to be some evidence to support this, 00:39:47.980 |
people tend to overestimate how much time has passed. 00:39:51.920 |
they tend to underestimate how much time has passed. 00:39:54.020 |
And this is independent of taking any kind of substance 00:39:56.200 |
that would increase or decrease dopamine or serotonin. 00:39:59.200 |
Now, this is important in terms of how one thinks 00:40:03.160 |
because I know many people are thinking about 00:40:08.760 |
Many, or I should say all of the literature at least 00:40:12.220 |
that I can find on productivity and things of that sort 00:40:15.400 |
point to the idea that we should be doing the hardest task, 00:40:23.440 |
as a kind of a psychological tool for getting it done 00:40:28.680 |
And I think that's an excellent protocol, frankly. 00:40:33.520 |
because of the way that we sense accomplishment, 00:40:37.340 |
or at least it's not only an excellent protocol 00:40:39.760 |
because of the way that we sense accomplishment. 00:40:42.040 |
Another reason to move something that's very hard 00:40:46.880 |
is that if indeed the dopaminergic and noradrenergic circuits 00:40:54.580 |
we are actually in a better position cognitively 00:40:59.840 |
because of the way that we are able to fine slice 00:41:04.720 |
and fine slice all the perceptual events outside us. 00:41:07.840 |
So what I'm really saying is that early in the day 00:41:09.960 |
you are a much more high resolution camera, so to speak, 00:41:15.660 |
Now, different types of tasks and different types of things 00:41:25.960 |
whereby for activities that involve more kind of 00:41:35.480 |
and in which we need to kind of blend different aspects 00:41:39.120 |
of our memory, different aspects of task utilization, 00:41:43.720 |
in other words, for creative works, for brainstorming, 00:41:47.000 |
for things that are a bit more fluid, so to speak, 00:41:49.900 |
the more serotonergic second half of the day, 00:41:55.160 |
second half of the day lends itself to our time perception, 00:41:58.440 |
may actually be more beneficial for those sorts of tasks. 00:42:01.520 |
And I'll put a reference to a couple of the studies 00:42:08.340 |
we are better at doing certain sorts of tasks, 00:42:15.840 |
and how the dopamine tends to be earlier in the day 00:42:18.920 |
and the serotonin later in the day, so to speak, 00:42:21.200 |
these are broad, I'm painting with broad strokes here, 00:42:29.020 |
of doing something that's hard or challenging 00:42:30.880 |
early in the day, but we should ask ourselves, 00:42:41.360 |
Basically what I'm saying is if you are doing work 00:42:47.000 |
so math or a recipe or execution of musical scales 00:42:55.420 |
or something that requires a lot of precision 00:42:57.560 |
where there's a right and wrong answer and it's hard, 00:43:03.300 |
because of the way that dopamine and norepinephrine 00:43:11.680 |
You are a higher resolution brain during those times, 00:43:17.000 |
to events and demands that require high resolution, 00:43:22.600 |
in this more of what I'm calling serotonergic state, 00:43:25.200 |
that's when you're going to be better at brainstorming 00:43:27.360 |
and creative works where there's some flexibility 00:43:30.120 |
in terms of how you're batching time and perceiving time, 00:43:40.280 |
but also to take us back to this critical role 00:43:43.400 |
of the circadian rhythm, there is a lot of evidence 00:43:49.240 |
when sleep is either too short or is fragmented 00:43:52.020 |
or is not of high enough quality for enough days, 00:44:13.720 |
why when we haven't slept well or we haven't slept enough, 00:44:17.080 |
we tend to feel a little off, like we can't concentrate. 00:44:19.680 |
Part of that lack of concentration is due to other things. 00:44:22.340 |
But part of that concentration could be due to the fact 00:44:24.260 |
that our sense of the passage of time is disrupted. 00:44:28.560 |
in keeping the dopaminergic, neurodegenergic state 00:44:34.200 |
and the serotonergic state, as we're calling it, 00:44:36.360 |
kind of pushed towards the second half of the day. 00:44:38.840 |
Now, there is a version of how dopamine and norepinephrine 00:44:41.820 |
can impact our perception of the passage of time 00:44:44.440 |
in ways that can be very disruptive or even maladaptive. 00:44:48.960 |
And the best example that I'm aware of is trauma. 00:44:55.640 |
or who have experienced some other form of major trauma 00:45:02.640 |
Overclocking is when levels of dopamine and norepinephrine 00:45:18.080 |
In other words, the frame rate is increased so much so 00:45:21.840 |
that we perceive things as happening in ultra slow motion. 00:45:25.960 |
Now, that might not seem like a bad thing overall, 00:45:37.640 |
So the memory system, which involves areas of the brain 00:45:40.740 |
like the hippocampus, but also the neocortex, 00:45:54.460 |
about the outside world except light coming in 00:45:56.320 |
through the eyes and whatever happens to hit our ears 00:45:58.620 |
in terms of sound waves and skin and so forth. 00:46:04.080 |
and it has to create a record of what happened. 00:46:07.400 |
Now, it doesn't create a record of everything that happened, 00:46:09.640 |
but car accidents and trauma and things of that sort 00:46:19.720 |
what we actually mean by the phrase stamped down, 00:46:22.240 |
is that the precise firing of the sequence of neurons 00:46:29.880 |
certain neurons are firing because of the flipping 00:46:32.240 |
of the car or there's screams or there's blood 00:46:42.240 |
in the hippocampus and then the sequence of the firing 00:46:47.860 |
So it's not just that neuron one, two, three, four fired 00:46:51.160 |
in that sequence, it's also that neuron one, two, three, four 00:46:55.420 |
So it would be one, two, three, four during the actual event 00:46:57.920 |
and then the memory is stored as firing of those neurons 00:47:02.360 |
If during the event it was one, two, three, four 00:47:07.820 |
is not going to be one, two, three, four, okay? 00:47:12.200 |
In other words, there's both a space code as we say, 00:47:15.680 |
meaning the particular neurons that fire is important 00:47:18.640 |
and there's a rate code, how quickly those neurons fire 00:47:22.200 |
or the relative firing, the timing of the firing 00:47:26.880 |
This affords our memory system tremendous flexibility. 00:47:29.280 |
What it means is that you can take the same set of neurons 00:47:31.760 |
in the hippocampus and stamp down many, many more memories 00:47:39.540 |
of the different rates of the different neurons 00:47:41.800 |
that were firing in order to set that code, right? 00:47:44.920 |
Otherwise, if you needed a different set of neurons 00:47:46.860 |
for every memory, you need an enormous hippocampus, 00:47:52.440 |
Overclocking is a case in which the frame rate is so high 00:47:59.140 |
and people have a very hard time shaking that memory 00:48:01.560 |
and the emotions associated with that memory. 00:48:03.840 |
And it's not the topic of today's conversation 00:48:06.040 |
but we will cover trauma in a future episode in detail 00:48:14.840 |
about ketamine therapies, exposure therapies, 00:48:34.780 |
of the rate of the experience in their memory, 00:48:38.200 |
not just whether or not the memory happened at all. 00:48:41.040 |
In fact, one of the first things that trauma victims learn 00:48:44.640 |
is that they aren't going to forget what happened. 00:48:50.600 |
is that the emotional weight of the experience 00:49:10.020 |
In other words, taking that firing of neurons 00:49:17.720 |
and getting the memory to play back at a rate of one, 00:49:21.520 |
two, three, four, or even one, two, three, four, 00:49:37.560 |
by which people can uncouple some of the emotional weight 00:49:48.400 |
of where the dopaminergic and the noradrenergic system 00:49:54.280 |
unfortunately, what seems like indelible mark 00:50:00.000 |
to uncouple the emotional load of that event. 00:50:02.560 |
Some of you are probably saying, why dopamine during trauma? 00:50:05.880 |
I thought dopamine was the feel-good molecule. 00:50:10.480 |
dopamine is not necessarily a molecule of reward, 00:50:13.120 |
it's a molecule of motivation, pursuit, and drive. 00:50:21.560 |
So whether or not dopamine is released during car crashes 00:50:27.140 |
But what we do know is that both the dopamine system 00:50:29.620 |
and noradrenergic system, when we say noradrenergic, 00:50:35.200 |
anytime there's a heightened state of arousal. 00:50:39.880 |
like meaning associated with an event that we really hate, 00:50:53.440 |
And so that's why we see evidence for dopamine 00:50:56.400 |
being associated with these changes in time perception, 00:50:59.200 |
both for positive events and for negative events. 00:51:04.880 |
between arousal, dopamine, time perception, and blinking. 00:51:09.880 |
And this is all supported by a really interesting paper. 00:51:15.520 |
T-E-R-H-U-N-E, is published in Current Biology, 00:51:25.360 |
are associated with heightened levels of dopamine. 00:51:32.440 |
And one of the ways that we fine slice time is by blinking. 00:51:39.040 |
or to limit the amount of light coming into our eyes, 00:51:49.500 |
that cognitive attention follows visual attention, 00:51:53.700 |
Well, it turns out that dopamine and increases in dopamine 00:51:57.920 |
are associated with increases in spontaneous blink rate. 00:52:01.320 |
So the more aroused we are, the more awake we are. 00:52:05.840 |
pupils dilate, heart rate increases, et cetera, 00:52:09.980 |
And every time we blink, this study cleanly shows, 00:52:16.040 |
leading to, as I mentioned before, overestimations of time. 00:52:22.520 |
blink rate is actually related to frame rate. 00:52:27.840 |
And the way that you could think about leveraging this 00:52:33.960 |
your perception of time, you would blink less. 00:52:36.880 |
And if you want to speed up your perception of time, 00:52:46.900 |
But I think it's a very interesting parameter 00:52:49.780 |
of our visual attention as it relates to time perception, 00:52:54.700 |
is that these neuromodulators like dopamine or serotonin 00:52:58.780 |
they're not doing it through some magical mechanism. 00:53:06.140 |
I haven't said today, oh, you know, it's the striatum. 00:53:14.940 |
in timing of movement, something for a future podcast, 00:53:18.600 |
but time perception is what we call a distributed phenomenon. 00:53:22.360 |
It's a network of areas in the brain working together, 00:53:30.940 |
seems to be controlling the frame rate on your experience. 00:53:45.140 |
and lakes and oceans and things of that sort. 00:53:47.860 |
There are a lot of different positive effects 00:53:54.240 |
brown fat stores, which are the good fat stores 00:53:59.700 |
stay warm in cold environments to reduce inflammation, 00:54:12.420 |
our baseline levels of dopamine robustly, 2.5X, 00:54:16.040 |
and it's a long-lasting increase in dopamine, 00:54:20.940 |
I'm sure there are some people out there addicted to ice baths 00:54:24.940 |
of dopamine-inducing behaviors that are addictive, 00:54:27.420 |
it seems to be more on the health-promoting side. 00:54:31.840 |
because cold water exposure increases dopamine, 00:54:36.880 |
And if you've ever done one of these cold water exposures, 00:54:40.420 |
You've experienced getting in and feeling like, wow, 00:54:43.360 |
making it three minutes is a really, really long time. 00:54:49.020 |
Part of that, just at a kind of a coarse level, 00:54:52.400 |
as you're thinking, this is painful, I don't like this, 00:54:56.420 |
But part of it is also that your dopamine levels 00:55:00.380 |
and therefore your perception of that discomfort 00:55:05.180 |
And so you could leverage a tool, for instance, 00:55:10.020 |
to something other than your immediate experience, right? 00:55:12.980 |
This is a kind of a controversy, if you will, 00:55:18.420 |
The question is, do you try and lean into the experience 00:55:21.360 |
and really feel it, or do you try and distract yourself? 00:55:34.920 |
that the dopamine level that's now increased in your system 00:55:44.060 |
So a minute is going to seem like a lot longer 00:55:51.380 |
decide to pay attention to some external cue. 00:55:55.060 |
Maybe it's a metronome that ticks once every 10 seconds. 00:55:59.540 |
You could decide to think about something else. 00:56:05.780 |
All of that will divorce you from the sensation 00:56:09.520 |
but more so it will divorce you from the perception 00:56:14.740 |
as governed by that dopamine increase in frame rate. 00:56:19.780 |
When you're in the ice bath, your dopamine levels are high. 00:56:23.700 |
your experience of the discomfort of that ice bath 00:56:28.220 |
Now, up until now, I've been talking about how dopamine 00:56:33.740 |
can differentially impact your perception of how fast 00:56:37.300 |
or how slowly things are happening in the moment. 00:56:43.820 |
we have our experience of time in the moment, 00:56:48.380 |
And there are beautiful studies that have showed 00:57:10.700 |
if something that you experience is fun or varied, 00:57:15.520 |
meaning it has a lot of different components in it, 00:57:23.060 |
you will experience that as going by very fast. 00:57:30.780 |
which I just said you experience as going by very slowly, 00:57:32.980 |
but here I'm talking about something that's fun 00:57:36.660 |
and you'll feel like it goes by very, very fast. 00:57:38.780 |
Imagine an amazing day for a kid in an amusement park. 00:57:48.380 |
But later, they will remember that experience 00:57:54.580 |
that it was a long day full of many, many events. 00:58:00.340 |
between how we perceive fun, exciting, varied events 00:58:03.440 |
in the present and how we remember them in the past. 00:58:10.040 |
it'll seem like, or an amazing vacation overall, 00:58:15.540 |
whoa, it went by so fast 'cause there's so much happening. 00:58:18.680 |
But in memory, six to eight months later, you remember, 00:58:21.700 |
wow, that just went, you know, that was a long, long thing. 00:58:26.700 |
We had this, then we had that, then we did this, 00:58:48.060 |
it will seem like that moment was very short. 00:58:50.260 |
So if the other day I was waiting in the waiting room 00:58:57.740 |
And it did seem like it was going on an awfully long time. 00:59:00.040 |
But indeed, looking back, it just seems like, 00:59:14.360 |
and low dopamine being associated with kind of empty, 00:59:17.600 |
boring, or what at the time seemed like long experiences. 00:59:24.420 |
into the scientific literature by those earlier experiments 00:59:30.620 |
You know, take away all the clocks and watches and cues 00:59:39.220 |
where they can either read and work and do things 00:59:44.140 |
When people are isolated in very boring environments 00:59:47.540 |
and they don't have access to time cues, time dilates. 00:59:51.420 |
They tend to assume that time has gone on very, very long. 00:59:58.100 |
we aren't just driven by these circadian clocks 01:00:00.460 |
and these circannual clocks and these ultradian clocks. 01:00:06.880 |
that vary depending on our level of excitement. 01:00:10.660 |
And they vary on depending on our level of excitement 01:00:13.480 |
because of these neuromodulators, dopamine and serotonin. 01:00:17.620 |
is that you have two clocks, two stopwatches. 01:00:30.180 |
And then the other hand, you have a stopwatch 01:00:34.860 |
big ticks along the hand is moving at bigger intervals, 01:00:41.440 |
And depending on whether or not you're excited 01:00:48.740 |
and therefore you're perceiving your experience differently. 01:00:51.980 |
One very interesting aspect to the way that neuromodulators 01:00:55.420 |
like dopamine and novelty interact with time perception 01:00:59.740 |
and memory is how we perceive our relationship 01:01:08.720 |
that the more novel experiences we have in a place, 01:01:11.560 |
the more we feel we know that place obviously, 01:01:19.420 |
So here's the kind of gedanken or thought experiment 01:01:32.320 |
and I would have a number of different experiences. 01:01:35.680 |
let's say I had 100 different exciting and new experiences. 01:01:44.120 |
feel as if I lived there a certain period of time, one year. 01:01:46.900 |
I would actually know I lived there one year. 01:01:48.560 |
If, however, I lived in three different places 01:01:50.540 |
in New York City and I met three times as many people 01:01:53.260 |
and I had three times as many novel experiences, 01:01:56.240 |
I would actually feel as if I had been there much longer 01:02:04.580 |
When we move to multiple or several novel environments 01:02:11.160 |
we tend to feel as if we know that person much better 01:02:16.140 |
to interact with those people in different contexts 01:02:18.180 |
and so indeed we do get the opportunity to see them, 01:02:21.140 |
for instance, at the coffee shop, how they order coffee, 01:02:26.140 |
how they act there, maybe how they interact with your family. 01:02:29.100 |
You're getting a sense of them in different contexts. 01:02:34.680 |
but it seems as if the more novelty you experience 01:02:37.760 |
with somebody, not only the more familiar they are to you, 01:02:41.040 |
but the more time you feel you've spent with them, 01:02:48.620 |
of how our perception of time and these neuromodulators 01:02:53.000 |
not just that we perceive a given event in our world, 01:02:56.900 |
but how we relate to a place or relate to a person. 01:02:59.940 |
So we've talked a lot about the different neurochemicals 01:03:06.020 |
We haven't talked a lot about the neural circuits 01:03:08.120 |
and the various areas of the brain that underlie this. 01:03:16.940 |
also a cell press journal, excellent journal. 01:03:19.420 |
The title of the paper is Behavioral, Physiological, 01:03:33.820 |
These were basketball games that actually took place, 01:03:37.800 |
And the subjects watching these basketball games, 01:03:46.780 |
the subjects in these studies had some prior knowledge 01:03:51.780 |
which team was likely to win or not likely to win. 01:04:00.120 |
by the release of dopamine in two areas of the brain, 01:04:09.300 |
So the two areas of the brain that are important here 01:04:25.220 |
So if I predict that my team dribbling down court 01:04:33.260 |
These two brain areas light up in the functional imaging, 01:04:37.080 |
so-called fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging 01:04:47.820 |
anytime that something the subject wanted to see happened, 01:04:52.060 |
anytime they wanted to see their team score, they scored, 01:05:01.060 |
that their team was going to score on this particular drive 01:05:12.180 |
not just for positive events, but for unexpected events. 01:05:25.540 |
when something that one hopes will happen happens, 01:05:33.980 |
it's not something that the subject wanted to happen. 01:05:43.740 |
regardless of what caused the dopamine release, 01:05:49.340 |
predicted how the subjects parsed the time bins 01:06:06.500 |
Well, you could batch a meal by the, I don't know, 01:06:09.580 |
the appetizer, the main course, and the dessert, 01:06:12.200 |
but it turns out that's not what you're doing. 01:06:14.640 |
You're batching time according to the frequency 01:06:17.860 |
of dopamine pulses, the frequency of dopamine release. 01:06:28.260 |
not whether or not it was a particular time point 01:06:33.880 |
was going down court or running back up court 01:06:47.400 |
what I mean is how often and when you release dopamine 01:06:55.780 |
not just for positive events or negative events. 01:06:58.740 |
So what this means is as you were going through life, 01:07:01.380 |
dopamine and the release of dopamine is saying, 01:07:07.140 |
that's over and now you're in a new phase of your life, 01:07:11.580 |
So if I get up in the morning and I really need 01:07:15.980 |
I wait 90 minutes to 120 minutes before I drink my coffee, 01:07:20.940 |
and surely there's a dopamine hit there, I promise you. 01:07:34.460 |
I'm actually carving up my experience according 01:07:37.380 |
to when I'm getting dopamine throughout my day. 01:07:50.500 |
And that is a tool that many people have talked 01:07:54.660 |
People have discussed habits in a variety of contexts, 01:08:03.620 |
and time perception, what this means is that placing 01:08:07.600 |
specific habitual routines at particular intervals 01:08:12.600 |
throughout your day is a very, not just convenient, 01:08:16.900 |
but a very good way to incorporate the dopamine system 01:08:26.180 |
It would mean waking up and having one specific habit 01:08:28.520 |
that you always engage in that causes a release of dopamine. 01:08:34.120 |
You could say, well, great, that'll make me feel good. 01:08:36.920 |
And I would agree, dopamine release generally makes us 01:08:40.080 |
feel motivated, but it would have an additional effect 01:08:47.940 |
Then inserting another habit, perhaps the beginning of, 01:08:56.000 |
You don't have to be obsessively precise about the timing, 01:09:04.460 |
is going to lead not just to dopamine release 01:09:07.060 |
as it relates to reward and motivation and feeling good, 01:09:09.980 |
but it actually becomes the way in which we carve up 01:09:17.820 |
You could say, well, of course, I do one thing, 01:09:23.660 |
That's my day, it's my list, it's my to-do list, et cetera. 01:09:27.200 |
But what I'm saying is that on the basis of this study, 01:09:31.180 |
his last name is Antony, it was Antony et al., 01:09:34.020 |
it was published in 2020, the study on basketball viewing, 01:09:39.020 |
what it points to is that by engaging in specific habits 01:09:45.040 |
we are actually setting the frame rate on our day. 01:09:52.680 |
okay, my morning routine and my afternoon routine, 01:10:01.520 |
but rather thinking about what's actually going on 01:10:18.780 |
during your morning, you might insert habit one 01:10:21.100 |
and habit two at say, I don't know, 8 a.m. and 10 a.m., 01:10:27.780 |
a little batch of time in your morning routine 01:10:31.620 |
that's distinct from the second half of your morning. 01:10:34.500 |
In other words, habits serve as flankers or markers 01:10:42.720 |
or why would I want to structure my life like that, 01:10:47.540 |
to structure their life like that and to utilize habits, 01:10:50.380 |
not just for sake of what you do during the habit, 01:10:52.800 |
but because of the fact that the habits serve as a marker 01:10:56.100 |
because of the way they can evoke dopamine release. 01:10:58.940 |
And in doing that, you're able to segment your day 01:11:02.500 |
into a bunch of smaller, if you want them to be smaller, 01:11:10.860 |
the Huberman Lab podcast puts out a newsletter. 01:11:24.080 |
And there you'll find the 12 Steps to Improving Sleep 01:11:43.280 |
and the relationship between habits and dopamine, 01:11:45.960 |
dopamine and time perception to structure your day 01:11:48.640 |
according to performance of particular types of tasks. 01:11:52.060 |
Today, we covered a lot about time perception. 01:11:54.200 |
We certainly didn't cover everything about time perception, 01:11:57.920 |
the role of dopamine, habits, and various routines 01:12:05.400 |
If you're interested in learning more about time perception, 01:12:08.440 |
I'd like to point you to a really excellent book 01:12:15.280 |
The book was written by Professor Dr. Dean Buonamono, 01:12:19.480 |
and a world expert in the neuroscience and physics of time. 01:12:27.400 |
If you're learning from and/or enjoying this podcast, 01:12:29.940 |
please subscribe to our podcast channel on YouTube. 01:12:53.960 |
On Instagram, I do short neuroscience tutorials 01:12:59.400 |
many of which are not included on the podcast. 01:13:10.080 |
Not so much today, but on many previous episodes 01:13:12.320 |
of the Huberman Lab Podcast, we discuss supplements. 01:13:15.020 |
And while supplements aren't necessary for everybody, 01:13:17.920 |
many people derive great benefit from supplements 01:13:24.240 |
is that what's listed on the bottle of various supplements 01:13:29.120 |
And the quality of ingredients varies tremendously 01:13:34.000 |
For that reason, we've partnered with Thorne, 01:13:37.880 |
because Thorne supplements have the highest levels 01:13:40.060 |
of stringency of any supplement company out there 01:13:46.620 |
and so we're delighted that we partnered with them. 01:13:48.360 |
If you'd like to see the supplements that I take, 01:13:50.260 |
you can go to Thorne, that's T-H-O-R-N-E.com/u/huberman 01:13:57.140 |
and you can get 20% off any of those supplements. 01:13:59.880 |
If you enter the Thorne site through that portal, 01:14:02.220 |
you can also get 20% off any of the supplements